The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×
ArticleFull Access

Neurological Soft Signs in First-Episode Schizophrenia: A Follow-Up Study

Abstract

OBJECTIVE: Neurological soft signs are frequently found in schizophrenia. They are indicators of both genetic liability and psychopathological symptoms. To further differentiate “trait” and “state” relations the authors compared the 1-year course of neurological soft signs in schizophrenia patients and comparison subjects. METHOD: Thirty-nine patients with first-episode schizophrenia spectrum disorders were examined after remission of acute symptoms and 14 months later. Established instruments assessed diagnoses, psychopathological symptoms, predictors of outcome, handedness, and neurological soft signs. Twenty-two age- and gender-matched comparison subjects were also examined twice. RESULTS: Neurological soft sign scores in patients were significantly elevated relative to comparison subjects at both measurement points. Whereas neurological soft signs remained stable in comparison subjects (time 1: mean=4.8, SD=3.3; time 2: mean=4.6, SD=3.9), they significantly decreased in patients (time 1: mean=15.7, SD=7.1; time 2: mean=10.1, SD=7.9). This effect was more pronounced in patients with a favorable versus a chronic course and was mainly accounted for by motor signs. Predictors of follow-up neurological soft sign scores were neurological soft sign levels at remission and compliance with treatment. CONCLUSIONS: Although neurological soft signs are intrinsic to schizophrenia, their level varies with the clinical course. Thus, neurological soft signs may correspond to both genetic liability and the activity of the disease process and may be considered as potential predictors of outcome.

Neurological soft signs refer to subtle neurological abnormalities comprising deficits in sensory integration, motor coordination, and sequencing of complex motor acts (1). A considerable body of research has established that neurological soft signs are more prevalent in schizophrenia patients, including first-episode cases, than in healthy subjects (2). Studies with neuroleptic-naive first-episode patients have demonstrated that neurological soft signs are present before medication exposure, thus they are thought to be an intrinsic feature of schizophrenia (3, 4). This notion is supported by findings of neurological soft signs in high-risk subjects (i.e., relatives of schizophrenia patients and unaffected co-twins of monozygotic twin pairs discordant for schizophrenia [57]). These studies found that relatives take an intermediate position between healthy and schizophrenia subjects.

It is generally accepted that neurological soft signs are associated with psychopathology, especially negative symptoms and formal thought disorders (810). This notion is consistent with earlier studies of our group (1012) in which we demonstrated a significant decrease of neurological soft signs that paralleled remission of symptoms under neuroleptic treatment.

The dysfunctional networks involved in the pathogenesis of neurological soft signs have not been fully identified to date. Neuroimaging studies have suggested associations of neurological soft signs with activation changes in the sensorimotor cortex and the supplementary motor area, cerebellar abnormalities, and subcortical findings involving the basal ganglia and thalamus (4, 10, 13, 14). Although these results and their interrelations have not yet been investigated in one joint study, they strengthen the hypothesis that neurological soft signs may refer to changes of frontal-thalamic-cerebellar pathways as conceptualized in the model of “cognitive dysmetria” (15).

Given these results, neurological soft signs can be interpreted as an expression of genetic liability toward the disease as have been presented in Meehl’s concept of “schizotaxia” (16) in which dysdiadochokinesia—among others—constitutes a trait-like marker of a baseline defect (“hypokrisia”). To further understand fluctuations in the level of neurological soft signs and in particular their increase with acute symptoms of schizophrenia, Huber’s hypothesis of “process activity” (17) may serve as a complementary concept. Huber differentiates reversible and irreversible symptoms, the former representing functional states of the active disease process that may remit with clinical stabilization. From his pneumencephalographic study (18) he concluded that once the active process persists, structural and psychopathological deteriorations run a parallel course toward an irreversible deficit. Hence, process activity does not refer to a static concept but addresses the variability of the clinical course.

Drawing on the outlined concepts, we hypothesized that 1) neurological soft sign scores would increase during acute phases of the illness and return to baseline values with stabilization through treatment (possibly reflecting the activity of the disease process); 2) a decrease in neurological soft sign levels would be a favorable prognostic criterion; and 3) even in patients with a remitting course and favorable outcome, neurological soft signs would remain increased relative to healthy comparison subjects as an expression of genetic liability or schizotaxia.

Method

Subjects

Thirty-nine schizophrenia patients whose first psychotic episode had remitted before discharge from an acute care ward were consecutively included in the study. Individuals with a history of or a concomitant neurological or medical disorder or severe substance abuse were excluded. The group consisted of 21 women and 18 men, all Caucasian, with a mean of 11.6 years (SD=1.6) of education; mean age at study intake was 27.0 years (SD=7.7). Patients were discharged on atypical antipsychotic regimens according to their psychiatrists’ choice (mean dose=579.5 mg in chlorpromazine equivalents [SD=267.3]). One year later, patients were invited for follow-up assessment; the exact interval amounted to a mean of 14.2 months (SD=1.6, range=10–18). Twenty-two healthy Caucasian comparison subjects were recruited from the general population through advertisements after screening for and exclusion of major psychiatric disorders. The group consisted of 10 men and 12 women, mean age=28.0 years (SD=3.8), mean education level=12.6 years (SD=0.9), and mean follow-up interval=10.0 months (SD=1.4, range=8–13). All ratings were performed by the same trained raters (S.B., C.B.) on both occasions. To maintain intra- and interrater reliability and avoid rater drift, we implemented an ongoing program of regular reliability checks. The study was approved by the ethics committee of the Medical Faculty, University of Heidelberg. After full explanation of proceedings subjects provided informed consent to participate.

Assessments

Neurological soft signs were assessed with the Heidelberg Scale (10), which consists of five subscales comprising 16 items (Appendix 1). All items except for gait, tandem gait, Ozeretzki’s test, articulation, and right/left orientation are assessed separately for both the right and the left side. Ratings are given on a 0–3-point scale (no/slight/moderate/marked abnormality, respectively). In the initial evaluation of the Heidelberg Scale (10), a sufficient internal reliability (Cronbach’s alpha=0.85/0.89 for schizophrenia patients/healthy subjects) and interrater reliability (r=0.88, p<0.005) were established.

The Edinburgh Inventory (19) was applied to establish handedness. By rating common activities, a laterality quotient is calculated that ranges from –100 (strong left hander) to 100 (strong right hander). We used narrow definitions of left handedness (laterality quotient less than –80) and right handedness (laterality quotient more than 80); the remaining range represented mixed handedness.

Diagnoses were established with the German version of the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (20), which also served to exclude further major psychiatric disorders. Psychopathological symptoms were rated on the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (21), and predictors of outcome were rated on the Strauss-Carpenter Scale (22). Side effects of medication were documented with the Simpson-Angus Rating Scale (23), Barnes Rating Scale for Drug-Induced Akathisia (24), and the Abnormal Involuntary Movement Scale (AIMS) (25). At the follow-up evaluation, diagnoses, symptoms, predictors of outcome, and side effects of medication were reassessed with the indicated instruments. Moreover, information on compliance and important areas of functioning during the follow-up interval was gathered through a thorough clinical interview. Compliance was rated as present only if patients reported regular medication intake.

Data Analysis

For all variables, Kolmogorov-Smirnov analyses were calculated to test for normal distribution. Using chi-square tests, extended by Fisher’s exact tests if warranted and analyses of variance (ANOVA), possible differences between patients and healthy subjects and between the sexes with respect to clinical variables were assessed. Those variables that had been rated twice during the clinical course were analyzed with repeated measures ANOVAs. To further analyze the changes in neurological soft sign scores over time, the patient group was dichotomized by a median split on the basis of the respective change; resulting subgroups were compared with respect to the other clinical parameters. Moreover, variables that preceded neurological soft signs at the follow-up evaluation were entered in a stepwise linear regression analysis to detect possible predictors of neurological soft sign change. Extending previous studies we established the test-retest reliability of the Heidelberg Scale. This was done in the comparison subjects, since neurological soft sign stability was expected in this group. All analyses were performed with the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences.

Results

Demographic and Clinical Data

Patients and comparison subjects were comparable with respect to age, gender, and education. Although eight patients and only one healthy subject had a family history of psychiatric disease, this difference did not reach significance level.

Patients’ initial assessment revealed the following diagnoses: schizophrenia (N=20), schizoaffective disorder (N=2), schizophreniform disorder (N=16), and psychosis not otherwise specified (N=1). At the follow-up evaluation, a diagnostic shift was seen in 16 cases: from schizophreniform disorder to schizophrenia (N=14), from psychosis not otherwise specified to schizophrenia (N=1), and from schizophrenia to schizoaffective disorder (N=1). During the follow-up period, 33 individuals adhered to psychiatric treatment regularly. Thirty-one patients received continuous treatment with atypical antipsychotics (mean dose=297.7 mg/day in chlorpromazine equivalents [SD=35.5]); four of these patients were prescribed a mood stabilizer or an antidepressant in addition. Side effects of medication were low and unchanged at the follow-up evaluation relative to remission according to the AIMS (time 1: median=0, range=13; time 2: median=0, range=5), the Barnes Rating Scale for Drug-Induced Akathisia (time 1: median=0, range=4; time 2: median=0, range=3), and the Simpson-Angus Rating Scale (time 1: median=12, range=7; time 2: median=11, range=4).

At the follow-up evaluation, the overall Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale score (mean=52.4, SD=25.6) did not represent a significant difference from the remission score (mean=52.0, SD=12.4). The mean follow-up Strauss-Carpenter Scale score (58.4, SD=11.5) was comparable to the mean initial score (57.4, SD=11.5). During the follow-up period two patients had been continuously ill. Relapses occurred in 13 individuals, six of whom were fully recovered at the second interview. The remaining seven subjects still suffered from psychotic symptoms (N=4) or had been readmitted to the hospital (N=3). Five individuals used cannabis, four of these were also regular alcohol users. Thirty-three patients were compliant with treatment, and 31 were compliant with their medication. Ten individuals had continued their education successfully, and 20 were fully employed. Regular participation in household duties was reported by 29 subjects, regular social contacts by 24 subjects.

Neurological Soft Signs and Handedness

The patient group consisted of a similar number of right handers (N=20) and mixed handers (N=19). In the comparison group right handers (N=19) outnumbered mixed handers (N=3). While this difference between groups was statistically significant (F=8.5, df=1, 59, p<0.01), analyses did not yield a difference between the sexes within diagnostic groups.

Patients’ mean initial scores on the Heidelberg Scale decreased significantly during the follow-up period (Table 1), whereas the scores of the comparison subjects remained almost unchanged. Good test-retest reliability of the Heidelberg Scale (r=0.80, df=20, p<0.001) was seen in the healthy subjects. Neurological soft sign subscales were analyzed separately. Only for the motor coordination subscale were significant results detected for time, diagnosis, and the time-by-diagnosis interaction.

To further investigate the decrease of neurological soft signs in patients, this group was split according to the median change in neurological soft sign total scores. Subsequently, 21 patients with a pronounced neurological soft sign decrease were compared with 18 patients with stable or increasing neurological soft sign levels and to healthy subjects by means of a repeated measures ANOVA, which yielded a significant difference (Table 2). Post hoc Duncan tests revealed that both patient groups were comparable at first assessment whereas the group experiencing a decrease in neurological soft sign levels took an intermediate position on follow-up and did not differ statistically from healthy subjects (Figure 1).

Additional analyses of the three groups were performed for the neurological soft sign subscales (Table 2) where significant differences emerged for motor coordination, sensory integration, and complex motor tasks.

The differences between patients and healthy subjects were not related to handedness, gender, family history of psychiatric disease, or any other sociodemographic variable.

Neurological Soft Sign Course and Clinical Measures

Table 3 depicts sociodemographic and clinical data for patients with decreasing and stable neurological soft sign scores. No differences between groups emerged with respect to gender, social relationships, household duties, relapse, or alcohol/cannabis use. The subgroup with decreasing neurological soft sign levels showed better compliance and educational and vocational achievement. Time-by-group interactions revealed more favorable findings for patients with decreasing neurological soft sign levels as opposed to those with stable levels in terms of scores on the Strauss-Carpenter Scale and the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale. In addition, for the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale there were significant main effects of group for the total score (F=5.0, df=1, 37, p<0.05), positive symptom score (F=5.0, df=1, 37, p<0.05), and global psychopathology score (F=4.3, df=1, 37, p<0.05).

A regression analysis was calculated for neurological soft signs at the follow-up evaluation to identify predictors of neurological soft sign decrease. Variables that preceded the second assessment were entered. The analysis revealed neurological soft sign levels at remission and compliance with treatment during the follow-up period to be relevant influences (Table 4).

Discussion

To our knowledge this is the first prospective longitudinal study to compare neurological soft signs in first-episode schizophrenia patients and healthy subjects. Whereas soft signs remained almost stable in healthy subjects, they significantly decreased in first-episode schizophrenia patients during a follow-up period of 14 months. This effect was related to better outcome. Despite the significant decrease, neurological soft signs remained elevated in patients relative to healthy subjects. Our findings support the initial hypotheses and contribute to the understanding of neurological soft signs in schizophrenia patients.

Neurological soft signs were present to a significantly greater extent in patients than in comparison subjects at both measurement points. The level of neurological soft signs at remission (time 1) is well within the range of remission scores in an earlier study by our group (10), and follow-up neurological soft sign levels of patients with decreasing scores closely corresponded to scores of unaffected co-twins of monozygotic twins discordant for schizophrenia (7). Our results clearly confirm the general finding of increased neurological soft sign scores in schizophrenia patients (1, 2, 10, 26), including first-episode cases, and accord with the view that neurological soft signs range among the most consistent neurobiological characteristics of schizophrenia (27).

During the follow-up period, neurological soft sign scores clearly decreased in patients but remained almost unchanged on a low level in healthy subjects. In particular, patients with decreasing neurological soft sign scores experienced further stabilization of symptoms and functioning, whereas clinical findings of patients with stable scores foreshadowed a chronic course. As suggested in previous studies (10, 28), this effect arose through disturbed motor and sensory integration signs rather than orientation difficulties or hard signs. Similar findings were obtained in earlier studies by our group, namely a parallel decrease of neurological soft signs and acute symptoms in patients with remitting schizophrenia as well as in first-episode patients under initial treatment with a typical neuroleptic (1012). Further support of a positive correlation between improvement in clinical status and neurological performance stems from studies describing a significant improvement of neurological soft signs during a 6-month follow-up period (26), and more pronounced long-term deterioration in neuroleptic-free compared with medicated patients (28). Along these lines, cross-sectional studies have consistently reported an association of neurological soft signs with increased symptom levels, poor premorbid adjustment, and unfavorable outcome (1, 3, 10, 29) as well as with neurobiological measures such as neuropsychological deficits and structural and functional cerebral abnormalities (1014). A correlation between neurological soft signs and negative symptoms was especially reported in drug-naive and medicated patients (2). Both neurological soft signs and negative symptoms might be a consequence of dopaminergic hypoactivity. This notion accords with the difference in neurological soft signs between medication responders and nonresponders (10, 12, 26) as well as with the finding that neurological soft signs are most prominent in chronic forms of schizophrenia (1). The aforementioned relationships were also confirmed by our study, since neurological soft signs were significantly related to the different symptom dimensions and predictors of outcome at both measurement points. Thus, our results hint at the necessity to differentiate between patient subgroups according to their symptoms and outcome.

Since neurological soft signs are present before medication exposure, it is generally accepted that they are an intrinsic feature of schizophrenia rather than a side effect of medication (2, 3). This view is supported by reports on spontaneous abnormal involuntary movements in schizophrenia patients, which had already been observed in the preneuroleptic era (30). Further support for a genetic determination stems from studies on relatives of schizophrenia patients (7, 8). Of interest in our study was that follow-up neurological soft sign scores of patients with a favorable outcome were in the range of unaffected co-twins of monozygotic twin pairs discordant for schizophrenia (7). This is in line with evidence from studies on neuroleptic-naive patients (3, 12, 27) and suggests that a remitting disease course leads to an increase in neurological soft sign levels during a limited period of time (i.e., an acute psychotic exacerbation) and then a return to the genetically determined baseline thereafter. These results are consistent with Meehl’s model of “schizotaxia” (16) in which dysdiadochokinesia—i.e., a sign of dysfunctional motor coordination—is conceptualized as a marker of the baseline defect (“hypokrisia”). On the other hand, the persisting elevation of neurological soft sign scores in chronic cases may hint at a still enduring process activity reflecting premorbid changes of neurodevelopmental origin (31) or the presence of acquired, irreversible deficits.

Thus, neurological soft signs in schizophrenia seem to adopt characteristics of both state-like and trait-like features. During the early course of acute psychosis when symptoms fluctuate, the state-like features of an active disease process (17, 18) may be predominant. On the other hand, the trait-like features that represent the genetically determined baseline may prevail after remission of the acute illness. In conclusion, our results and the body of literature allow for the notion that neurological soft signs represent surrogate markers of the schizophrenic disease process, i.e., the process activity that is more prominent and fluctuating during the early course of the disease; in later phases of the disease the process may come to a standstill, but it may also completely resolve or deteriorate into deficit states (17, 18).

This study may be limited by a recruitment bias. However, patients initially were included in the study consecutively as they necessitated hospital treatment. Neuroleptic medication was not standardized but restricted to atypical compounds, chosen according to the patients’ individual needs, and extrapyramidal side effects were rare and not associated with neurological soft signs. Most important, compliance with medication was a positive predictor of neurological soft sign decrease. In general, the comparability among studies is limited because of the absence of a universally accepted structured instrument. However, most instruments in use comprise a set of similar subtests, e.g., tandem gait, Romberg, diadochokinesis, finger-nose tapping, finger-thumb opposition, fist-edge palm test, Ozeretski’s test, mirror movements, graphesthesia, stereognosis, right/left orientation (32). Therefore, overall data comparability among studies is relatively high. In spite of this methodological limitation, the evidence for a higher rate of neurological abnormalities in schizophrenia is consistent and compelling. The test-retest reliability of our Heidelberg Scale over a longer period of time was sufficient.

In summary, neurological soft signs are intrinsic to schizophrenia but their measured quantity and magnitude may serve as a surrogate marker for the activity of the disease process as well as a predictor of outcome. Overall, the assessment of neurological soft signs represents a hardly time-consuming, inexpensive, and meaningful tool in clinical psychiatry and has the potential to bridge the gulf between neurobiological research and clinical practice.

TABLE 1
TABLE 2
TABLE 3
TABLE 4

Presented in part at the ninth International Congress on Schizophrenia Research, Colorado Springs, Colo., March 29–April 2, 2003. Received Aug. 13, 2004; revision received Dec. 20, 2004; accepted Feb. 9, 2005. From the Section of Geriatric Psychiatry, Department of Psychiatry, University of Heidelberg, Germany. Address correspondence and reprint requests to Dr. Schröder, Professor of Psychiatry, Section of Geriatric Psychiatry, University of Heidelberg, Voss-Str. 4, 69115 Heidelberg, Germany; (e-mail). Supported by the Medical Faculty of the University of Heidelberg and in part by the Theodore and Vada Stanley Foundation. The authors thank Paul Richter, Ph.D., for statistical advice and Aoife Hunt, M.D., for proofreading the manuscript.

APPENDIX 1

APPENDIX 1

Enlarge table
Figure 1.

Figure 1. Neurological Soft Sign Levels at Remission and 14-Month Follow-Up Evaluation in First-Episode Schizophrenia Patients Classified by Symptom Course and Age- and Gender-Matched Healthy Comparison Subjectsa

aThe patient group was dichotomized by a median split according to neurological soft sign score changes into those with decreasing (N=21) and those with stable neurological soft sign levels (N=18) (F=64.81, df=2, 58, p<0.001, repeated-measures analysis of variance).

References

1. Heinrichs DW, Buchanan RW: Significance and meaning of neurological signs in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 1988; 145:11–18LinkGoogle Scholar

2. Dazzan P, Murray RM: Neurological soft signs in first-episode psychosis: a systematic review. Br J Psychiatry 2002; 181:S50-S57Google Scholar

3. Browne S, Clarke M, Gervin M, Lane A, Waddington JL, Larkin C, O’Callaghan E: Determinants of neurological dysfunction in first episode schizophrenia. Psychol Med 2000; 30:1433–1441Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

4. Keshavan MS, Sanders RD, Sweeney JA, Diwadkar VA, Goldstein G, Pettegrew JW, Schooler NR: Diagnostic specificity and neuroanatomical validity of neurological abnormalities in first-episode psychoses. Am J Psychiatry 2003; 160:1298–1304LinkGoogle Scholar

5. Lawrie SM, Byrne M, Miller P, Hodges A, Clafferty RA, Cunningham Owens DG, Johnstone EC: Neurodevelopmental indices and the development of psychotic symptoms in subjects at high risk of schizophrenia. Br J Psychiatry 2001; 178:524–530Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

6. Ismail B, Cantor-Graae E, McNeil TF: Neurological abnormalities in schizophrenic patients and their siblings. Am J Psychiatry 1998; 155:84–89LinkGoogle Scholar

7. Niethammer R, Weisbrod M, Schiesser S, Grothe J, Maier S, Peter U, Kaufmann C, Schröder J, Sauer H: Genetic influence on laterality in schizophrenia? a twin study of neurological soft signs. Am J Psychiatry 2000; 157:272–274LinkGoogle Scholar

8. Manschreck TC, Ames D: Neurological features and psychopathology. Biol Psychiatry 1984; 19:703–719MedlineGoogle Scholar

9. Arango C, Kirkpatrick B, Buchanan RW: Neurological signs and the heterogeneity of schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 2000; 157:560–565LinkGoogle Scholar

10. Schröder J, Niethammer R, Geider FJ, Reitz C, Binkert M, Jauß M, Sauer H: Neurological soft signs in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 1992; 6:25–30CrossrefGoogle Scholar

11. Schröder J, Tittel A, Stockert A, Karr M: Memory deficits in subsyndromes of chronic schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 1996; 21:19–26Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

12. Schröder J, Silvestri S, Bubeck B, Karr M, Demisch S, Scherrer S, Geider FJ, Sauer H: D2 dopamine receptor up-regulation, treatment response, neurological soft signs, and extrapyramidal side effects in schizophrenia: a follow-up study with 123I-iodobenzamide single photon emission computed tomography in the drug-naive state and after neuroleptic treatment. Biol Psychiatry 1998; 43:660–665Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

13. Schröder J, Essig M, Baudendistel K, Jahn T, Gerdsen I, Stockert A, Schad LR, Knopp MV: Motor dysfunction and sensorimotor cortex activation changes in schizophrenia: a study with functional magnetic resonance imaging. Neuroimage 1999; 9:81–87Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

14. Bottmer C, Bachmann S, Pantel J, Essig M, Amann M, Schad L, Schröder J: Reduced cerebellar volume and neurological soft signs in first-episode schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res (in press)Google Scholar

15. Andreasen NC, O’Leary DS, Cizadlo T, Arndt S, Rezai K, Boles Ponto LL, Watkins GL, Hichwa RD: Schizophrenia and cognitive dysmetria: a positron-emission tomography study of dysfunctional prefrontal-thalamic-cerebellar circuitry. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1996; 93:9985–9990Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

16. Meehl PE: Toward an integrated theory of schizotaxia, schizotypy, and schizophrenia. J Personal Disord 1990; 4:1–99CrossrefGoogle Scholar

17. Huber G: Psychiatrie, 5th ed. Stuttgart, Germany, Schattauer, 1994, p 343Google Scholar

18. Huber G: Pneumencephalographische und psychopathologische Bilder bei Schizophrenie. Berlin, Springer, 1957, pp 158–245Google Scholar

19. Oldfield RC: The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh Inventory. Neuropsychologia 1971; 9:97–113Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

20. Wittchen HU, Wunderlich U, Gruschwitz S, Zaudig M: SKID-I: Strukturiertes klinisches Interview für DSM-IV. Göttingen, Hogrefe, 1997Google Scholar

21. Kay SR, Fiszbein A, Opler LA: The Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 1987; 13:261–276Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

22. Strauss JS, Carpenter WT Jr: The prediction of outcome in schizophrenia, II: relationship between predictors and outcome variables. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1974; 31:37–42Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

23. Simpson GM, Angus JWS: A rating scale for extrapyramidal side effects. Acta Psychiatr Scand Suppl 1970; 212:11–19Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

24. Barnes TRE: A rating scale for drug-induced akathisia. Br J Psychiatry 1989; 154:672–676Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

25. Guy W (ed): ECDEU Assessment Manual for Psychopharmacology: Publication ADM 76–338. Washington, DC, US Department of Health, Education, and Welfare, 1976, pp 534–537Google Scholar

26. Madsen AL, Vorstrup S, Rubin P, Larsen JK, Hemmingsen R: Neurological abnormalities in schizophrenic patients: a prospective follow-up study 5 years after first admission. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1999; 100:119–125Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

27. Torrey EF: Studies of individuals with schizophrenia never treated with antipsychotic medications: a review. Schizophr Res 2002; 58:101–115Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

28. Whitty P, Clarke M, Browne S, McTigue O, Kamali M, Feeney L, Lane A, Kinsella A, Waddington JL, Larkin C, O’Callaghan E: Prospective evaluation of neurological soft signs in first-episode schizophrenia in relation to psychopathology: state versus trait phenomena. Psychol Med 2003; 33:1479–1484Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

29. Johnstone EC, Macmillan JF, Frith CD, Benn DK, Crow TJ: Further investigation of the predictors of outcome following first schizophrenic episodes. Br J Psychiatry 1990; 157:182–189Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

30. Kraepelin E: Psychiatrie. Leipzig, Germany, Barth, 1913, p 751Google Scholar

31. Weinberger DR: Schizophrenia as a neurodevelopmental disorder, in Schizophrenia. Edited by Hirsch SR, Weinberger DR. Oxford, UK, Blackwell Science, 1995, pp 293–323Google Scholar

32. Sanders RD, Keshavan MS: The neurologic examination in adult psychiatry: from soft signs to hard science. J Neuropsychiatry 1998; 10:395–404Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar