The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×

Abstract

Objective:

Observational studies of prenatal antidepressant safety are hindered by methodological concerns, including susceptibility to surveillance bias. Some studies address potential bias by using alternative strategies to operationalize study comparison groups. In a meta-analysis of the association between prenatal antidepressant exposure and autism risk, the authors examined the utility of comparison group operationalization in reducing surveillance bias.

Methods:

A systematic search of multiple databases through August 2017 was conducted, selecting controlled observational studies of the association of prenatal antidepressant exposure with autism. Study quality was assessed using the Newcastle-Ottawa Scale. Random-effects meta-analysis produced summary effect measures with 95% confidence intervals stratified by comparator group composition, antidepressant class, and trimester of exposure.

Results:

Fourteen studies were included, with 13 reporting results using a population-based comparison group, five using a psychiatric control group, and four using a discordant-sibling control group. Eight of the 14 studies were rated poor because of inadequate control for prenatal depression and maternal ethnicity. Autism risk estimates after prenatal exposure to any antidepressant were decidedly different for population-based designs (hazard ratio=1.42, 95% CI=1.18, 1.70; odds ratio=1.58, 95% CI=1.25, 1.99) compared with psychiatric control (hazard ratio=1.14, 95% CI=0.84, 1.53; odds ratio=1.24, 95% CI=0.93, 1.66) and discordant-sibling (hazard ratio=0.97, 95% CI=0.68, 1.37; odds ratio=0.85, 95% CI=0.54, 1.35) designs. Findings for prenatal exposure to selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors were similar. Meta-regression of population-based studies demonstrated that despite statistical adjustment, ethnicity differences remained a significant source of study heterogeneity.

Conclusions:

In this meta-analysis, neither psychiatric control nor discordant-sibling designs supported an association between prenatal antidepressant exposure and autism. Discordant-sibling designs effectively addressed surveillance bias in pharmacovigilance reports derived from national registries and other large databases.

Optimizing clinical management of major depressive disorder during pregnancy entails weighing the respective risks, to mother and baby alike, of prenatal major depressive disorder compared with prenatal antidepressant exposure (for a review, see reference 1). Unfortunately, because ethical considerations generally preclude conducting randomized clinical trials to evaluate antidepressant safety or efficacy during gestation, risk estimates are derived from observational studies, which are susceptible to numerous sources of bias and confounding. For example, physician awareness of prenatal antidepressant exposure may generate more intense screening for potential adverse outcomes, creating an ascertainment (surveillance) bias affecting even large-scale national and health system databases (23). In addition, purportedly prospective studies of prenatal safety often rely on retrospective data collection, which has been shown to introduce a recall bias, potentially overestimating the effect of antidepressant exposure (4). Recognizing these and other deficiencies, a 2014 review by the Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality (AHRQ) of the U.S. Department of Health and Human Services found that studies of prenatal antidepressant safety are “inadequate to allow well-informed decisions…because comparison groups were not exclusively depressed women” (5). Studies of the purported association between antidepressant therapy during pregnancy and subsequent diagnosis of autism in exposed offspring provide an opportunity to examine the effect of these methodological concerns.

A causal link between fetal antidepressant exposure and autism is biologically plausible and thereby merits rigorous investigation. Converging lines of preclinical and clinical evidence suggest a pivotal neurotropic role for serotonin in neurodevelopment (6) and implicate aberrant serotonin signaling in the pathophysiology of autism spectrum disorder (for reviews, see references 79). Considering that antidepressants alter serotonin neurotransmission, readily cross the human placenta (1011), and have been shown in a rodent model to bind to the serotonin transporter in the fetal brain (12), it is reasonable to posit a role for fetal antidepressant exposure in the pathogenesis of autism.

Beginning in 2011, a rapidly accruing series of observational studies examining the possible link between autism and prenatal antidepressant exposure produced discrepant findings. Despite these inconsistencies, previous meta-analyses (1315) have reported a significant association between antidepressant exposure and autism, although Brown and colleagues (13) postulated that the association, nonsignificant when limited to women with histories of mental illness, is perhaps a by-product of residual confounding.

These previous meta-analyses have not systematically examined the potential for bias in these studies, nor the contribution for alternative study designs, particularly comparator group selection (which was specifically emphasized by AHRQ) to discriminate the contribution of bias compared with confounding underlying the discordant results. This meta-analysis is the first, to our knowledge, to systematically evaluate the effect of alternative study designs, particularly comparator group selection, on the observed association between prenatal antidepressant exposure and subsequent autism diagnosis.

Methods

We followed the guidelines established by the Meta-analysis of Observational Studies in Epidemiology Group (16).

Search Strategy and Selection Criteria

The senior author (D.J.N.) conducted searches using BIOSIS, CINAHL Plus, Embase, MEDLINE, PsycINFO, PubMed, and Scopus databases from their respective inceptions through August 2017 for articles addressing the association between prenatal antidepressant exposure and autism and autism spectrum disorder diagnoses. The search strategy comprised three initial searches selecting articles regarding antidepressants (search terms: antidepressant, serotonin reuptake inhibitor, serotonin-norepinephrine reuptake inhibitor [SNRI], selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor [SSRI], tricyclic antidepressant, and the generic names for all commercially available antidepressants), pregnancy (search terms: antenatal, fetal, pregnancy, and prenatal), and autism (search terms: Asperger’s syndrome, autism, autism spectrum disorder, and autistic). These three sets were then joined into a single result set using the Boolean AND operator. This process was repeated for each of the seven databases. Finally, the bibliographies of all selected articles and review articles were searched to identify any articles that were overlooked in the database searches.

Peer-reviewed original research articles of controlled studies were selected for inclusion. Articles were excluded if the outcome was operationalized as the presence or severity of symptoms of autism rather than autism diagnosis.

