The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×

Abstract

Objective

Despite increased exposure to cancer risk factors, several studies have demonstrated a lower incidence of cancer in schizophrenia patients than in the general population. Lower cancer rates in first-degree relatives of schizophrenia patients suggest that the inverse relationship between cancer and schizophrenia may be related to genetic factors. Few studies of schizophrenia have focused on cancer-related genes. The MET proto-oncogene is primarily linked to tumor metastasis, but MET is also involved in neurodevelopment and influences risk for autism. Thus, MET may be of particular interest as a candidate gene for neuropsychiatric diseases with a developmental etiology, including schizophrenia.

Method

The authors examined the relationship between 21 single-nucleotide polymorphisms in MET and schizophrenia in 173 Caucasian patients and 137 comparison subjects. They then genotyped a second independent sample (107 patients and 112 comparison subjects) for replication. Finally, they tested for MET's effects on general cognitive ability (g).

Results

In the initial cohort, the authors identified four haplotype blocks and found one block to be globally associated with schizophrenia. In block 3, the most common haplotype was overrepresented in comparison subjects (frequency, 47%) relative to schizophrenia patients (frequency, 33%) (p=4.0×10–4; odds ratio=0.56). The authors replicated the block 3 finding in the second sample with similar frequencies: 46% in comparison subjects and 36% in schizophrenia patients (p=0.03; odds ratio=0.66). Moreover, the protective haplotype was associated with a higher g in the combined comparison sample.

Conclusion

These data suggest that MET variation influences schizophrenia risk and neurocognition, supporting a neurodevelopmental role across CNS-relevant phenotypes. These results add to the growing evidence suggesting an intriguing relationship between cancer-related genes and schizophrenia susceptibility.

Several studies have demonstrated a significantly lower incidence of cancer in schizophrenia patients relative to the general population. This association is counterintuitive, given health risks in the majority of schizophrenia patients, such as heavy smoking, poor dietary habits, obesity, and substance abuse (1).

Hippisley-Cox et al. (2) conducted a study to determine the risk for six common cancers in schizophrenia patients. In a sample of 40,441 incident cases of cancer, odds ratios for cancer risk associated with schizophrenia and bipolar disorder were calculated with adjustments made for smoking, body mass index, socioeconomic status, comorbid physical conditions, and medication use. Analyses revealed a 47% lower risk for respiratory cancer in schizophrenia patients relative to patients without schizophrenia. In contrast, risk rates associated with bipolar illness in this cohort were comparable to those reported in nonpsychiatric samples, suggesting a relatively specific effect in schizophrenia. These findings are consistent with a recent meta-analysis of cancer incidence in schizophrenia patients and their first-degree relatives, which reported lower rates for a number of non-smoking-related cancers in schizophrenia patients as well as a lower risk for lung cancer after controlling for smoking prevalence (1). Taken together, these studies suggest that the discrepancy between cancer risk exposure and cancer incidence in schizophrenia is consistent with a protective effect; however, the nature of the proposed protective effect is unknown.

It has been hypothesized that decreased cancer susceptibility in schizophrenia is either related to medications used to treat the illness (3) or genetically influenced, linked with tumor suppressor genes (4) or enhanced natural killer cell activity (5). To date, results are inconsistent with regard to the potentially protective effects of antipsychotic medication, with some studies reporting decreased cancer rates in patients treated with antipsychotics (6) while others have reported an increased incidence of certain cancers (i.e., colon) in patients treated with antipsychotics compared with those not treated with these agents (2). A genetic influence on the paradoxical cancer-schizophrenia relationship is supported by family studies, in which decreased cancer rates have been reported in unaffected family members of schizophrenia patients never exposed to antipsychotic medications (1, 79), as well as case-control studies describing associations between schizophrenia and the tumor suppressor genes adenomatous polyposis coli (APC) (10) and p53 (11, 12). Tumor suppressor genes may act to increase risk for schizophrenia by disruption of cell growth or through abnormal apoptosis during neurodevelopment while simultaneously acting to decrease risk for cancer due to the same—in this case advantageous—apoptotic mechanism (4).

While previous studies have focused on tumor suppressor genes and their potential role in disorders such as schizophrenia, one widely studied cancer-related gene is the MET proto-oncogene, whose primary functions are related to the metastasis of several forms of cancer and peripheral organ development and repair (1316). MET spans 125 kb on chromosome 7q31, consists of 21 exons, and codes for the MET receptor tyrosine kinase. While activation of the MET signaling pathway can result in the abnormal growth and spread of cancerous tumors, MET also plays a critical role in cortical and cerebellar development (1720), making it a candidate gene of interest for a broad range of neuropsychiatric disorders with a neurodevelopmental etiology, including autism. Campbell et al. (21) reported that the C allele at a single nucleotide polymorphism (SNP; rs1858830) in the promoter region of MET was significantly overtransmitted to affected family members in two independent family cohorts ascertained for autism spectrum disorders (21) and subsequently replicated this finding in a third independent sample (22).

Although DSM-IV-TR criteria exclude the presence of schizophrenia and autism in the same individual (23), both disorders may be related to abnormal neurodevelopment. Although the two illnesses are clearly distinct, they have a number of clinical features in common. Autism spectrum disorders are characterized by social deficits, abnormal language development, restricted interests, and repetitive behaviors. In addition, approximately 30% of individuals with autism are significantly cognitively impaired, although the degree of impairment varies by disorder subtype (24). Similarly, schizophrenia patients also commonly display social deficits (asociality), anhedonia (23), and significant neurocognitive impairment (25). Finally, data from family and molecular genetic studies suggest an overlap between these illnesses (26).

To date, there have been no studies examining the relationship between MET and susceptibility to schizophrenia or other phenotypes common to autism and schizophrenia, such as neurocognitive impairment. Thus, we initially conducted a case-control study comprising 173 Caucasian schizophrenia patients and 137 Caucasian healthy comparison subjects. We genotyped 21 SNPs within MET and tested for an association with schizophrenia. We next carried out a replication study of the same SNPs in a second sample of 107 patients with schizophrenia and 112 healthy comparison subjects. Finally, we explored the effect of MET variation on neurocognition in a combined sample of 191 schizophrenia patients and 188 healthy comparison subjects.

Method

Participants

The initial study group included 173 Caucasian schizophrenia patients; 110 of them were male; the group's mean age was 37.7 years (SD=10.7); their mean age at illness onset was 21.4 years (SD=5.9); their mean Global Assessment of Functioning Scale (GAF) score was 39.4 (SD=17); and their mean estimated IQ, based on the Wide-Range Achievement Test, 3rd ed. (WRAT-3), was 96.5 (SD=12.4). The second sample comprised 107 schizophrenia patients; 70 of them were male; their mean age was 37.4 years (SD=11.6); their mean age at illness onset was 24.4 years (SD=8.4); their mean GAF score was 42.1 (SD=16.6); and their mean estimated IQ was 96.6 (SD=12.8). Patients for both the initial and the replication samples were recruited from the Zucker Hillside Hospital in Glen Oaks, New York. All patients provided written informed consent to the study protocol, which was approved by the hospital's institutional review board.

Caucasian healthy comparison subjects were recruited from the general population; potential participants were excluded if they had a DSM-IV axis I diagnosis or a first-degree relative with a known or suspected axis I disorder or if they had a history of CNS trauma, neurological disorder, or previously diagnosed learning disability. In the initial sample of comparison subjects (N=137), 76 were male, the mean age was 42.9 years (SD=13.0), and the mean estimated IQ was 104.2 (SD=9.4). In the replication sample of comparison subjects (N=112), 43 were male, the mean age was 54.8 years (SD=21.9), and the mean estimated IQ was 105.9 (SD=7.2).

Race was self-identified as Caucasian, and population structure was assessed using a principal components analysis approach applied to the full data set (N=365,721 SNPs passing quality control filters [27], implemented in SVS7 software [GoldenHelix, Inc., Bozeman, Mont.], using default settings derived from EIGENSTRAT [28]). The first two principal components had eigenvalues >1 (2.04 and 1.95), and the remaining eight ranged from 0.83 to 0.63. Case and comparison subjects did not differ on the first principal component (p=0.99); however, between-group differences were noted on both the second and third principal components (p values ≤0.001). These two variables were included as covariates in the association analyses described below. There were no significant differences between case and comparison subjects on any of the remaining principal components.

Diagnostic Measures

Patients' diagnoses were established with the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID) (29) and confirmed by diagnostic consensus conference, which utilizes expert clinical opinion alongside SCID and corroborating medical record information. Comparison subjects were assessed with the SCID–Non-Patient Edition to rule out axis I diagnoses.

Cognitive Measures

Patients were clinically stable and were assessed for estimated intellectual functioning (WRAT-3), auditory attention and verbal working memory (WAIS-R, digit span subtest), visual attention (Continuous Performance Test–Identical Pairs Version), rapid visual search (Trail Making Test, part A), verbal learning (California Verbal Learning Test), executive functioning (letter/category fluency), and set-shifting (Trail Making Test, part B) (30). A measure of "general cognitive ability," or g, was calculated with an unrotated principal components analysis, as in our prior work (31). A single-factor model resulted, including extracted variables with eigenvalues >1.0. This single factor explained 49.5% of the variance and represented g. Cognitive analysis focused on g since global cognitive impairment is characteristic of both autism and schizophrenia, making this a phenotype of particular interest with regard to MET.

DNA Analysis

We genotyped 21 SNPs in MET on 7q31 (B36 positions 116057424–116253319) using the Affymetrix 500K chip (SA1). The Tagger program (r2=1.0) (http://www.broad.mit.edu/mpg/tagger/) was used to reduce the redundancy of the included SNPs prior to analyses; 16 SNPs were retained with frequencies >0.05. Using pairwise tagging (r2≥0.80) and downloaded data from the HapMap project CEU sample, we calculated that these 16 SNPs were able to capture 71% (70/98 alleles) of the common allelic variation in the MET region. Linkage disequilibrium structure was examined using Haploview 3.32 (32) with solid spine D′>0.80 (Figure 1). In the replication sample, identical genotyping methods were used (SA1). Phase and diplotype assignments were estimated using PHASE 2.1.1 (33) for each of the MET haplotype blocks individually and in each sample separately.

Figure 1. Linkage Disequilibrium Structure of MET Haplotype Blocks in the Initial Samplea

aLinkage disequilibrium (D′) for the region was computed using Haploview 3.32 (32).

Statistical Analyses

MET disease association

Haploview was used to test for the significance of each haplotype (with frequency ≥10%) within the defined blocks, and global chi-square values were calculated using the VassarStats web site (http://faculty.vassar.edu/lowry/VassarStats.html). Phased individual haplotypes were tested in SPSS, version 11.5 (SPSS, Inc., Chicago), to determine the best model for significant associations, and neurocognitive analyses used the best-fit genetic model. Correction for multiple testing was carried out using permutation testing in Haploview, including 10,000 permutations. For all association analyses, odds ratios were calculated as a measure of effect size with 95% confidence intervals.

MET association with cognition

To optimize statistical power, we tested for MET's effect on cognition using a univariate analysis of covariance first in the combined comparison sample (N=188). Secondary analyses in the schizophrenia cohort included 191 patients with complete neurocognitive data. MET diplotype status was entered as a fixed factor, and age was used as a covariate. Eta2 was calculated as an estimate of effect size.

Results

We first tested for association between MET variation and schizophrenia in the initial sample. Four haplotype blocks were identified, each consisting of three major haplotypes. The strongest association was observed in block 3, with the most common haplotype (GCAATACA) overrepresented in the comparison group (47% frequency) compared with the schizophrenia group (33% frequency) (Table 1). Post hoc analyses indicated that the best fitting model was a dominant model of inheritance, with individuals carrying at least one copy of GCAATACA being significantly less likely to develop schizophrenia as compared with those carrying no copies (χ2=13.4, p=2.5×10–4; odds ratio=0.40; 95% CI=0.24–0.65). Results remained significant after correction with permutation (corrected permutation p=0.0019). Individual SNP associations for the initial sample are presented in Table 2. Three of the SNPs survived correction, all residing in block 3; rs2237717 is located in intron 11, rs41735 is located in intron 19, and rs42336 is just downstream from the MET coding region. At each of these individual SNPs, the associated "protective" allele is the ancestral allele.

Table 1. MET Variation and Schizophrenia in Two Schizophrenia Samples and Healthy Comparison Groupsa
Table 1. MET Variation and Schizophrenia in Two Schizophrenia Samples and Healthy Comparison Groupsa
Enlarge table

For replication, we repeated the block 3 haplotype association analyses in our second independent sample and tested the effect of MET GCAATACA on disease susceptibility. The results were highly consistent with the initial analyses, at both the haplotype and SNP levels. The most common haplotype in block 3 (GCAATACA) was overrepresented in the comparison group (frequency, 46%) relative to the schizophrenia group (frequency, 36%) (Table 1). The dominant model was also significant, with individuals carrying at least one copy of GCAATACA being significantly less likely to develop schizophrenia relative to those carrying no copies (χ2=4.0, p=0.05; odds ratio=0.55, 95% CI=0.30–0.99). SNP associations for the replication sample are consistent with results from the initial sample (Table 2). To address the possible influence of population stratification, we carried out a backward stepwise logistic regression with subject type as the dependent variable and GCAATACA haplotype status, as well as the second and third principal components from the population structure analysis, as independent factors. We found that although both principal components remained in the model (p≤0.001), GCAATACA haplotype remained significant (p=0.001) (SA1).

Table 2. Individual SNP Association With Schizophrenia in Initial and Replication Samples
Table 2. Individual SNP Association With Schizophrenia in Initial and Replication Samples
Enlarge table

MET Association With Cognition

We next tested for an effect of MET on neurocognition. To maximize power, we merged the initial and replication data sets for all participants for whom complete neurocognitive data were available (191 schizophrenia patients and 188 comparison subjects) and tested for effects in each diagnostic group separately.

The sample characteristics are presented in Table 3 by MET GCAATACA haplotype group. The haplotype groups did not differ significantly in age in the comparison group or in the schizophrenia group, nor did they differ in estimated premorbid IQ. Significant differences were noted for sex distribution in the schizophrenia group (χ2=6.8, df=1, p=0.01) but not in the comparison group. In the schizophrenia group, illness characteristics (GAF score, age at illness onset, and duration of illness) did not differ by genotype.

Table 3. Participant Characteristics by MET GCAATACA Haplotype in the Combined Initial and Replication Samples
Table 3. Participant Characteristics by MET GCAATACA Haplotype in the Combined Initial and Replication Samples
Enlarge table

Univariate analyses of covariance in the combined comparison group revealed a significant effect of MET GCAATACA (F=3.99, df=1, 187, p=0.05) and age (F=16.99, df=1, 187, p<0.001) on general cognitive ability (g) (Figure 2). MET GCAATACA carriers had significantly better cognitive performance than noncarriers, with MET haplotype explaining approximately 2.1% of the variance in g. In the schizophrenia group, a similar pattern of performance was revealed, with carriers outperforming noncarriers (Figure 2), although the results did not reach statistical significance (MET GCAATACA, F=1.48, df=1, 190, p=0.23; ŋ2=0.01).

Figure 2. Effects of MET on General Cognitive Ability in the Combined Samplea

aThe x-axis labels the subject groups by MET genotype. The y-axis represents the composite g using a z score scale with a mean of 0 and a standard deviation of 1. The effect of MET GCAATACA on neurocognition is significant in the comparison group (p=0.05) but not in the schizophrenia group (p=0.23).

Discussion

We report a significant influence of genetic variation within the MET proto-oncogene and susceptibility to schizophrenia in two independent cohorts. The strongest association we observed was with a haplotype spanning the majority of the coding region of MET, as participants carrying one or more copies of the most common haplotype (GCAATACA) were significantly less likely to develop schizophrenia than those carrying no copies (odds ratio=0.40; 95% CI=0.3–0.8). These data represent the first report of an association between MET and schizophrenia and the second of an association of MET with susceptibility to neuropsychiatric illness. Campbell et al. (21) reported an association between a SNP within the promoter region of MET (rs1858830) and autism and subsequently replicated this finding and expanded the association to include several other genes within the MET pathway (22).

We also report that the MET GCAATACA genotype had a significant impact on neurocognition, such that healthy MET GCAATACA carriers in the comparison group performed significantly better than noncarriers on an empirically derived factor of general cognitive ability (g). This preliminary evidence of a role for MET in neurocognitive function may shed light on the mechanism through which variation in MET might influence risk for both autism and schizophrenia, as the phenotype of neurocognitive impairment is common to both diseases. These data could be interpreted to indicate that the influence of MET on disease susceptibility might not be specific to risk for schizophrenia or autism. Rather, these findings may be reflective of the effects that MET may have on brain function in general. This potential explanation warrants follow-up in other clinical disorders characterized by brain morphological abnormalities, neurodevelopmental pathology, or cognitive impairment.

The role of MET in cancer has been definitively established. There are several mechanisms by which MET influences cancer development and progression: 1) overexpression of MET—the most common alteration of MET in human tumors; 2) autocrine/paracrine activation in which MET is activated by its ligand, hepatocyte growth factor, which may be abnormally produced by cancer cells; 3) hepatocyte growth factor-independent activation via transactivation by other membrane receptors; and 4) MET structural alterations, such as missense mutations, which result in hereditary forms of cancer. Regardless of the mechanism by which MET activity is altered, increased MET activation is associated with abnormal tissue growth related to tumor development and metastasis. In contrast, MET activation plays a beneficial role during normal physiological states and is critical to normal cortical and cerebellar development (1720). Disrupted MET signaling in the cerebral cortex results in a lower number of interneurons and abnormal interneuron migration from the ganglionic eminence (17, 18). In the cerebellum, reduced MET signaling decreases proliferation of granule cells, resulting in cerebellar volume reductions, especially in the vermis (19). Abnormalities that result from perturbation of this system are consistent with known changes in the brains of schizophrenia patients, including dysfunction and a lower number of inhibitory interneurons in the prefrontal cortex (34) and volumetric reductions in the vermis, both of which are correlated with cognitive dysfunction (35). Taken together, these data suggest that MET may influence risk for disorders such as autism and schizophrenia via a decreased level of pathway activity during critical periods of neurodevelopment. This hypothesis is supported by data from Campbell et al. (21), who found that an autism risk allele (C) at rs1858830 was associated with a reduction in MET transcription. A subsequent study by the same group demonstrated that MET mRNA expression was decreased in the postmortem brain tissue of individuals with autism and that the level of MET expression was associated with rs1858830 in healthy comparison subjects (36). In the present study, minor alleles of multiple intragenic SNPs (such as rs42336) were associated with an elevated risk for schizophrenia; these alleles, or perfect proxies of them, have similarly been associated with reduced gene expression in prior studies (37, 38).

These data implicating the MET gene in schizophrenia, coupled with the autism data, are consistent with a recent study that used computational probabilistic modeling to investigate the relationship among multiple common disorders that might be likely to share genetic susceptibility, including cancer, autism, and schizophrenia. Rzhetsky et al. (39) proposed a disease network hypothesis based on an analysis of more than 1.5 million patient records from a clinical database and tested whether a genetic variant that predisposes an individual to a given disease may increase or decrease the risk for multiple other diseases. As in many Mendelian disorders, the tested hypothesis was that complex phenotypes are probably explained by genetic variation that is shared, in either a competitive or a cooperative manner, by multiple disease phenotypes. Among 161 disorders studied, a highly connected network of pairwise correlations emerged, including a strong positive correlation between autism and schizophrenia (p=5.78×10–11), suggesting significant genetic overlap, such that approximately 20%–75% of autism-predisposing variants were estimated also to predispose to schizophrenia. Of particular note, a strong inverse relationship between schizophrenia and breast cancer was also observed in these analyses (p=1.22×10–10) (39), providing support for the hypothesis that genetic variation at a single locus, such as MET, may have both shared and divergent effects across different phenotypes.

A specific example of the potential pleiotropic effects of MET is provided by a recent study (40) that investigated the effect of the MET promoter SNP rs1858830 on susceptibility to autism in 214 autism families that were characterized for gastrointestinal comorbid conditions. MET rs1858830 was associated with an increased risk for autism and increased susceptibility to gastrointestinal disease in 118 families with at least one child with co-occurring autism and a gastrointestinal condition; however, no association with autism was observed in families with children who did not have a comorbid gastrointestinal disease. These data provide preliminary evidence that decreased activity in the MET pathway may have both central and peripheral effects, contributing to abnormal brain development (autism) as well as dysfunctional organ repair (40).

Our study has a number of limitations. First, medications may confound analyses in samples of patients with chronic schizophrenia; however, we would not expect that medication type or dosage would differ significantly by genotype, as the severity of illness was comparable between groups in our sample. We did not have sufficient data on medication dosage to conduct a formal test of this question. Second, the genotyped markers were selected using an Affymetrix 500K chip based on genomic spacing; we were able to capture only 71% of the common allelic variation within the MET region using the 16 SNPs analyzed. The use of genome-wide association methods raises the issue as to which analyses are considered appropriate in the context of statistically nonsignificant genome-wide results. In the strictest statistical sense, analytic follow-up of results that do not meet the genome-wide threshold after correction may not be considered valid. However, we suggest that genes such as MET, with prior data suggestive of biological plausibility related to pathophysiology, also warrant follow-up, especially given the previous association with autism and the consistency of our results in two independent samples. Finally, we acknowledge that the magnitude of the effect of MET on both disease susceptibility and neurocognitive function is relatively small, which is consistent with the complexity of the genetic architecture of schizophrenia and its related phenotypes.

The data we present here highlight the importance of assessing molecular networks that may be implicated in the pathophysiology of multiple CNS disorders with overlapping phenotypes. Our results also provide evidence for at least one potential genetic locus that may explain both a cooperative polymorphism model (increasing risk for both schizophrenia and autism) and a competitive polymorphism model (increasing risk for schizophrenia and protecting against cancer) via a specific hypothesized biological mechanism.

Received May 4, 2009; revisions received June 15 and Aug. 10, 2009; accepted Oct. 5, 2009. From the Zucker Hillside Hospital, North Shore-Long Island Jewish Health System, Department of Psychiatry Research, Glen Oaks, N.Y.; Albert Einstein College of Medicine, Department of Psychiatry, New York; and the Feinstein Institute for Medical Research, Manhasset, New York. Address correspondence to Dr. Burdick, Zucker Hillside Hospital, North Shore-Long Island Jewish Health System, 75-59 263rd St., Glen Oaks, NY 11004; (e-mail).

Drs. Burdick and DeRosse report no financial relationships with commercial interests. Dr. Kane has served as a consultant or speaker or on advisory boards for Abbott, AstraZeneca, Bristol-Myers Squibb, Clinical Data, Inc., Eli Lilly, Janssen, Johnson & Johnson PRD, Lundbeck, Otsuka, Pfizer, Vanda Pharmaceuticals, and Wyeth Pharmaceuticals. Dr. Lencz has served as a consultant to Eli Lilly, GoldenHelix, Clinical Data, Inc., and InforMed Insights. Dr. Malhotra has served as a consultant or speaker for Bristol-Myers Squibb, Clinical Data, Inc., Eli Lilly, AstraZeneca, and Vanda Pharmaceuticals and has received research support from Bristol-Myers Squibb, Eli Lilly, Janssen Pharmaceuticals, and Pfizer.

Drs. Lencz and Malhotra contributed equally to this work.

References

1 Catts VS , Catts SV , O'Toole BI , Frost AD : Cancer incidence in patients with schizophrenia and their first-degree relatives: a meta-analysis. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2008; 117:323–336 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

2 Hippisley-Cox J , Vinogradova Y , Coupland C , Parker C : Risk of malignancy in patients with schizophrenia or bipolar disorder: nested case-control study. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2007; 64:1368–1376 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

3 Carrillo JA , Benítez J : Are antipsychotic drugs potentially chemopreventive agents for cancer? Eur J Clin Pharmacol 1999; 55:487–488 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

4 Catts VS , Catts SV : Apoptosis and schizophrenia: is the tumour suppressor gene, p53, a candidate susceptibility gene? Schizophr Res 2000; 41:405–415 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

5 Yovel G , Sirota P , Mazeh D , Shakhar G , Rosenne E , Ben-Eliyahu S : Higher natural killer cell activity in schizophrenic patients: the impact of serum factors, medication, and smoking. Brain Behav Immun 2000; 14:153–169 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

6 Dalton SO , Johansen C , Poulsen AH , NØrgaard M , SØrensen HT , McLaughlin JK , Mortensen PB , Friis S : Cancer risk among users of neuroleptic medication: a population-based cohort study. Br J Cancer 2006; 95:934–939 Google Scholar

7 Levav I , Lipshitz I , Novikov I , Pugachova I , Kohn R , Barchana M , Ponizovsky A , Werner H : Cancer risk among parents and siblings of patients with schizophrenia. Br J Psychiatry 2007; 190:156–161 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

8 Lichtermann D , Ekelund J , Pukkala E , Tanskanen A , Lonnqvist J : Incidence of cancer among persons with schizophrenia and their relatives. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2001; 58:573–578 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

9 Dalton SO , Laursen SM , Mellemkjær L , Johansen C , Mortensen PB : Risk for cancer in parents of patients with schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 2004; 161:903–908 LinkGoogle Scholar

10 Cui DH , Jiang KD , Jiang SD , Xu YF , Yao H : The tumor suppressor adenomatous polyposis coli gene is associated with susceptibility to schizophrenia. Mol Psychiatry 2005; 10:669–677 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

11 Park JK , Lee HJ , Kim JW , Park YH , Lee SS , Chang HI , Song JY , Yoon DJ , Bahn GH , Shin YH , Kim YJ , Kim SA , Choe BK , Kim CJ , Chung JH : Differences in p53 gene polymorphisms between Korean schizophrenia and lung cancer patients. Schizophr Res 2004; 67:71–74 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

12 Yang Y , Xiao Z , Chen W , Sang H , Guan Y , Peng Y , Zhang D , Gu Z , Qian M , He G , Qin W , Li D , Gu N , He L : Tumor suppressor gene TP53 is genetically associated with schizophrenia in the Chinese population. Neurosci Lett 2004; 369:126–131 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

13 Birchmeier C , Birchmeier W , Gherardi E , Vande Woude GF : Met, metastasis, motility, and more. Nat Rev Mol Cell Biol 2003; 4:915–925 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

14 Tahara Y , Ido A , Yamamoto S , Miyata Y , Uto H , Hori T , Hayashi K , Tsubouchi H : Hepatocyte growth factor facilitates colonic mucosal repair in experimental ulcerative colitis in rats. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 2003; 307:146–151 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

15 Zhang YM , Vande Woude GF : HGF/SF-met signaling in the control of branching morphogenesis and invasion. J Cell Biochem 2003; 88:408–417 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

16 Huh CG , Factor VM , Sánchez A , Uchida K , Conner EA , Thorgeirsson SS : Hepatocyte growth factor/c-met signaling pathway is required for efficient liver regeneration and repair. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2004; 101:4477–4482 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

17 Powell EM , Mars WM , Levitt P : Hepatocyte growth factor/scatter factor is a motogen for interneurons migrating from the ventral to dorsal telencephalon. Neuron 2001; 30:79–89 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

18 Powell EM , Campbell DB , Stanwood GD , Davis C , Noebels JL , Levitt P : Genetic disruption of cortical interneuron development causes region- and GABA cell type-specific deficits, epilepsy, and behavioral dysfunction. J Neurosci 2003; 23:622–631 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

19 Ieraci A , Forni PE , Ponzetto C : Viable hypomorphic signaling mutant of the Met receptor reveals a role for hepatocyte growth factor in postnatal cerebellar development. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2002; 99:15200–15205 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

20 Levitt P , Eagleson KL , Powell EM : Regulation of neocortical interneuron development and the implications for neurodevelopmental disorders. Trends Neurosci 2004; 27:400–406 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

21 Campbell DB , Sutcliffe JS , Ebert PJ , Militerni R , Bravaccio C , Trillo S , Elia M , Schneider C , Melmed R , Sacco R , Persico AM , Levitt P : A genetic variant that disrupts MET transcription is associated with autism. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2006; 103:16834–16839 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

22 Campbell DB , Sutcliffe JS , Persico AM , Levitt P : Genetic evidence implicating multiple genes in the MET receptor tyrosine kinase pathway in autism spectrum disorder. Autism Res 2008; 1:159–168 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

23 American Psychiatric Association: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th ed, Text Revision (DSM-IV-TR). Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Association, 2000 Google Scholar

24 Chakrabarti S , Fombonne E : Pervasive developmental disorders in preschool children: confirmation of high prevalence. Am J Psychiatry 2005; 162:1133–1141 LinkGoogle Scholar

25 Keefe RS , Fenton WS : How should DSM-V criteria for schizophrenia include cognitive impairment? Schizophr Bull 2007; 33:912–920 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

26 Daniels JL , Forssen U , Hultman CM , Cnattingius S , Savitz DA , Feychting M , Sparen P : Parental psychiatric disorders associated with autism spectrum disorders in the offspring. Pediatrics 2008; 121:1357–1362 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

27 Lencz T , Morgan TV , Athanasiou M , Dain B , Reed CR , Kane JM , Kucherlapati R , Malhotra AK : Converging evidence for a pseudoautosomal cytokine receptor gene locus in schizophrenia. Mol Psychiatry 2007; 12:572–580 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

28 Price AL , Patterson NJ , Plenge RM , Weinblatt ME , Shadick NA , Reich D : Principal components analysis corrects for stratification in genome-wide association studies. Nat Genet 2006; 38:904–909 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

29 First MB , Spitzer RL , Gibbon M , Williams JBW : Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders, Patient Edition (SCID-P), version 2. New York, New York State Psychiatric Institute, Biometrics Research, 1995 Google Scholar

30 Spreen O , Strauss E : A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests: Administration, Norms, and Commentary, 2nd ed. New York, Oxford University Press, 1998 Google Scholar

31 Burdick KE , Lencz T , Funke B , Finn CT , Szeszko PR , Kane JM , Kucherlapati R , Malhotra AK : Genetic variation in DTNBP1 influences general cognitive ability. Hum Mol Genet 2006; 15:1563–1568 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

32 Barrett JC , Fry B , Maller J , Daly MJ : Haploview: analysis and visualization of LD and haplotype maps. Bioinformatics 2005; 21:263–265 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

33 Stephens M , Donnelly P : A comparison of Bayesian methods for haplotype reconstruction from population genotype data. Am J Hum Genet 2003; 73:1162–1169 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

34 Lewis DA , Hashimoto T , Volk DW : Cortical inhibitory neurons and schizophrenia. Nat Rev Neurosci 2005; 6:312–324 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

35 Laywer G , Nyman H , Agartz I , Arnborg S , Jonsson EG , Sedvall GC , Hall H : Morphological correlates to cognitive dysfunction in schizophrenia as studied with Bayesian regression. BMC Psychiatry 2006; 6:31 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

36 Campbell DB , D'Oronzio R , Garbett K , Ebert PJ , Mirnics K , Levitt P , Persico AM : Disruption of cerebral cortex MET signaling in autism spectrum disorder. Ann Neurol 2007; 62:243–250 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

37 Lo HS , Wang Z , Hu Y , Yang HH , Gere S , Buetow KH , Lee MP : Allelic variation in gene expression is common in the human genome. Genome Res 2003; 13:1855–1862 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

38 Milani L , Gupta M , Andersen M , Dhar S , Fryknäs M , Isaksson A , Larsson R , Syvänen AC : Allelic imbalance in gene expression as a guide to cis-acting regulatory single nucleotide polymorphisms in cancer cells. Nucleic Acids Res 2007; 35:34 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

39 Rzhetsky A , Wajngurt D , Park N , Zheng T : Probing genetic overlap among complex human phenotypes. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2007; 104:11694–11699 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

40 Campbell DB , Buie TM , Winter H , Bauman M , Sutcliffe JS , Perrin JM , Levitt P : Distinct genetic risk based on association of MET in families with co-occurring autism and gastrointestinal conditions. Pediatrics 2009; 123:1018–1024 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar