The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×

Abstract

OBJECTIVE: This study examined the utility of baseline psychomotor speed, measured with neuropsychological tests, to predict fluoxetine response in moderately depressed outpatients. The authors hypothesized that since psychomotor slowing in depressed patients has been linked to reduced dopaminergic neurotransmission, patients with slowing would be unresponsive to fluoxetine, a selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor. METHOD: After baseline neuropsychological testing, patients were treated openly with fluoxetine for 12 weeks. Thirty-seven patients completed the trial. RESULTS: Compared to the 25 patients who responded, the 12 patients who did not respond to fluoxetine exhibited significantly poorer performance in verbal fluency on the Controlled Oral Word Association Test FAS and in color naming on the Stroop Color and Word Test. In addition, the nonresponders tended to perform worse than the responders on the Stroop Color and Word Test reading subtest and the WAIS-III digit symbol subtest. Differential treatment response was specific to psychomotor speed because responders and nonresponders did not perform differently on tasks of executive functioning, attention, visuospatial functioning, or verbal intelligence. CONCLUSIONS: Psychomotor slowing may identify a subgroup of depressed patients who have a dopaminergic deficit that is unresponsive to fluoxetine monotherapy and who should therefore receive an alternative treatment.

Although the last five decades have seen many pharmacological advances in the treatment of depressive illness, still only half of all patients respond to their initial antidepressant treatment, leaving the remaining half symptomatic and functionally impaired (1). There is little empirical basis to guide clinicians in selecting one medication over another. The potential benefits of discovering predictors for individual antidepressants are considerable because patients could be matched to a medication to which they are most likely to respond. Because the first choice of treatment for the majority of depressed patients is a selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor (SSRI), it would also be useful to prospectively identify patients who are likely to be unresponsive to SSRI monotherapy because they could be given some alternative.

Presumably, differences in underlying pathophysiology account for the variability in antidepressant treatment response. To the extent that biological heterogeneity is reflected in symptom heterogeneity, phenomenology should inform treatment selection. Psychomotor retardation, a symptom exhibited by a subgroup of patients with depression, has been empirically linked with a specific pathophysiology and may therefore be a candidate predictor of antidepressant treatment outcome. Several lines of evidence link an underlying dopaminergic abnormality to the expression of psychomotor retardation, defined as a general slowing of motor activity and difficulty responding spontaneously and quickly to the environment. First, because psychomotor retardation is comparable to bradykinesia (i.e., a slowing down of spontaneous movement) and bradyphrenia (i.e., a slowness in thinking) in Parkinson’s disease, it has been suggested that, like patients with Parkinson’s disease, depressed patients with psychomotor retardation may have an abnormality in dopaminergic functioning. Moreover, decreased levels of CSF homovanillic acid, the main metabolite of dopamine, has been reported in depressed patients with psychomotor retardation (2, 3), and more recently, brain imaging techniques using dopamine D2 receptor binding have demonstrated reduced dopaminergic striatal functioning in the caudate-putamen of depressed patients with psychomotor retardation (4, 5).

The speed of psychomotor processing, a quantitative measure of psychomotor retardation assessed with neuropsychological tests (e.g., reaction time, speech rate, motor speed, mental speed, initiation and spontaneity of response), has also been linked to dopaminergic functioning. For example, reduced verbal fluency and poor performance on the digit symbol substitution test of the WAIS have been correlated with decreased dopamine functioning in the striatum (6). The influence of striatal dopamine on verbal fluency is supported by the finding that patients with Parkinson’s disease who discontinued levodopa exhibited impaired verbal fluency, which significantly improved when they resumed taking levodopa (7). Together, these studies suggest that verbal fluency and psychomotor speed may be contingent upon brain dopamine levels in the striatum.

It is reasonable to assume that in addition to dysfunction of dopaminergic striatal areas, an abnormality at any level within the functional network involved in psychomotor retardation would also result in psychomotor slowing (e.g., in regions within the dorsolateral prefrontal circuit that connect to the striatum). This is supported by the consistently replicated finding of an inverse correlation between psychomotor retardation and blood flow or metabolism to the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex of the left hemisphere (8, 9) and bilaterally (10, 11). Although reduced dorsolateral prefrontal perfusion has also been reported in depressed patients as a whole (i.e., with or without psychomotor retardation) (12, 13), as well as in other patient populations, such as those with schizophrenia (14, 15), it has been suggested that this is a result of subgroups, each with psychomotor slowing, in the groups studied. For example, the use of reduced speech to quantify psychomotor speed in depressed and schizophrenic patients with reduced speech output showed decreased blood flow in the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex that was evident even after control was added for dysphoria and diagnoses (8).

Taken together, various lines of investigation suggest an association between processing speed and dopamine levels and a link between psychomotor retardation and abnormal functioning of the basal ganglia and the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. This suggests that depressed patients with psychomotor slowing may have a dopaminergic abnormality within frontal-subcortical networks that causes or contributes to the pathophysiology of their disorder. This may have important treatment implications because these patients may preferentially benefit from a medication that directly targets dopaminergic neurotransmission. As such, one might predict that these patients would be unresponsive to SSRI antidepressants that primarily enhance serotonergic functioning. The only study to our knowledge that has investigated whether psychomotor speed on neuropsychological tests is related to SSRI response in depressed adults found no association (16). Of importance, however, the small group of 14 patients in this study limited its power to detect group differences. The current study investigated whether psychomotor speed would predict response to 12 weeks of SSRI treatment in a group of moderately depressed adult outpatients. We hypothesized that psychomotor slowing, measured with neuropsychological tests, would predict nonresponse to fluoxetine.

Method

The subjects were recruited from an outpatient research clinic at the New York State Psychiatric Institute. All patients met DSM-IV criteria for major depressive disorder and were between ages 18 and 65 years. Subjects were excluded if they met DSM-IV criteria for delusional, psychotic, bipolar, antisocial, personality, substance use, or organic mental disorders; schizophrenia; or the presence of psychotic features. The subjects did not have unstable physical disorders, acquired brain injury, degenerative diseases, cognitive changes following medical illness or surgery, memory disorders, language disorders, learning disability, or seizure disorder. All patients were native English speakers. After complete description of the study to the subjects, written informed consent was obtained.

The study design was a 12-week open trial of fluoxetine treatment with a 7–10-day medication-free lead-in period. During the lead-in period, the subjects were administered a short battery of neuropsychological tests. After the lead-in period, the patients whose depression was rated “much improved” or “very much improved” from baseline severity according to the Clinical Global Impression (CGI) Scale did not enter the treatment phase of the study. The patients whose depression was rated “minimally improved” to “worse” began a fixed flexible-dosing trial with fluoxetine (10 mg/day at week 1, 10–20 mg/day at weeks 2–4, 10–40 mg/day at weeks 5–8, and 10–60 mg/day at weeks 8–12). Response at week 12 was rated by an independent evaluator who was blind to the patients’ neuropsychological test results and the course of response or nonresponse during the 12 weeks. The patients who no longer met the criteria for major depression and had a CGI Scale score of “much improved” or “very much improved” were considered to be fluoxetine responders. All others were nonresponders.

Forty-seven patients who completed the neuropsychological test battery entered into the acute treatment phase. Ten dropped out before the end of the study, resulting in a total of 37 patients who completed a 12-week trial with fluoxetine. There were no differences between the dropouts and the completers on any neuropsychological or clinical measures. Table 1 gives the demographic and clinical characteristics of the 25 patients who were rated as fluoxetine responders and the 12 patients who were classified as nonresponders. There were no significant differences between the groups in gender, age, educational level, age at onset of the first depressive episode, pretreatment depression severity, or estimated pre-illness cognitive ability (vocabulary subtest of the WAIS-III).

The neuropsychological tests were selected based on prior research that associated psychomotor speed with dopaminergic functioning, with brain areas within striatofrontal circuitry, and/or with antidepressant response. Since performance on these tests requires the recruitment of many cognitive abilities, some control measures were administered to evaluate the specificity of the function in question.

The overall difference on neuropsychological measures of psychomotor speed was assessed through a multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) with performance on the four tests as the dependent measure and treatment response as the between-subject factor. Follow-up univariate t tests were then conducted.

The total 17-item Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (HAM-D) score at week 12 (i.e., depression severity at the end of the study) was used as an alternative means of quantifying response to fluoxetine treatment. Backward multiple regressions were used to predict HAM-D score outcome with measures of psychomotor speed as predictor variables. Pearson’s product-moment correlation analysis examined the relationship between performance on neuropsychological measures and pretreatment clinical variables.

A secondary analysis with independent t tests was employed to examine performance differences between responders and nonresponders on tasks measuring function in other cognitive domains, including attention, executive functioning, visuospatial functioning, and verbal intelligence.

Results

MANOVA indicated that the overall difference on neuropsychological measures of psychomotor speed significantly differentiated fluoxetine responders from nonresponders (Wilks’s lambda=0.639, F=4.51, df=4, 32, p=0.005). The results of follow-up univariate t tests are summarized in Table 2. At baseline, the patients who subsequently did not respond to fluoxetine verbalized significantly fewer words on the Controlled Oral Word Association Test FAS (COWAT FAS) (17) and named significantly fewer colors on the Stroop Color and Word Test (18) compared to the responders. Although the results were not statistically significant, the nonresponders tended to perform worse than the responders on the remaining two measures of processing speed (i.e., the Stroop Color and Word Test word reading subtest and the WAIS-III digit symbol subtest). Effect sizes for all four measures ranged from medium to large (0.61–1.44).

A backward multiple regression analysis with control for baseline depression severity demonstrated that of the tests of psychomotor speed, the baseline COWAT FAS significantly predicted week-12 HAM-D scores, with the model explaining 33% of the variance (Table 3). This, together with the significant inverse correlation between the COWAT FAS scores and the week-12 HAM-D scores (r=–0.535, p=0.001) and the weaker associations between the HAM-D scores and those of the other timed tests (e.g., the Stroop Color and Word Test color naming subtest: r=–0.256, p=0.13; the Stroop Color and Word Test word reading subtest: r=–0.240, p=0.15; and the WAIS-III digit symbol subtest: r=–0.182, p=0.28), suggests that COWAT FAS performance may be the strongest predictor of fluoxetine response. Figure 1 plots the individual COWAT FAS scores at baseline for the fluoxetine responders and the nonresponders, together with the published COWAT FAS normative scores for a healthy group matched to the depressed patients by age and level of education (19). As shown, all of the nonresponders generated fewer words at baseline than the normative mean, with the difference between the two groups approaching significance (t=–1.83, df=252, p<0.07). The depressed patients who responded to fluoxetine performed significantly better on the COWAT FAS than the normative group (t=2.22, df=265, p<0.03). With the mean for the normative group used as a cutoff, those who scored below the mean had a response rate of only 40% (eight of 20) to fluoxetine. In contrast, 100% (17 of 17) of the depressed patients who scored above the mean responded to fluoxetine.

No significant relationships were found between processing speed and pretreatment depression severity, chronicity, or age at onset of the first depressive episode (range=–0.213 to 0.105). A backward multiple regression analysis predicting outcome with control for these baseline characteristics again demonstrated that of the measures of processing speed, only the COWAT FAS scores significantly predicted the outcome of HAM-D scores.

The differentiation of responders and nonresponders according to baseline physician-rated psychomotor retardation (i.e., the HAM-D psychomotor retardation item) approached significance; 50% of the nonresponders (six of 12) were judged to have psychomotor retardation at baseline versus 16% of the responders (four of 25) (χ2=3.19, df=1, p<0.08). To assess whether physician-rated psychomotor retardation is as powerful a predictor of outcome as the four measures of psychomotor speed, a backward multiple regression analysis was performed. After we controlled for baseline severity (HAM-D scores minus the psychomotor retardation item scores), only the COWAT FAS (β=–0.281, SE=0.093, p=0.005) and the HAM-D psychomotor retardation item (β=3.352, SE=1.878, p<0.09) remained in the model. Baseline depression severity did not significantly predict outcome.

In terms of performance in other cognitive domains, no significant differences were found between the responders and the nonresponders on the Stroop Color and Word Test interference score, the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (number of categories completed and number of perseverative errors) (20), or the WAIS-III subtests for block design, digit span, or vocabulary (Table 2).

Discussion

This study was designed to investigate whether baseline performance on neuropsychological tests of processing speed predicts response to fluoxetine in depressed outpatients. The patients who were resistant to 12 weeks of fluoxetine treatment exhibited significantly reduced pretreatment performance on the COWAT FAS of verbal fluency and the Stroop Color and Word Test color naming subtest compared to the patients who responded to fluoxetine. A less-than-significant tendency in the same direction was demonstrated for the Stroop Color and Word Test word reading subtest and the WAIS-III digit symbol subtest.

If, as previously discussed, depressed subjects with psychomotor slowing are unresponsive to the serotonin-acting agent fluoxetine because of a neural dopaminergic dysfunction, one would expect that treatment that directly targeted dopamine neurotransmission might be effective. This was nicely demonstrated by Rampello et al. (21), who assessed the effectiveness of antidepressants with different affinities to dopamine in patients diagnosed with retarded depression. After 6 weeks of treatment, the patients who were treated with amineptine (a selective inhibitor of dopamine reuptake) showed greater improvement on both the Psychomotor Retardation Scale and the HAM-D than the patients treated with minaprine (a nonselective reuptake inhibitor), clomipramine (affects serotonin and, to a lesser extent, norepinephrine), and placebo. Furthermore, the patients had a better response to minaprine than to clomipramine, and clomipramine response was not superior to that of placebo. This suggests that in depressed patients with psychomotor retardation, there is a graded response to treatments with increasing affinities to dopamine. The poor response in patients with psychomotor retardation to clomipramine, a drug that primarily affects serotonin, lends validity to the current findings of an unfavorable response to an SSRI in patients with psychomotor slowing.

Of interest, verbal fluency and psychomotor retardation have also been associated with treatment outcome in patients with late-life depression. In particular, Kalayam and Alexopoulos (22) found that elderly depressed patients who were unresponsive to antidepressants (the majority of which were SSRIs and tricyclics) showed baseline psychomotor retardation, a prolonged latency of the P300 evoked potential (an electrophysiological measure of psychomotor speed), and a low initiation-perseveration score (which consists largely of verbal fluency) on the Mattis Dementia Rating Scale compared to responders. The present study identified a younger subgroup of depressed patients also characterized by reduced verbal fluency, psychomotor retardation, and SSRI resistance, suggesting a possible common underlying pathophysiology between the younger and geriatric subgroups.

The findings of this study were specific to tests of processing speed. Fluoxetine responders and nonresponders demonstrated similar performance on tasks that index functioning in other cognitive domains, including executive functioning, attention, visuospatial functioning, and verbal intelligence. This suggests not only that processing speed uniquely predicted fluoxetine response but also that these other cognitive functions had a negligible contribution to the nonresponders’ reduced processing speed. For example, since fluoxetine responders and nonresponders performed similarly on a test of attention, attentional differences between the groups could not account for the differences exhibited on the timed tasks. The specificity of a differential treatment response based on measures of psychomotor speed is supported by the study of Kalayam and Alexopoulos (22) in which antidepressant responders and nonresponders differed in baseline psychomotor slowing but not on tasks of attention, conceptualization, memory, or construction.

The COWAT FAS emerged as the strongest predictor of fluoxetine response, followed by clinician ratings of psychomotor retardation. Because this test measures the initiation and speed of verbal response, clinicians may have detected and rated slowed or reduced verbal processing at baseline as psychomotor retardation in fluoxetine nonresponders. That the COWAT FAS is a more powerful predictor than clinicians’ ratings, however, suggests that it is more sensitive. Nonetheless, the contribution of reduced verbal processing to psychomotor slowing in fluoxetine nonresponders is consistent with previous findings by our group. In the studies that used dichotic measures to assess hemispheric laterality, fluoxetine nonresponders demonstrated a reduced advantage of the left hemisphere for verbal processing compared to responders (2325). Conversely, the responders had a significantly larger left-hemisphere advantage for verbal processing than both the nonresponders and the normal comparison subjects. Similarly, in the present study, verbal processing measured with the COWAT FAS was lower for the nonresponders than that of the published norms matched for age and education, whereas the mean performance of the responders was significantly higher than the normative mean. Neuroanatomically, because the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and the left anterior cingulate are activated during verbal fluency tasks (2629), activation of these brain structures may differ between fluoxetine responders and nonresponders. This is supported by neuroimaging studies that found increased baseline rostral anterior cingulate and dorsolateral prefrontal cortical activity in depressed patients who subsequently responded to antidepressant treatment (3033).

This study has several limitations. First, it was an open treatment study (i.e., with no placebo control group), and therefore, it is unknown whether response was attributable to the drug or to something nonspecific. Another drawback is that the predictive value of the findings does not necessarily generalize to SSRIs other than fluoxetine, and therefore, response to various SSRIs, in addition to other classes of antidepressants, should be examined in patients with slow processing speed. Finally, because the flexible-dose design used in this study may have led to an underestimation of the predictive value of psychomotor slowing, it would be worthwhile for future studies to use a fixed dose.

Psychomotor speed as a predictor of fluoxetine response has important clinical and heuristic implications. On a clinical level, prospectively identifying patients who are unlikely to respond to one of the most frequently prescribed antidepressants would guide physicians to initiate treatment with an alternative medication. This is critical because it can potentially reduce the time to symptom relief and increase treatment compliance. Furthermore, it can prevent the premature discontinuation of treatment that often results from feelings of hopelessness, helplessness, and frustration due to an ineffective therapeutic response. Another significant advantage of using cognitive measures to predict treatment response is that unlike some other approaches that may eventually offer predictive value (e.g., brain imaging), tests of psychomotor speed are noninvasive, are of minimal cost, and can be performed quickly. For example, the COWAT FAS can be administered in 5 minutes. On a heuristic level, depressed patients with psychomotor retardation may define a homogeneous subgroup of depressed patients with a unique pathophysiology and prognosis for different classes of antidepressants. Studying this homogeneous entity with functional neuroimaging will help to elucidate the functional neural networks involved in its underlying pathophysiology and possibly reveal structural and functional differences between fluoxetine responders and nonresponders.

TABLE 1
TABLE 2
TABLE 3

Received June 1, 2004; revision received Aug. 4, 2004; accepted Sept. 13, 2004. From the Depression Evaluation Service, New York State Psychiatric Institute, Department of Psychiatry, Columbia University College of Physicians and Surgeons. Address correspondence and reprint requests to Dr. Taylor, Depression Evaluation Service, New York State Psychiatric Institute, Department of Psychiatry, Columbia University College of Physicians and Surgeons, 1051 Riverside Dr., New York, NY 10032; (e-mail). Dr. Quitkin died October 2005. Supported by a grant from Eli Lilly and Company.

Figure 1.

Figure 1. Baseline Controlled Oral Word Association Test FAS Scores of Depressed Responders (N=25) and Nonresponders (N=12) to 12 Weeks of Fluoxetine Treatment

References

1. Rush AJ, Trivedi M, Fava M: Depression, IV: STAR*D treatment trial for depression (image, neuro). Am J Psychiatry 2003; 160:237LinkGoogle Scholar

2. van Praag HM, Korf J: Retarded depression and the dopamine metabolism. Psychopharmacologia 1971; 19:199–203Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

3. Post RM, Kotin J, Goodwin FK, Gordon EK: Psychomotor activity and cerebrospinal fluid amine metabolites in affective illness. Am J Psychiatry 1973; 130:67–72LinkGoogle Scholar

4. Ebert D, Feistel H, Loew T, Pirner A: Dopamine and depression—striatal dopamine D2 receptor SPECT before and after antidepressant therapy. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1996; 126:91–94Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

5. Paillère Martinot M-L, Bragulat V, Artiges E, Dollé F, Hinnen F, Jouvent R, Martinot J-L: Decreased presynaptic dopamine function in the left caudate of depressed patients with affective flattening and psychomotor retardation. Am J Psychiatry 2001; 158:314–316LinkGoogle Scholar

6. Shah PJ, Ogilvie AD, Goodwin GM, Ebmeier KP: Clinical and psychometric correlates of dopamine D2 binding in depression. Psychol Med 1997; 27:1247–1256Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

7. Gotham AM, Brown RG, Marsden CD: “Frontal” cognitive function in patients with Parkinson’s disease “on” and “off” levodopa. Brain 1988; 111(part 2):299-321Google Scholar

8. Dolan RJ, Bench CJ, Liddle PF, Friston KJ, Frith CD, Grasby PM, Frackowiak RS: Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex dysfunction in the major psychoses: symptom or disease specificity? J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1993; 56:1290–1294Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

9. Galynker II, Cai J, Ongseng F, Finestone H, Dutta E, Serseni D: Hypofrontality and negative symptoms in major depressive disorder. J Nucl Med 1998; 39:608–612MedlineGoogle Scholar

10. Mayberg HS, Lewis PJ, Regenold W, Wagner HN Jr: Paralimbic hypoperfusion in unipolar depression. J Nucl Med 1994; 35:929–934MedlineGoogle Scholar

11. Videbech P, Ravnkilde B, Pedersen TH, Hartvig H, Egander A, Clemmensen K, Rasmussen NA, Andersen F, Gjedde A, Rosenberg R: The Danish PET/Depression Project: clinical symptoms and cerebral blood flow: a regions-of-interest analysis. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2002; 106:35–44Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

12. Baxter LR Jr, Schwartz JM, Phelps ME, Mazziotta JC, Guze BH, Selin CE, Gerner RH, Sumida RM: Reduction of prefrontal cortex glucose metabolism common to three types of depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1989; 46:243–250Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

13. Drevets WC, Videen TO, Price JL, Preskorn SH, Carmichael ST, Raichle ME: A functional anatomical study of unipolar depression. J Neurosci 1992; 12:3628–3641Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

14. Berman KF, Illowsky BP, Weinberger DR: Physiological dysfunction of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia, IV: further evidence for regional and behavioral specificity. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1988; 45:616–622Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

15. Weinberger DR, Berman KF, Zec RF: Physiologic dysfunction of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia, I: regional cerebral blood flow evidence. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1986; 43:114–124Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

16. Dunkin JJ, Leuchter AF, Cook IA, Kasl-Godley JE, Abrams M, Rosenberg-Thompson S: Executive dysfunction predicts nonresponse to fluoxetine in major depression. J Affect Disord 2000; 60:13–23Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

17. Benton AL, Hamsher K, Sivan AB: Multilingual Aphasia Examination (3rd ed.). Iowa City, AJA Associates, 1983Google Scholar

18. Golden CJ: Stroop Color and Word Test: A Manual for Clinical and Experimental Uses. Wood Dale, Ill, Stoelting Co, 1978Google Scholar

19. Tombaugh TN, Kozak J, Rees L: Normative data for the Controlled Oral Association Test (1996), in A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests, 2nd ed. Edited by Spreen O, Strauss E. New York, Oxford University Press, 1998, pp 453-457Google Scholar

20. Heaton RK, Chelune GJ, Talley JL, Kay GG, Curtiss G: Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Manual. Odessa, Fla, Psychological Assessment Resources, 1993Google Scholar

21. Rampello L, Nicoletti G, Raffaele R: Dopaminergic hypothesis for retarded depression: a symptom profile for predicting therapeutical responses. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1991; 84:552–554Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

22. Kalayam B, Alexopoulos GS: Prefrontal dysfunction and treatment response in geriatric depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1999; 56:713–718Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

23. Bruder GE, Stewart JW, McGrath PJ, Deliyannides D, Quitkin FM: Dichotic listening tests of functional brain asymmetry predict response to fluoxetine in depressed women and men. Neuropsychopharmacology 2004; 29:1742–1761CrossrefGoogle Scholar

24. Bruder GE, Otto MW, McGrath PJ, Stewart JW, Fava M, Rosenbaum JF, Quitkin FM: Dichotic listening before and after fluoxetine treatment for major depression: relations of laterality to therapeutic response. Neuropsychopharmacology 1996; 15:171–179Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

25. Bruder GE, Stewart JW, Tenke CE, McGrath PJ, Leite P, Bhattacharya N, Quitkin FM: Electroencephalographic and perceptual asymmetry differences between responders and nonresponders to an SSRI antidepressant. Biol Psychiatry 2001; 49:416–425Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

26. Ravnkilde B, Videbech P, Rosenberg R, Gjedde A, Gade A: Putative tests of frontal lobe function: a PET-study of brain activation during Stroop’s test and verbal fluency. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2002; 24:534–547Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

27. Frith CD, Friston KJ, Liddle PF, Frackowiak RS: A PET study of word finding. Neuropsychologia 1991; 29:1137–1148Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

28. Friston KJ, Frith CD, Liddle PF, Frackowiak RS: Investigating a network model of word generation with positron emission tomography. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 1991; 244:101–106CrossrefGoogle Scholar

29. Schlosser R, Hutchinson M, Joseffer S, Rusinek H, Saarimaki A, Stevenson J, Dewey SL, Brodie JD: Functional magnetic resonance imaging of human brain activity in a verbal fluency task. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1998; 64:492–498Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

30. Mayberg HS, Brannan SK, Mahurin RK, Jerabek PA, Brickman JS, Tekell JL, Silva JA, McGinnis S, Glass TG, Martin CC, Fox PT: Cingulate function in depression: a potential predictor of treatment response. Neuroreport 1997; 8:1057–1061Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

31. Pizzagalli D, Pascual-Marqui RD, Nitschke JB, Oakes TR, Larson CL, Abercrombie HC, Schaefer SM, Koger JV, Benca RM, Davidson RJ: Anterior cingulate activity as a predictor of degree of treatment response in major depression: evidence from brain electrical tomography analysis. Am J Psychiatry 2001; 158:405–415LinkGoogle Scholar

32. Saxena S, Brody AL, Ho ML, Zohrabi N, Maidment KM, Baxter LR Jr: Differential brain metabolic predictors of response to paroxetine in obsessive-compulsive disorder versus major depression. Am J Psychiatry 2003; 160:522–532LinkGoogle Scholar

33. Davidson RJ, Irwin W, Anderle MJ, Kalin NH: The neural substrates of affective processing in depressed patients treated with venlafaxine. Am J Psychiatry 2003; 160:64–75LinkGoogle Scholar