Quality Assessment

Publication quality was assessed using the Newcastle-Ottawa Scale (1718). The authors rated each article independently, and then a consensus rating was assigned. The cohort study version of the Newcastle-Ottawa Scale awards each study up to nine stars across three sections (selection, 4 stars; comparability, 2 stars; and outcome, 3 stars). The case-control version of the scale also awards up to nine stars across three sections (selection, 4 stars; comparability, 2 stars; and exposure, 3 stars). By convention, the two comparability factors of the scale were designated with respect to covariates deemed most important to the analysis in question. In view of the importance placed by the AHRQ on adequately controlling for prenatal depression (5), we elected a priori to designate current maternal depression during pregnancy as a comparability factor. We selected maternal ethnicity and nationality a priori as a comparability factor, knowing that ethnicity and nationality (i.e., country of origin) have previously been implicated as sources of autism diagnosis misclassification (1922) and having observed during our preliminary review that the majority of the qualifying studies reported statistically significant between-group differences in maternal ethnicity and nationality.

A Newcastle-Ottawa Scale quality threshold developed for the AHRQ (23) was used to rate the quality of each study as good, fair, or poor. To be assigned a good rating, a study must have three or more stars in the selection domain, one or more stars in the comparability domain, and two or more stars in the outcome and exposure domain. Studies rated as fair must have two stars in the selection domain, one or more stars in the comparability domain, and two or more stars in the outcome and exposure domain. Finally, studies rated as poor have ≤1 star in the selection domain or zero stars in the comparability domain or ≤1 star in the outcome and exposure domain.

Meta-Analysis

Analyses were performed using the Comprehensive Meta-Analysis software program, version 3.3 (BioStat, Frederick, Md.). All statistical tests were two-tailed with alpha set at 0.05. A meta-analysis using a random-effects model was performed with summary measures of effect presented as odds ratios or hazard ratios, the latter for time-to-event data, with 95% confidence intervals. Because random-effects modeling accommodates analysis of studies drawn from different populations, it was used (in lieu of fixed-effects modeling) in light of our hypothesis that the varied approaches to comparator group operationalization alter effect estimates (2425). The most fully adjusted odds ratio or hazard ratio estimates reported in each study were used in the meta-analysis. Under the rare disease assumption (26), risk estimates from case-control and cohort studies were combined when calculating summary odds ratio and hazard ratio estimates.

In several cases, findings from the same, or overlapping, patient samples were reported in two or more studies (Table 1). Specifically, four studies used data from the Swedish Medical Birth Register (2730), three from the Danish Registry (3133), and two from the Partners Healthcare database (3435). To avoid data redundancy in the meta-analyses, preliminary random-effects meta-analyses were performed to produce a single pooled odds ratio or hazard ratio estimate for each set of overlapping studies, and the pooled estimates were incorporated into the final meta-analyses.

TABLE 1. Description of studies included in the meta-analysisa

Study CharacteristicsData SourceAntidepressant ClassTrimesterComparison GroupNewcastle-Ottawa ScalebEthnicity Differences in Outcome or Exposure
Study, Design, and GroupNOutcomeCountry (State)DatabaseBirth YearSSRISNRIAnyFirstSecond or ThirdAnyPopulationPsychiatricDiscordantSelectionComparabilityExposure/OutcomeStudy Quality
Croen et al. (40)Odds ratioUnited States (Calif.)Kaiser Permanente Northern California1995–1999YesYesYesYesYesYesYesOne starOne starTwo starsPoorAutism diagnosis: fewer Hispanic participants
 Case-control design
  Case subjects298
  Control subjects1,507
Hviid et al. (31)Hazard ratioDenmarkDanish Medical Birth Registry1996–2005YesYesYesTwo starsOne starThree starsFairAntidepressant exposure: fewer immigrant participants
 Cohort design
  Exposed subjects6,068
  Unexposed subjects620,807
Rai et al. (27)Odds ratioSwedenStockholm Youth Cohort2001–2007YesYesYesYesYesTwo starsOne starTwo starsFairAutism diagnosis: fewer immigrant participants
 Case-control design
  Case subjects4,429
  Control subjects43,277
Sørensen et al. (32)Hazard ratioDenmarkDanish Civil Registration System1996–2006YesYesYesYesYesYesYesYesThree starsThree starsPoorNot reported
 Cohort design
  Exposed subjects8,833
  Unexposed subjects646,782
Gidaya et al. (32)Odds ratioDenmarkDanish Civil Registration System1997–2006YesYesYesYesYesTwo starsTwo starsPoorNot reported
 Case-control design
  Case subjects5,215
  Control subjects52,150
Harrington et al. (38)
 Case-control designOdds ratioUnited States (Calif.)Childhood Autism Risks from the Genetics and Environment Study2003–2010YesYesYesYesYesYesThree starsOne starTwo starsGoodAutism diagnosis: more immigrant participants
  Case subjects492
  Control subjects320
Clements et al. (34)Odds ratioUnited States (Mass.)Partners HealthCare Electronic Health Record1997–2010YesYesYesYesYesYesYesOne starOne starTwo starsPoorNo differences
 Case-control design
  Case subjects1,377
  Control subjects4,022
Boukhris et al. (41)Hazard ratioCanadaQuebec Pregnancy/Children Cohort1998–2009YesYesYesYesYesTwo starsTwo starsPoorNot reported
 Cohort design
  Exposed subjects4,724
  Unexposed subjects140,732
Castro et al. (35)Odds ratioUnited States (Mass.)Partners HealthCare, Beth Israel, and Boston Children’s Hospital1997–2010YesYesYesYesYesOne starOne starTwo starsPoorNo differences
 Case-control design
  Case subjects1,245
  Control subjects3,405
Malm et al. (39)Hazard ratioFinlandFinnish Medical Birth Register1996–2010YesYesYesYesThree starsOne starThree starsGoodAntidepressant exposure: fewer immigrant participants
 Cohort design
  Exposed subjects15,729
  Unexposed psychiatric control subjects9,651
  Unexposed healthy volunteers31,394
Brown et al. (37)Hazard ratioCanadaOntario Health Administrative Data2002–2010YesYesYesYesYesYesYesYesYesThree starsThree starsPoorNot reported
 Cohort design
  Exposed subjects2,837
  Unexposed subjects33,069
Rai et al. (28)Odds ratioSwedenStockholm Youth Cohort2001–2011cYesYesYesYesThree starsOne starThree starsGoodAntidepressant exposure: fewer immigrant participants
 Cohort design
  Exposed subjects3,342
  Unexposed psychiatric control subjects12,325
  Unexposed healthy volunteers238,943
Sujan et al. (29)Hazard ratioSwedenSwedish Medical Birth Register1996–2012YesYesYesYesYesYesYesThree starsOne starThree starsGoodAntidepressant exposure: fewer immigrant participants
 Cohort design
  Exposed subjects22,544
  Unexposed1,558,085
Viktorin et al. (30)Odds ratioSwedenSwedish Medical Birth Register2006–2007YesYesYesYesYesYesThree starsThree starsPoorNot reported
 Cohort design
  Exposed subjects3,982
  Unexposed subjects172,646

aExposed subjects were those who were exposed to antidepressants, and unexposed subjects were those who had no exposure to antidepressants; SNRI=serotonin-norepinephrine reuptake inhibitor; SSRI=selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor.

bUsing Newcastle-Ottawa Scale thresholds developed for the Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality, a rating of good requires ≥3 selection stars, ≥1 comparability stars, and ≥2 exposure and outcome stars; a rating of fair requires two selection stars, ≥1 comparability stars, and ≥2 exposure and outcome stars; and a rating of poor requires ≤1 selection stars or zero comparability stars or ≤1 exposure and outcome star.

cThe participant was living in (not necessarily born in) Stockholm county in 2001–2011.

TABLE 1. Description of studies included in the meta-analysisa

Enlarge table

Results of meta-analyses were grouped by antidepressant class and trimester of exposure (first, second, third, any). Because the window of risk for adverse neurodevelopmental effects of fetal antidepressant exposure is unclear, data from all trimesters were evaluated. Initial meta-analyses were performed for studies reporting results using population-based comparator groups. Analyses were then performed for psychiatric control (i.e., control mothers limited to those with histories of depression) comparator group results and family-based (i.e., siblings discordant for prenatal antidepressant exposure or autism diagnosis) comparator group results. Finally, heterogeneity testing was performed to evaluate differences between results yielded with the three comparator group definitions.

Meta-Regression

Post hoc meta-regression was performed to investigate whether comparability factors used in the assessment of study quality (prenatal depression and maternal ethnicity and nationality) and other study characteristics (study design, study location, and publication year) were sources of heterogeneity in the population-based studies. Incremental insertion of candidate moderators into the regression model was performed.

Publication Bias

Potential publication bias was explored by constructing funnel plots and performing the Egger funnel plot asymmetry test (linear regression method) (36). Standard error of the exposure effect was used as the measure of study precision.

Results

Search Results

The database search returned 499 entries. After exclusion of 260 duplicate entries and 225 articles that did not fulfill inclusion and exclusion criteria, 14 qualifying studies were selected for inclusion (Table 1; see also Figure S2 in the online supplement). The 14 included studies comprised eight cohort studies (six reported hazard ratios, and two reported odds ratios) and six case-control studies (all reporting odds ratios) encompassing 3,650,230 children. Raw data were extracted from hazard ratio-reporting studies and used to calculate odds ratios that were in turn incorporated into odds ratio meta-analyses. In 13 of the 14 studies, results from analyses of a population-based comparator group were reported. In three studies, results from analyses of psychiatric comparator groups were reported; of these three studies, two used discordant-sibling comparator groups, and two used both psychiatric and discordant-sibling comparator groups. In the family-based analyses, siblings discordant for prenatal antidepressant exposure (39, 32, 37) or autism diagnosis (28) were paired.

Quality Assessment

Under the AHRQ criteria (23), the overall study quality was disappointing, because the majority of studies (N=8/14) were rated poor, two were rated fair, and only four were rated good (Table 1). Within the selection domain, quality was more reassuring, with seven studies achieving good quality (3–4 stars) (3830, 32, 3739) and three studies rated poor (0–1 stars) (3435, 40). In the exposure and outcome domain, quality ratings were better, with all 14 studies fulfilling the 2- to 3-star threshold for good quality. However, this favorable finding was tempered by the recognition that in only one of the 14 studies were all children examined by the investigative team, using the Autism Diagnostic Interview–Revised and the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule in an effort to minimize misclassification of autism diagnosis (38). Each of the remaining 13 studies operationalized autism diagnosis as the presence of an ICD-9 or ICD-10 diagnostic code for autism spectrum disorder in the source database.

Study quality was especially lacking within the comparability domain. In fact, none of the 14 studies reliably identified whether participants had current or active depression during pregnancy, relying instead on lifetime diagnosis of depression as evidenced by an ICD-8, ICD-9, or ICD-10 diagnostic code for a history of depression. Five studies were rated poor in the comparability domain (zero stars) because maternal ethnicity and nationality were not reported (30, 3233, 37, 41). In addition, there was cause for concern even among the nine studies that did document participant ethnicity, because seven reported group disparities in maternal ethnicity. Ethnic minority representation was significantly lower among participants with an autism diagnosis or antidepressant exposure in six of the seven studies (2729, 31, 3940) and higher in only one study (38) (Table 1).

Meta-Analyses

Because of the paucity of studies reporting psychiatric and discordant-sibling comparator group data for SSRI exposure or any antidepressant exposure in the second and third trimesters (see Table S1 in the online supplement) and SNRI exposure in any trimester, comparator group effects could not be adequately examined within these strata. Therefore, examination of comparator group effects was limited to first-trimester exposure or any trimester exposure to SSRIs and any antidepressants (Table 2 and Figures 1 and 2; see also Figures S3 and S4 in the online supplement).

TABLE 2. Results of meta-analyses of autism risk after antidepressant exposure during pregnancy and the first trimestera

Comparator Subgroup
PopulationPsychiatricDiscordant SiblingHeterogeneity Between Comparator Subgroup Analyses
Antidepressant ClassOdds/Hazard Ratio95% CINZpOdds/Hazard Ratio95% CINZpOdds/Hazard Ratio95% CINZpQdfp
Antidepressant exposure during any trimester
 Hazard ratio
  Any1.421.18–1.7063.74<0.0011.140.84–1.5330.840.4000.970.68–1.373–0.200.8454.2720.118
  SSRI1.521.29–1.8054.92<0.0011.160.88–1.5131.040.2970.830.58–1.182–1.050.29210.3820.006
 Odds ratio
  Any1.581.25–1.99113.79<0.0011.240.93–1.6661.470.1420.850.54–1.354–0.680.4965.8020.055
  SSRI1.521.16–2.0083.020.0021.260.89–1.7941.320.1880.760.50–1.162–1.290.1997.3720.025
Antidepressant exposure during first trimester
 Hazard ratio
  Any1.401.15–1.7153.310.0011.460.93–2.3021.640.1000.830.55–1.261–0.880.3785.3220.070
  SSRI1.611.45–1.7938.91<0.0011.400.78–2.5211.120.2620.810.58–1.141–1.220.22214.5820.001
 Odds ratio
  Any1.621.30–2.0174.30<0.0011.571.04–2.3622.140.0330.620.40–0.961–2.170.03015.532<0.001
  SSRI1.811.45–2.2655.19<0.0011.700.64–4.5511.060.2910.830.58–1.191–1.030.30513.2420.001

aSSRI=selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor.

TABLE 2. Results of meta-analyses of autism risk after antidepressant exposure during pregnancy and the first trimestera

Enlarge table
FIGURE 1.

FIGURE 1. Forest plots of autism risk after antidepressant exposure during pregnancy, stratified by comparison group definition

a Single pooled estimate was calculated for studies from the same data source.

FIGURE 2.

FIGURE 2. Forest plots of autism risk after SSRI exposure during pregnancy, stratified by comparison group definition

a Single pooled estimate was calculated for studies from the same data source.

Initial meta-analyses were conducted using studies reporting population-based comparator group data. Consistent with previous meta-analyses (1315), all eight summary hazard ratio and odds ratio estimates derived from population-based comparator studies uniformly demonstrated highly significant positive associations for autism diagnosis with SSRI or any antidepressant exposure during the first trimester or any trimester (Table 2). Summary hazard ratio estimates were in the range of 1.40–1.42 for any antidepressants and 1.52–1.61 for SSRIs, and summary odds ratio estimates were in the range of 1.58–1.62 for any antidepressants and 1.52–1.81 for SSRIs. It is noteworthy that the highest hazard ratio and odds ratio estimates were attributed to two studies with significant participant group ethnic disparities (Figures 1 and 2; see also Figures S3 and S4 in the online supplement) (29, 40).

Whereas all eight population-based summary hazard ratio and odds ratio estimates achieved statistical significance (Table 2), only one of eight summary effect estimates derived from psychiatric control comparator studies was statistically significant (the summary odds ratio estimate for any antidepressant exposure during the first trimester was 1.57 [95% CI=1.04, 2.36]). Not only were the remainder of the psychiatric control summary estimates nonsignificant, they were consistently lower than population-based summary estimates. The range of psychiatric control summary hazard ratio estimates was 1.14–1.46 for any antidepressants and 1.16–1.40 for SSRIs, and the range of summary odds ratio estimates was 1.24–1.57 for any antidepressants and 1.26–1.70 for SSRIs.

Analysis of discordant-sibling comparator studies produced findings even more markedly distinct from those of population-based comparator group studies (Table 2). All summary odds ratio and hazard ratio effect estimates derived from discordant-sibling studies were <1.0, ranging from 0.62 to 0.97. In fact, discordant-sibling comparison of first-trimester exposure to any antidepressant produced a statistically significant summary odds ratio estimate of 0.62 (95% CI=0.40, 0.96), indicating a possible protective benefit of antidepressant therapy.

Between-group heterogeneity testing (Table 2) demonstrated that the five of eight sets of summary hazard ratio and odds ratio estimates produced using population, psychiatric, and discordant-sibling comparator groups were significantly different (p values of <0.001, 0.001, 0.001, 0.006, and 0.025), with two additional sets that fell short of statistical significance (p values of 0.055 and 0.070).

Results of analysis of the effects of second-trimester and third-trimester exposure to SSRIs or any antidepressants are presented in Table S1 in the online supplement. As previously noted, the paucity of psychiatric control and discordant-sibling comparator studies reporting second- and third-trimester exposure data did not provide sufficient data to meaningfully assess the effect of comparator group definition on outcome.

Meta-Regression

Meta-regression was conducted to investigate whether comparability factors chosen for the meta-analysis (prenatal depression and maternal ethnicity and nationality), and other study characteristics (study design, location, and publication year) remained sources of heterogeneity in the population-based analyses despite statistical adjustment for potential confounding in the contributing studies. Because none of the 14 studies reliably documented the presence of active prenatal depression, this comparability factor could not be subjected to meta-regression. Maternal ethnicity was operationalized as a categorical moderator (no group ethnicity difference compared with significant group ethnicity difference) in the meta-regression with no ethnicity difference designated as the reference condition. Study design (cohort compared with case-control) and study location (Europe compared with North America) were also operationalized as categorical moderators. Publication year was defined as a continuous moderator. Meta-regression of population-based studies of prenatal exposure to any antidepressant demonstrated that despite efforts to adjust for confounding, maternal ethnicity differences remained a source of moderate (42) heterogeneity (I2=30%; Q=5.97, df=1, p<0.02), with the log odds ratio equaling 0.018 among studies with no group ethnicity difference compared with 0.484 among studies with significant group ethnicity differences (Figure 3). None of the other candidate moderators were significant predictors of study heterogeneity (see Table S2 in the online supplement). Meta-regression could not be performed for antidepressant studies using psychiatric or discordant-sibling controls or any SSRI studies because of the paucity of studies without ethnicity differences to serve as the reference condition.

FIGURE 3.

FIGURE 3. Regression of maternal ethnicity differences on summary odds ratio in population-based studies of autism risk following antidepressant exposure during pregnancya

a Model: Q=5.97, df=1, p<0.02, I2=30%; goodness of fit: Q=2.52, df=5, p=0.77.

Publication Bias

Funnel plots were constructed for studies of autism associated with prenatal exposure to any antidepressants and SSRIs during any trimester. These groupings were selected in order to maximize the number of studies included and thus the statistical power of the analysis.

For studies of any antidepressant exposure, results of Egger’s asymmetry test were not indicative of publication bias (intercept=0.67, t=0.56, p=0.58). Similarly, Egger’s asymmetry test results for studies of SSRI exposure were not indicative of publication bias (intercept=0.50, t=0.49, p=0.63). Visual inspection of the funnel plots for studies of any antidepressant exposure (see Figure S5 in the online supplement) and of SSRI exposure (see Figure S6 in the online supplement) revealed symmetric distributions but with additional horizontal scatter. The horizontal scatter was likely a consequence not of publication bias but of the study heterogeneity identified in the meta-regression.

Discussion

In this meta-analysis, we demonstrated a marked effect of comparator group composition on observational studies of prenatal antidepressant exposure and autism. First, most of the analyses of between-comparator subgroup heterogeneity (Table 2) demonstrated highly significant differences. Moreover, whereas summary effect estimates derived from population-based comparator studies uniformly implicated fetal antidepressant exposure in the pathogenesis of autism, psychiatric control and discordant-sibling comparator studies, with largely nonsignificant and progressively lower summary estimates, indicate otherwise (Table 2).

Additionally, our finding of significant between-group heterogeneity indicates that meta-analyses of prenatal antidepressant exposure and autism risk should be limited to studies using the same comparator group definition, because combining studies using different comparison groups will likely produce unreliable effect estimates. Thus, we are led to question which comparator group definition is preferred. Results of the meta-regression of population-based studies (Figure 3; see also Table S2 in the online supplement), a test of within-group heterogeneity, indicate that the effect estimates provided by the meta-analyses using population-based studies are unreliable (43) because of unresolved differences in maternal ethnicity. Unfortunately, existing studies did not permit meta-regression of psychiatric control and discordant-sibling studies. Nevertheless, if the AHRQ position (i.e., that studies employing comparator groups comprising depressed women are better equipped to elucidate the risks of antenatal antidepressant therapy [5]) is accepted, then the summary estimates from psychiatric control and discordant-sibling studies, which do not support an association between prenatal antidepressant exposure and autism, are preferred. In summary, our meta-analysis does not support an association between prenatal antidepressant exposure and autism.

The AHRQ design recommendation is predicated on a conviction that control subjects with depression are necessary to disentangle the effect of depression itself from that of antidepressant exposure. Numerous adverse outcomes have, in fact, been attributed to both prenatal depression and prenatal antidepressant therapy, including preterm birth, miscarriage, low birth weight, gestational hypertension and preeclampsia, child motor and cognitive deficits, and a variety of offspring behavioral and emotional perturbations (4445), including autism (46). Additionally, prospective studies concomitantly controlling for the occurrence of both depression and antidepressant exposure during gestation have successfully discriminated the adverse effects of prenatal depression (47) from those of prenatal antidepressant exposure (48), underscoring the importance of appropriate control for maternal depression.

However, none of the 14 studies included in our meta-analysis reliably ascertained whether participants experienced an acute episode of depression during the index pregnancy, relying instead on a level of ICD diagnostic coding that only delineates lifetime diagnoses of depression. In planning our analyses, we elected a priori that adequate control for an episodic disorder such as depression necessitates determining whether a depressive episode occurred during pregnancy. How, then, in the absence of adequate control for prenatal depression with any comparator group design, are we to understand the decidedly lower summary estimates derived from psychiatric control and discordant-sibling studies? The answer must lie elsewhere. If, as has been suggested, a genetic relationship exists between maternal depression and autism (4950), then perhaps controlling for depression as a lifetime trait variable is adequate. Alternatively, because any lifetime history of depression is a risk factor for recurrence of depression during pregnancy (51), both psychiatric control and discordant-sibling comparisons may have afforded at least partial control for acute prenatal depression.

Factors other than adjustment for maternal depression, however, likely explain our finding that summary hazard ratio and odds ratio estimates from discordant-sibling comparisons are decidedly lower than both population-based and psychiatric control designs. For example, discordant-sibling comparisons afford better control for genetic susceptibility to autism (52), which is especially important in view of the recently reported 83% heritability (53). Perhaps more importantly, significant differences in ethnicity in half of the studies (2729, 31, 3840), coupled with failure to document ethnicity in five more studies (30, 3233, 37, 41), indicate an additional advantage of the discordant-sibling design. With one exception (38), the specific ethnic differences reported were that the prevalence of autism was significantly lower among children of Hispanic or immigrant mothers (27, 40), and the prevalence of antidepressant exposure was significantly lower among immigrant mothers (2829, 31, 39). Such differences should not be surprising. Ethnic disparities in clinical recognition of autism are well established, with U.S. studies consistently reporting underrecognition of autism in Latino children (1922) and other studies specifically denoting poor English proficiency as a principal barrier to autism diagnosis among Latinos in the United States (21, 54). In contrast to a recent review of 17 studies suggesting a higher prevalence for autism diagnoses among children of immigrant mothers in Europe (55), autism diagnoses were significantly lower among children of immigrant mothers in the only European case-control study in our meta-analysis to report maternal ethnicity (27). Similarly, immigrant status and poor language proficiency have been identified as barriers to access to mental health care in several countries (5660). Taking these data together, we may surmise that ethnic minority and immigrant mothers in the contributing studies, particularly those with poor language proficiency, were less likely to have access both to treatment for depression during pregnancy and to a diagnostic evaluation for their children exhibiting symptoms of autism. Consequently, the observed association between prenatal antidepressant exposure and autism in population-based comparisons is unlikely to denote a causal relationship. In addition, it is also unlikely to be a consequence of residual confounding as proposed by Brown and colleagues (13). Instead, the most plausible explanation is that the association is the product of a surveillance bias, arising because women in prenatal antidepressant exposure groups are more likely to secure a diagnostic evaluation for autism for their children. Moreover, reports linking maternal depression with autism (61) in offspring may lead to enhanced autism screening, thereby constituting another potential source for surveillance bias. Such biases are not amenable to statistical adjustment but can be addressed by designating a comparator group (such as a discordant-sibling control) with a similar likelihood of screening for the outcome of interest (62).

Limitations of this meta-analysis are those imposed by the 14 contributing studies. As previously noted, the available studies did not permit an analysis of the effect of research design on results of SNRI exposure or second- and third-trimester exposure to other antidepressants. Moreover, the studies did not afford sufficient control of the confounding effect of maternal depression, although it appears that some measure of control for depression may have been provided by psychiatric control and discordant-sibling designs. In addition, the studies did not reliably provide the data necessary to control adequately for concomitant prenatal pharmacological exposures, with only five studies controlling for exposure to other psychotropic drug classes (28, 3031, 3839) and five studies controlling for tobacco exposure (2728, 31, 3839).

The design implications of this meta-analysis for future observational studies using data derived from large-scale national registries and health care databases are far-reaching. While it is easy to be impressed by the voluminous sample sizes they produce, their data are primarily collected to address clinical and business demands, not to answer research questions. Thus, such databases are especially susceptible to the sources of bias known to hinder all observational designs. Whereas the value of family-based designs in genetic association studies has long been recognized, our study highlights an additional strength of a family-based design, namely, holding constant not only genetic but also family-level environmental variables, thereby minimizing their potential for surveillance bias and residual confounding (52). Even psychiatric control designs fail to achieve this level of rigor. These results lead us to recommend that pharmacovigilance reports of large-scale registry and database data more carefully consider sources of bias, particularly surveillance bias, and that they accordingly consider incorporating alternative designs, such as family-based discordant-sibling designs, in lieu of conventional population-based comparisons to more effectively address the potential for surveillance bias.

Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, University of Miami Miller School of Medicine, Miami (Vega, Bozhdaraj, Saltz); Department of Psychology, University of South Florida St. Petersburg (G.C. Newport); Department of Psychiatry (Nemeroff, D.J. Newport), University of Texas at Austin Dell Medical School, Austin.

Presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Psychiatric Association, New York, May 5–9, 2018

Send correspondence to Dr. Newport ().

Dr. Nemeroff has received research grant support from NIH; he has served on scientific advisory boards for the American Foundation for Suicide Prevention (AFSP), the Anxiety and Depression Association of America (ADAA), the Brain and Behavior Research Foundation, Skyland Trail, and Xhale; he has served on the board of directors for AFSP, ADAA, and Gratitude America; he has served as a consultant to Bracket, Janssen, Intra-Cellular Therapies, Magstim, Navitor, SK Pharma, Sunovion, Taisho, Takeda, TC-MSO, and Xhale; he is a shareholder in Abbure, Antares, Calgene, Corcept, EMA Wellness, Seattle Genetics, TC-MSO, Trends in Pharma Development, and Xhale; he has income sources or equity of $10,000 from American Psychiatric Association Publishing, Bracket, Intra-Cellular Therapies, and TC-MSO; and he holds patents for a method and devices for transdermal delivery of lithium (U.S. patent number 6,375,990B1) and for a method of assessing antidepressant drug therapy via transport inhibition of monoamine neurotransmitters by ex vivo assay (U.S. patent number 7,148,027B2). Dr. D.J. Newport has received research grant support from Eli Lilly, GlaxoSmithKline, Janssen, the National Alliance for Research on Schizophrenia and Depression, NIH, Sage Therapeutics, Takeda Pharmaceuticals, the Texas Health and Human Services Commission, and Wyeth; he has served on speakers bureaus for AstraZeneca, Eli Lilly, GlaxoSmithKline, Pfizer, and Wyeth; and he has served on the advisory board of GlaxoSmithKline. The other authors report no financial relationships with commercial interests.

References

1 Newport DJ, Stowe ZN: Psychopharmacology during pregnancy and lactation, in Essentials of Clinical Psychopharmacology, 3rd ed. Edited by Schatzberg A, Nemeroff CB. Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Publishing, 2013, pp 751–780Google Scholar

2 Koren G, Nickel S: Sources of bias in signals of pharmaceutical safety in pregnancy. Clin Invest Med 2010; 33:E349–E355Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

3 Ornoy A, Koren G: Selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors in human pregnancy: on the way to resolving the controversy. Semin Fetal Neonatal Med 2014; 19:188–194Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

4 Newport DJ, Brennan PA, Green P, et al.: Maternal depression and medication exposure during pregnancy: comparison of maternal retrospective recall to prospective documentation. BJOG 2008; 115:681–688Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

5 McDonagh M, Matthews A, Phillipi C, et al: Antidepressant Treatment of Depression During Pregnancy and the Postpartum Period. Evidence Report/Technology Assessment No. 216. Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality Publication No. 14-E003-EF. Rockville, Md, Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality, 2014. www.effectivehealthcare.ahrq.gov/reports/final.cfmGoogle Scholar

6 Brummelte S, Mc Glanaghy E, Bonnin A, et al.: Developmental changes in serotonin signaling: implications for early brain function, behavior and adaptation. Neuroscience 2017; 342:212–231Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

7 Fernández M, Mollinedo-Gajate I, Peñagarikano O: Neural circuits for social cognition: implications for autism. Neuroscience 2018; 370:148–162Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

8 Harrington RA, Lee LC, Crum RM, et al.: Serotonin hypothesis of autism: implications for selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor use during pregnancy. Autism Res 2013; 6:149–168Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

9 Muller CL, Anacker AMJ, Veenstra-VanderWeele J: The serotonin system in autism spectrum disorder: from biomarker to animal models. Neuroscience 2016; 321:24–41Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

10 Hendrick V, Stowe ZN, Altshuler LL, et al.: Placental passage of antidepressant medications. Am J Psychiatry 2003; 160:993–996LinkGoogle Scholar

11 Loughhead AM, Stowe ZN, Newport DJ, et al.: Placental passage of tricyclic antidepressants. Biol Psychiatry 2006; 59:287–290Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

12 Capello CF, Bourke CH, Ritchie JC, et al.: Serotonin transporter occupancy in rats exposed to serotonin reuptake inhibitors in utero or via breast milk. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 2011; 339:275–285Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

13 Brown HK, Hussain-Shamsy N, Lunsky Y, et al.: The association between antenatal exposure to selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors and autism: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Clin Psychiatry 2017; 78:e48–e58Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

14 Man KK, Tong HH, Wong LY, et al.: Exposure to selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors during pregnancy and risk of autism spectrum disorder in children: a systematic review and meta-analysis of observational studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2015; 49:82–89Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

15 Mezzacappa A, Lasica PA, Gianfagna F, et al.: Risk for autism spectrum disorders according to period of prenatal antidepressant exposure: a systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Pediatr 2017; 171:555–563Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

16 Stroup DF, Berlin JA, Morton SC, et al.: Meta-analysis of observational studies in epidemiology: a proposal for reporting: Meta-analysis of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (MOOSE) group. JAMA 2000; 283:2008–2012Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

17 Stang A: Critical evaluation of the Newcastle-Ottawa Scale for the assessment of the quality of nonrandomized studies in meta-analyses. Eur J Epidemiol 2010; 25:603–605Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

18 Wells GA, Shea B, O’Connell D, et al: The Newcastle-Ottawa Scale (NOS) for assessing the quality of nonrandomized studies in meta-analyses, 2013. http://www.ohri.ca/programs/clinical_epidemiology/oxford.aspGoogle Scholar

19 Durkin MS, Maenner MJ, Baio J, et al.: Autism spectrum disorder among US children (2002–2010): socioeconomic, racial and ethnic disparities. Am J Public Health 2017; 107:1818–1826Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

20 Zaroff CM, Uhm SY: Prevalence of autism spectrum disorders and influence of country of measurement and ethnicity. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 2012; 47:395–398Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

21 Zuckerman KE, Lindly OJ, Reyes NM, et al.: Disparities in diagnosis and treatment of autism in Latino and non-Latino white families. Pediatrics 2017; 139:e20163010Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

22 Imm P, White T, Durkin MS: Assessment of racial and ethnic bias in autism spectrum disorder prevalence estimates from a US surveillance system. Autism 2019; 23:1927–1935Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

23 Penson DF, Krishnaswami S, Jules A, et al: Evaluation and treatment of cryptorchidism, in AHRQ Comparative Effectiveness Reviews. Dec Report No. 13-EHC001-EF. Rockville, Md, Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality, 2012Google Scholar

24 Nikolakopoulou A, Mavridis D, Salanti G: Demystifying fixed and random effects meta-analysis. Evid Based Ment Health 2014; 17:53–57Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

25 Borenstein M, Hedges LV, Higgins JPT, et al.: A basic introduction to fixed-effect and random-effects models for meta-analysis. Res Synth Methods 2010; 1:97–111Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

26 Greenland S, Thomas DC: On the need for the rare disease assumption in case-control studies. Am J Epidemiol 1982; 116:547–553Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

27 Rai D, Lee BK, Dalman C, et al.: Parental depression, maternal antidepressant use during pregnancy, and risk of autism spectrum disorders: population based case-control study. BMJ 2013; 346:f2059Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

28 Rai D, Lee BK, Dalman C, et al.: Antidepressants during pregnancy and autism in offspring: population based cohort study. BMJ 2017; 358:j2811Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

29 Sujan AC, Rickert ME, Öberg AS, et al.: Associations of maternal antidepressant use during the first trimester of pregnancy with preterm birth, small for gestational age, autism spectrum disorder, and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in offspring. JAMA 2017; 317:1553–1562Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

30 Viktorin A, Uher R, Reichenberg A, et al.: Autism risk following antidepressant medication during pregnancy. Psychol Med 2017; 47:2787–2796Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

31 Hviid A, Melbye M, Pasternak B: Use of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors during pregnancy and risk of autism. N Engl J Med 2013; 369:2406–2415Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

32 Sørensen MJ, Grønborg TK, Christensen J, et al.: Antidepressant exposure in pregnancy and risk of autism spectrum disorders. Clin Epidemiol 2013; 5:449–459Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

33 Gidaya NB, Lee BK, Burstyn I, et al.: In utero exposure to selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors and risk for autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord 2014; 44:2558–2567Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

34 Clements CC, Castro VM, Blumenthal SR, et al.: Prenatal antidepressant exposure is associated with risk for attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder but not autism spectrum disorder in a large health system. Mol Psychiatry 2015; 20:727–734Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

35 Castro VM, Kong SW, Clements CC, et al.: Absence of evidence for increase in risk for autism or attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder following antidepressant exposure during pregnancy: a replication study. Transl Psychiatry 2016; 6:e708Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

36 Egger M, Davey Smith G, Schneider M, et al.: Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. BMJ 1997; 315:629–634Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

37 Brown HK, Ray JG, Wilton AS, et al.: Association between serotonergic antidepressant use during pregnancy and autism spectrum disorder in children. JAMA 2017; 317:1544–1552Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

38 Harrington RA, Lee LC, Crum RM, et al.: Prenatal SSRI use and offspring with autism spectrum disorder or developmental delay. Pediatrics 2014; 133:e1241–e1248Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

39 Malm H, Brown AS, Gissler M, et al.: Gestational exposure to selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors and offspring psychiatric disorders: a national register-based study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2016; 55:359–366Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

40 Croen LA, Grether JK, Yoshida CK, et al.: Antidepressant use during pregnancy and childhood autism spectrum disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2011; 68:1104–1112Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

41 Boukhris T, Sheehy O, Mottron L, et al.: Antidepressant use during pregnancy and the risk of autism spectrum disorder in children. JAMA Pediatr 2016; 170:117–124Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

42 Deeks JJ, Higgins JPT, Altman DG: Analysing data and undertaking meta-analyses, in Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions, Version 5.1.0. Edited by Higgins JPT, Green S. London, The Cochrane Collaboration, 2011. www.handbook.cochrane.orgGoogle Scholar

43 O’Rourke K, Detsky AS: Meta-analysis in medical research: strong encouragement for higher quality in individual research efforts. J Clin Epidemiol 1989; 42:1021–1024Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

44 Henry AL, Beach AJ, Stowe ZN, et al.: The fetus and maternal depression: implications for antenatal treatment guidelines. Clin Obstet Gynecol 2004; 47:535–546Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

45 Jarde A, Morais M, Kingston D, et al.: Neonatal outcomes in women with untreated antenatal depression compared with women without depression: a systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry 2016; 73:826–837Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

46 Oberlander TF, Zwaigenbaum L: Disentangling maternal depression and antidepressant use during pregnancy as risks for autism in children. JAMA 2017; 317:1533–1534Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

47 Nulman I, Rovet J, Stewart DE, et al.: Child development following exposure to tricyclic antidepressants or fluoxetine throughout fetal life: a prospective, controlled study. Am J Psychiatry 2002; 159:1889–1895LinkGoogle Scholar

48 Newport DJ, Hostetter AL, Juul SH, et al.: Prenatal psychostimulant and antidepressant exposure and risk of hypertensive disorders of pregnancy. J Clin Psychiatry 2016; 77:1538–1545Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

49 Smalley SL, McCracken J, Tanguay P: Autism, affective disorders, and social phobia. Am J Med Genet 1995; 60:19–26Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

50 Yirmiya N, Shaked M: Psychiatric disorders in parents of children with autism: a meta-analysis. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2005; 46:69–83Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

51 Lancaster CA, Gold KJ, Flynn HA, et al.: Risk factors for depressive symptoms during pregnancy: a systematic review. Am J Obstet Gynecol 2010; 202:5–14Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

52 Gong T, Brew B, Sjölander A, et al.: Towards non-conventional methods of designing register-based epidemiological studies: an application to pediatric research. Scand J Public Health 2017; 45:30–35Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

53 Sandin S, Lichtenstein P, Kuja-Halkola R, et al.: The heritability of autism spectrum disorder. JAMA 2017; 318:1182–1184Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

54 St Amant HG, Schrager SM, Peña-Ricardo C, et al.: Language barriers impact access to services for children with autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord 2018; 48:333–340Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

55 Kawa R, Saemundsen E, Lóa Jónsdóttir S, et al.: European studies on prevalence and risk of autism spectrum disorders according to immigrant status-a review. Eur J Public Health 2017; 27:101–110MedlineGoogle Scholar

56 Bauer AM, Chen CN, Alegría M: English language proficiency and mental health service use among Latino and Asian Americans with mental disorders. Med Care 2010; 48:1097–1104Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

57 Brendler-Lindqvist M, Norredam M, Hjern A: Duration of residence and psychotropic drug use in recently settled refugees in Sweden: a register-based study. Int J Equity Health 2014; 13:122Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

58 Derr AS: Mental health service use among immigrants in the United States: a systematic review. Psychiatr Serv 2016; 67:265–274LinkGoogle Scholar

59 Fassaert T, Peen J, van Straten A, et al.: Ethnic differences and similarities in outpatient treatment for depression in the Netherlands. Psychiatr Serv 2010; 61:690–697LinkGoogle Scholar

60 Sentell T, Shumway M, Snowden L: Access to mental health treatment by English language proficiency and race/ethnicity. J Gen Intern Med 2007; 22(Suppl 2):289–293Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

61 Ayano G, Maravilla JC, Alati R: Risk of autistic spectrum disorder in offspring with parental mood disorders: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Affect Disord 2019; 248:185–197Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

62 Haut ER, Pronovost PJ: Surveillance bias in outcomes reporting. JAMA 2011; 305:2462–2463Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar