The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×
Special ArticleFull Access

Is Schizophrenia a Progressive Neurodevelopmental Disorder? Toward a Unitary Pathogenetic Mechanism

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1176/ajp.155.12.1661

Abstract

Objective:The author 1) reassesses the case against a neuronal degeneration hypothesis for schizophrenia; 2) demonstrates that the hypothesis that schizophrenia is a disorder caused by early (i.e., pre- or perinatal) and static (i.e., fixed, nonprogressive) damage to the brain is unsatisfactory because it cannot readily account for brain imaging results from schizophrenic patients and lacks both satisfactory clinical examples and experimental models of early, static developmental disorders resulting in the late spontaneous functional deterioration that characterizes schizophrenia; and 3) offers an alternative pathogenetic hypothesis for schizophrenia that is consistent with the available imaging and neuropathological data.Method:Published data on schizophrenia and relevant clinical and experimental studies of neurodevelopment and its disorders are reviewed.Results:The neuropathological studies provide strong evidence against a classic neurodegenerative pathogenesis of schizophrenia and moderate support for prenatal developmental abnormalities. The imaging data provide strong evidence that excessive brain volume loss occurs after maximum brain volume expansion and equivocal evidence that it continues after onset of overt illness. The available clinical and experimental models of late deterioration after static, early brain lesions are unconvincing. A progressive developmental mechanism can reconcile the neuropathological and imaging data, while being compatible with both early onset and late deterioration.Conclusions:It matters whether the pathogenetic agent in schizophrenia is static or progressive, since if it is the latter it is worthwhile to search not only for means of prevention but also for interventions that will arrest progression as early as possible. Am J Psychiatry 1998; 155: 1661-1670

When schizophrenia was first described, it was believed to be caused by a new form of progressive neuronal degeneration characterized by earlier onset than that seen with previously described entities, such as Huntington’s disease or Alzheimer’s disease. In recent years, however, the original Kraepelinian pathogenetic theory of premature progressive neuronal degeneration has come to be opposed by a pathogenetic model that postulates that schizophrenia results from a nonprogressive pre- or perinatal derangement of development. Nevertheless, since there is a plethora of sometimes contradictory-appearing findings and as yet no firmly established cause for schizophrenia, there is currently a search by many observers and research groups for a unifying hypothesis(14). A synthesis of apparently conflicting data is also the goal of this article, which begins with an analysis of the case against the neurodegenerative model, follows with a critical look at the currently dominant form of neurodevelopmental hypothesis, and finally outlines a potential unifying hypothesis: schizophrenia as a progressive developmental disorder.

MAJOR BASES FOR REJECTION OF NEURODEGENERATION AS A PATHOGENETIC MECHANISM OF SCHIZOPHRENIA

The roots of the debate as to whether schizophrenia is neurodegenerative or neurodevelopmental go back a century (5,6), and the somewhat parallel debate about imaging evidence for progression after onset of illness is almost as old (7). The current stage of the debate began in 1976 with the first (8) of a flood of new imaging studies using computerized tomography (CT) that soon led to the strong claim that excessive brain volume loss was seen in many schizophrenic patients, that it was already present at the onset of illness, and that it did not progress thereafter (9). Beginning in 1983, reports of a number of neuropathological studies of schizophrenia using new methods were published (1013), and the combination of these results with the imaging data completed the modern case against a neurodegenerative, and for a neurodevelopmental, pathogenesis of schizophrenia (13,14).

Neuropathological Evidence

The essence of the case against a neurodegenerative mechanism is that gliosis, which is regarded as a necessary neuropathological hallmark of neuronal degeneration (15), has not been found in a number of careful controlled postmortem studies that used either immunochemical staining for glial fibrillary acid protein (11, 13, 16) or Nissl staining (10, 12, 17, 18) in examinations of brains of schizophrenic persons.

There are several possible criticisms of this negative evidence. First, there were earlier studies that did show gliosis(19, 20), and the difference might be explained by possible dependence of the results on specific staining procedures, i.e., less sensitivity of the glial fibrillary acid protein and Nissl methods than of Holzer staining (6) or vulnerability to long fixation times (21). Beyond these technical issues there is also an apparent contradiction between the failure to find gliosis in postmortem specimens and the large body of epidemiological studies indicating that perinatal brain damage plays a role in the development of schizophrenia (2224). This dilemma arises because the brain can respond to injury with gliosis as early as the 20th week of gestation (25), and certainly throughout the third trimester (26), so that perinatal injuries should have also resulted in gliosis (26). Nevertheless, the balance of neuropathological evidence is strongly against excessive gliosis being characteristic of schizophrenia.

Neuroimaging Evidence

The failure of the first prospective CT studies of schizophrenia (2729) to show the same age-disproportionate progressive increase in ventricle-brain ratio (VBR) after onset of illness that is seen in disorders such as Huntington’s disease (14) also led to rejection of a classic neurodegeneration model for the disease. However, there are problems with both the empirical basis and the underlying assumption leading to this conclusion. First of all, since 1989 there have been both negative (30,31) and positive (3234) longitudinal CT findings of progressive volume loss in chronic schizophrenia and both negative (31, 3537) and positive (3840) longitudinal magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) results in follow-up investigations of first-episode schizophrenia. In addition, there is now evidence for progressive ventricular enlargement in childhood-onset schizophrenia (41).

Second, failure to find consistent imaging evidence of ongoing brain volume loss is not by itself decisive evidence against neuronal degeneration unless one argues that the temporal relationship of degeneration to symptomatic illness in schizophrenia is like that in Huntington’s disease (in which patients become symptomatic relatively early in the course of their illness, i.e., before neuronal death has affected the majority of the neurons at risk). This is so because only patients with neuronal degenerative disorders who become symptomatic early are likely to show progressive volume loss with age on repeated imaging (figure 1 contains an illustration of this pattern). However, if schizophrenia is a disorder like Parkinson’s disease, in which symptoms develop late relative to neuronal loss (42), then most of the excessive volume loss might already be present at the onset of the illness, and observable subsequent progression of volumeloss may be largely limited to first-episode patients (38, 39), very young patients (41), and poor-outcome, clinically deteriorating patients (33,34).

Absence of gliosis remains the cornerstone of the case against classically defined neurodegeneration as a major pathogenetic mechanism of schizophrenia, and the relative weakness of the imaging case against progressive volume loss does not change this conclusion. The question of progression or lack thereof is nevertheless important because although neurodegeneration necessarily implies at least some degree of progression, the converse need not be true, and thus the absence of gliosis alone does not exclude all forms of neuropathological progression.

CRITIQUE OF NEURODEVELOPMENTAL HYPOTHESES OF SCHIZOPHRENIA

There are at least six broad major distinctions that are relevant to current theories of the causation of schizophrenia: 1) homogeneous or heterogeneous, 2) genetic or environmental, 3) single or multiple factors, 4) static or progressive, 5) short or long latency, and 6) developmental or nondevelopmental. Although these six dichotomies are independent of one another, so that all the combinations are possible, the analysis to follow is primarily concerned with only the last three. Because of this focus, the discussion could be read as implying that schizophrenia is assumed to be a single disorder with a single cause, but no such strong claim is intended, and the unifying hypothesis put forward, although intended to apply to a majority of schizophrenic patients, is consistent with causation by different genes in different subgroups, with multifactorial (either polygenic or genetic plus environmental) causation in individual patients, and even with the existence of a relatively small subgroup of patients whose illness has a primarily environmental etiology.

In the interest of clarity it should be noted that “developmental” is used in the literature with two meanings. Classically, it has been used in a temporal sense to indicate that the time during which a pathogenetic process actively damages the nervous system is limited to fetal or perinatal life, but for some time the same term has also been used to refer to the elaborate neurobiological mechanisms controlling the process of development. If the term is used only in a static temporal sense, then disorders of neural development are by definition early and nonprogressive, but if developmental refers to the neurobiological processes themselves, several of which normally extend into adult life, then developmental disorders caused by defects in one or another of these processes can continue to exert active pathological effects postnatally, i.e., to progress. It is crucial to note that the terms “static” and “progressive” are being used here in a narrow and mutually exclusive sense to contrast early termination to continued operation of a pathogenetic agent, and not in a broad sense that would encompass all of the continually unfolding secondary consequences of the pathology.

The major developmental processes of the human telencephalon following closure of the neural tube are neuronal proliferation, astrocytic proliferation, neuronal migration, neurite (i.e., axonal and dendritic) proliferation, neuronal apoptosis, axon myelination, and neurite pruning. All of these processes begin during gestation, but neurite proliferation, axonal myelination, and neurite pruning exert their major effects during postgestational development. It is interesting that physiological neuronal apoptosis may also be a normal part of human aging in certain restricted brain regions, most notably the substantia nigra, zona compacta (43,44).

The current positive evidence for a neurodevelopmental pathogenesis of schizophrenia (in the static temporal sense of the term “developmental”) has been set forth in a number of reviews (6, 26, 45, 46). It includes neuropathological reports of cytoarchitectural abnormalities that are interpreted as being developmental in origin; absence of normal cerebral left-right temporal lobe asymmetries; results of prospective longitudinal studies comparing “high-risk” children, one or both of whose parents have schizophrenia or schizophrenia spectrum disease, to “low-risk” subjects; epidemiological studies relating higher prevalence of schizophrenia to winter season of birth, second-trimester maternal influenza, first-trimester malnutrition, and obstetric complications; and finally, data from retrospective studies that show subtle motor and facial expression abnormalities, mild learning difficulties, and impairment of social skills to be already present years before overt illness. This large body of evidence will not be reviewed in detail because, while it makes a strong case for placing the beginnings of pathogenesis in the pre- or perinatal period, it does not exclude subsequent postnatal progression.

Given the large range of possible neurodevelopmental derangements that might be implicated in disease pathogenesis, the main neurodevelopmental hypotheses for schizophrenia set forth in the last 10 years (14, 26, 47, 48) are relatively restricted and share three assumptions: 1) the primary pathogenetic defect is an early derangement of the orderly development of the central nervous system that occurs in the pre- or perinatal period; 2) the period of active operation of the causative agent is of short duration, meaning that it is essentially static; and 3) the behavioral consequences of this static process remain relatively latent until long after the primary pathogenetic process has run its course. The various neurodevelopmental models hypothesizing an early, static, and long-latency defect differ from one another primarily in the relative weight given to genetic versus environmental factors within individuals and in the degree of emphasis on hetero- or homogeneity of the disorder. (There is one neurodevelopmental model hypothesizing a late, static, short-latency defect [49], but discussion of that model will be deferred to the next major section of this article.)

There are two major problems with the models involving an early, static defect. First, the excessive extra­cerebral CSF found in many imaging studies of schizophrenia is difficult for these hypotheses to explain. Second, schizophrenia is an illness that typically manifests itself in late adolescence or early adult life with major, progressive, and usually irreversible deterioration from previous levels of functioning (1), and attempts to explain such a devastating disease by a subtle, localized, nonprogressive prenatal genetic or environmental insult to the brain run counter to a large body of data on functional recovery after early brain lesions (discussed later).

Implications of Imaging Data

The progression of quantitative brain imaging from CT scanning and the VBR to whole-brain volumetry with MRI has made reliable quantitative measurements of intracranial volume and its compartments, extracerebral CSF, ventricular CSF, gray matter, and white matter feasible. Since 1991 a number of MRI studies of chronically ill schizophrenic patients using quantitative measurements of gray and white matter and CSF, and using either ratios or covariance to control for intracranial volume, have shown a significant excessive overall loss of brain tissue (50,51) or gray matter (5255) or an increase in extracerebral (sulcal) CSF space (52, 5456) or a decrease of brain tissue in frontal regions (50,52) or in anterior cerebral cortex (57) or orbitofrontal and mesiotemporal cortex (58). Negative quantitative MRI results have been reported by one group for whole brain (59) and for prefrontal regions (60).

These findings of extracerebral CSF increase and brain volume decrease have important implications for any model of the neuropathogenesis of schizophrenia because of three fundamental principles of normal neural development: first, brain growth drives intracranial cavity growth; second, the brain grows outward from the ventricles; and third, intracranial cavity expansion is not reversible after skull sutures fuse. As a result, after brain growth reaches its maximum, intracranial size remains constant and any loss of brain volume leads to an equivalent increase in extracerebral and intraventricular CSF volume (see references52 and 61 for more extensive discussions).

Given these principles, one can readily predict that diffuse loss of brain tissue limited to the pre- or perinatal time period will result in a smaller intracranial cavity and persistent enlargement of the lateral ventricles but not to an increase in extracerebral CSF space. This is so because brain volume more than triples between birth and age 5 years, and any increase in extracerebral CSF caused by an earlier nonprogressive lesion would tend to be “filled up” by subsequent “outward” growth. Only a diffuse lesion resulting in a loss of brain tissue volume after maximum brain volume expansion has already taken place will result in an equivalent increase in total CSF space, both extracerebral and ventricular, and no change in intracranial volume; because the ventricles normally account for only about 10%–15% of total intracranial CSF volume (50,61), most of the compensating increase in intracranial CSF space will be extracerebral.

It is just because the ventricles can enlarge with both early and late tissue volume loss that cross-sectional studies, which look only at the ratio of lateral ventricular volume to total brain tissue volume (i.e., the VBR), can detect tissue loss but cannot distinguish between early and later time of occurrence. Only volume measurements that include extracerebral CSF volumes can determine whether or not there has been later volume loss.

Although the combination of excessive tissue volume loss and CSF space increase observed in patients with chronic schizophrenia might be a nonspecific consequence of psychosis, rather than reflecting an underlying causative neuropathological process, there are other imaging results that are not consistent with CSF volume expansion as an epiphenomenon of psychosis. The one published MRI study of new-onset schizophrenia in which tissue volume and both ventricular and extracerebral CSF were measured (62) showed that, in relation to comparison subjects, the patients had a mean of 30 cc less brain tissue, 25 cc more extracerebral CSF, and 3 cc more ventricular CSF. Since intracranial volume did not differ between the groups, these results imply that the patients’ volume loss occurred in the time period after maximum brain volume expansion but before onset of overt illness.

Moreover, in a longitudinal “high risk” study that looked at the effects of both perinatal complications and genetic loading for schizophrenia on ratios of ventricular and sulcal (i.e., extracerebral) CSF volumes to intracranial volume, Cannon et al. (63) found that while both of these ratios were positively correlated with genetic loading, ventricular enlargement was related to a history of perinatal injuries while sulcal enlargement was not. Since most of the subjects scanned in this large study did not have a diagnosis of schizophrenia themselves, and since the results held up when the overtly schizophrenic subjects were excluded, the positive relationship of extracerebral CSF volume to genetic risk for schizophrenia found in these subjects is not related to perinatal injury and cannot be a consequence of psychosis.

It should be emphasized that positive evidence for later brain volume loss in schizophrenia does not exclude early loss or failure of growth, even in the same patients. As already noted, early volume loss should be manifest as smaller intracranial volume, and a significant decrease in the latter was found in the frontal regions in a patient group who also demonstrated an overall excess of extracerebral CSF(52, 61) and was also found in a meta-analysis of head size data from a large number of imaging studies (64). Thus, although the rate of brain volume growth may be slower than normal beginning in early life (perhaps before birth) and also regress excessively in the period after onset of puberty, the critical point is that this later volume loss cannot be explained by the model of an early, static defect.

Problem of Latency of Clinical Illness Manifestation

The second major problem for neurodevelopmental models of schizophrenia that point to an early, static defect is the long latency between birth and the onset of clinically overt illness. To answer this objection, proponents of these hypotheses have sought parallels in established human and experimental animal models of developmental disorders. The most commonly proposed human models are developmental disorders such as dyslexia, in which the disorder is presumed to originate prenatally but not become manifest until the individual is faced with a later functional challenge (e.g., learning to read). In particular, the “lack of asymmetry” developmental model of schizophrenia (4) draws heavily on the lack of cerebral asymmetry observed in both dyslexia (65) and some but not all studies of schizophrenia (66). One general problem with this analogy is that schizophrenia is an absolute deterioration from previous levels of functioning (1), not a failure to keep pace with unaffected peers. There is, however, also a potentially important specific difference between the anatomy of schizophrenia and that of dyslexia; in schizophrenia the lack of asymmetry appears to result from a reduction in volume of the side that is normally larger (see the raw data of Bilder et al. [66]), while in dyslexia there is an expansion of the side that is normally smaller (65).

Other advocates of developmental models hypothesizing a genetic and/or environmental early, static defect (see, for instance, references (14, 46, and 48) have looked to a combination of human neuroanatomical data (e.g., late myelination of parahippocampal regions[67]) and animal models (46, 6871) for plausible explanations or examples of late deterioration of function after static acquired pre- or perinatal lesions. The findings of Goldman (72) in monkeys with prefrontal lesions acquired in infancy or the later juvenile period are frequently cited as illustrating just such a pattern, but a careful examination of those results does not support this interpretation. In that study, lesioned monkeys were repeatedly tested on a delayed alternation task at approximate ages 15 and 30 months (for lesions acquired in infancy) or at approximate ages 34 and 43 months (for lesions during the juvenile period). The performance of the early-lesioned animals was somewhat worse than that of control subjects at 15 months but did not change significantly from 15 to 30 months; what changed was the control subjects’ performance, which improved. Thus, the gap between the performance of the lesion and control groups present at 15 months had widened by 30 months, owing to improvement by the control subjects rather than any overall deterioration in the lesioned animals (72). It should be noted that the performance of the monkeys with early lesions was always superior to that of the monkeys with later lesions, implying some degree of compensatory reorganization (73). These conclusions, which are the same as those drawn by the original author (74), make it clear that this study does not provide an example of late functional deterioration after an early static lesion.

More generally, neurobiologists have had a long-standing interest in the consequences of early brain lesions, and the issue has been extensively studied. As originally noted by Kennard (75), the developing central nervous system is able to compensate to a considerable degree for the deleterious effects of exogenous insults, even quite major ones, such as necrosis of a whole hemisphere (76), so that the presumption is that if the fetus or infant survives and if the external agent stops being active (e.g., chronic seizure activity does not develop [[77, p. 348]), then compensation will take place and, with certain exceptions, the earlier the insult, the greater the compensation (78). The pattern of compensation is one of a relative sparing of early-maturing functions followed by a relative deficit of later-maturing functions (79). There are experimental models in which early lesions are worse than later lesions (80,81), but in general these are not models of early lesions followed by a late deterioration of function from previously achieved levels.

One apparent exception to this conclusion that there is a dearth of late-deterioration models is a series of studies by Lipska and colleagues of pharmacologically induced neonatal ventral hippocampal lesions in rats (6870). Such lesions are reported to lead to late-developing (postpubertal) vulnerability to both stress and medications (amphetamine, apomorphine, and others) that varies significantly in severity between one strain of rats and another (69). This model does address the long-latency problem of the static-defect developmental hypothesis and also shows an intriguing interaction between an external injury and the genetic anlage. Nevertheless, careful analysis of the reported data suggests that in the experiments that were limited to environmental (i.e., nonpharmacological) stresses or stimuli, the model does not demonstrate spontaneous late deterioration of function after a static, early lesion.

On certain measures of behavior induced by environmental stresses, rats with ventral hippocampal lesions acquired on postnatal day 7 showed greater differences from control subjects, given sham operations, when tested on postnatal day 56 than they had when given the same tests on day 35 (68). However, the data do not show that the absolute level of function of the lesioned rats got worse from day 35 to day 56. For example, in the case of what is known as prepulse inhibition, in which the startle response to a loud noise is attenuated by an immediately preceding small tone, on day 35 the unmedicated sham-operation rats had about 29% prepulse inhibition after a 4-dB warning tone, while the unmedicated lesioned rats had approximately an 18% prepulse inhibition, a nonsignificant difference (see figures 3 and 4 in reference 68). However, when the experiment was repeated at day 56, the sham-operation rats showed a 59% startle reduction and the lesioned rats a 30% reduction. This lesion-control difference was significant, but the changes over time came about not because the prepulse inhibition of the lesioned rats deteriorated from day 35 to 56, but because their performance improved only 12% while that of the control rats improved 30%.

A similar pattern is seen for another behavior, spontaneous locomotion. Unmedicated lesioned rats actually showed a slight decrease in spontaneous locomotion between the ages of 35 and 56 days, but the decline was significantly less than that seen in control rats (see figure 3 of reference 70). Here too there was a relative but not absolute decline in function with age.

In another animal model paradigm of schizophrenia that involves an early pharmacological lesion, it has been proposed that early lesions lead to a neural disinhibition that results in delayed secondary excitotoxicity, which in turn leads to cortical volume loss (71). This model can account for both the evidence that there are early brain lesions in schizophrenia and that there is later brain volume loss, but it deals with the lack of gliosis in the cortex of schizophrenic persons by simply concluding that this particular secondary excitotoxic process does not result in substantial persistent gliosis (71).

Thus, if the issue is whether schizophrenia is neurodegenerative in the classic sense or neurodevelopmental in the static temporal sense, then it must be concluded that the combination of lack of gliosis with late loss of cortical volume poses serious problems for both hypotheses. If, however, the term “developmental disorder” is extended to encompass defects in one or another of the developmental processes that have prenatal origins but exert their major effects during childhood, adolescence, and even early adult years, then a hypothesis compatible with both the imaging and neuropathological lines of evidence is possible.

OUTLINES OF A UNIFYING HYPOTHESIS: SCHIZOPHRENIA AS A PROGRESSIVE DEVELOPMENTAL DISORDER

In light of the previous discussion, a unifying pathogenetic theory for schizophrenia that is consistent with all of the data requires a process that 1) begins prenatally; 2) progresses until it reaches a critical threshold, typically in the second or third decade; 3) causes progressive brain volume loss at a rate that is maximal in the first two decades and slows down with age; and 4) does not cause persistent gliosis (at least as detectable with current methods). Because the neuropathological data are contradictory as to whether there is actually cortical neuronal loss (10, 12) or simply atrophy (21, 82) and because atrophy (i.e., neuritic pruning) could be either pathological in itself or a manifestation of compensatory neural plasticity (83), several variants of a model satisfying these criteria are presented.

One possible pathogenetic model is that of excessive neuronal apoptosis. Cell death, including neuronal death, can occur by two morphologically distinguishable mechanisms, necrosis and apoptosis (84, 85). Necrosis in the central nervous system leads to inflammatory changes and gliosis, while apoptosis, which does not cause inflammation, occurs normally in early development and, at least in certain regions, with aging (43, 86).

Apoptosis can also be pathological, arising either as a normal response to brain injury of many types or as the result of an intrinsic defect in its own regulation (85). Many pathogenetic stimuli can trigger either necrosis or apoptosis, and the outcome seems to depend on the intensity of the stimulus (87). Exogenous insults that cause apoptosis of neurons may or may not also cause proliferation (i.e., gliosis) and/or apoptosis of astrocytes (88). Pathological neuronal apoptosis resulting from a genetically determined intrinsic regulatory defect has been convincingly demonstrated in the nematode Caenorhabditis elegans (89), and the excessive neuronal death in this model, which results from a defect in the ced-9 gene, can be partially blocked by expression of the homologous human gene bcl-2 (90). Moreover, “gene knockout” mice homozygously deficient in bcl-2 show a substantial excess of postnatal degeneration of peripheral sensory, sympathetic, and motor neurons (33% less of the last type than found in control subjects by day 44 [91]), while knockout mice deficient in the pro-apoptotic gene bax show a 51% excess of neurons in the facial nucleus (92).

Thus, it seems reasonable to suppose that defects in bcl-2,bax, other pro- and antiapoptotic genes in the bcl-2 family and in other apoptotic regulatory genes may also occur in humans and result in excessive (or deficient) neuronal apoptosis. What is harder to know, however, is whether or not excessive neuronal loss by this mechanism would necessarily be accompanied by glial proliferation. If not, even widespread neuronal loss would be difficult to detect neuropathologically owing to the rapidity of the apoptotic process, which for a single cell takes about 1 hour from onset of observable morphological changes to removal of any detectable residuum (93). Moreover, if postnatal pathological neuronal loss can result from nongliotic apoptosis rather than necrosis, the absence of gliosis would no longer limit the time of occurrence of that loss to early in gestation, and since apoptosis may be much more pronounced in one neuronal system than another (85), the major brunt of the deficit could be restricted to a single anatomically distributed system.

Such nongliotic pathological apoptosis in schizophrenia has recently been suggested by Akbarian et al. (94) as a possible cause of abnormalities in distribution and number of subcortical plate neurons (albeit not cortical neurons) in brains of schizophrenic persons. That study showed normal numbers of cortical neurons but too few subcortical plate neurons in the white matter just below the cortex and too many in the deeper white matter. Because the number of cortical neurons was normal, it was inferred that the problem causing the abnormalities in subcortical plate neurons was not one of cellular migration, as had been postulated in earlier papers (95,96), but rather aberrant apoptosis. Interestingly, although the published statistical analysis of the data was limited to showing that the subcortical plate neuronal distribution was abnormal, the accompanying numerical data and figure make it clear that there has also been an excessive overall loss of subplate neurons. Furthermore, there was no evidence of excessive gliosis in the affected areas, even though subplate neuronal apoptosis does not normally begin until late in the third trimester (77, p. 70). This is an important negative finding because the fetal brain can react with gliosis as early as the 20th week of gestation (25) and certainly throughout the third trimester (26).

Aberrant apoptosis is not the only possible progressive neurodevelopmental pathogenetic mechanism for schizophrenia. Two groups (21, 82) have found neuropathological evidence of increased neuronal density without cell loss (i.e., neuronal atrophy) in the cortex of schizophrenic persons, suggesting that the basic pathogenetic mechanism may be reflected not in neuronal loss but in greater than normal neurite pruning. (Interestingly, in the bcl-2-deficient mice mentioned earlier, many of the neurons remaining in the facial nucleus were shrunken [91].) Moreover, since pruning is related to both plasticity and learning (83), it is quite possible that some or all of the excessive neuronal atrophy in schizophrenia might not be pathological in itself, but rather the result of attempted compensation for an ongoing pathological process, be it aberrant apoptosis or pruning. (The initial suggestion that pruning might be pathological in schizophrenia [49] was nonspecific as to whether the problem was too much pruning, too little pruning, or abnormal pruning. However, observations of generalized cortical neuronal atrophy in schizophrenia [][21, 82] seem to exclude too little pruning as the cause.)

Since several MRI studies of brain changes normally occurring in childhood and adolescence show a rate of gray matter volume loss that is maximal beginning at about age 5 and then decreases asymptotically during adolescence (9799), and since this volume change is believed to be due to neuronal pruning and not cell loss (83), one can conclude that the rate of normal pruning gradually declines during childhood and adolescence. If pathological pruning were quantitatively greater but followed a similar asymptotic curve, the rate of loss might appear to plateau soon after crossing a threshold and producing overt illness. Figure 1 illustrates this by showing idealized patterns of cortical volume loss seen with normal age-related pruning, with hypothesized pathological pruning, and with a prototypical neuronal degenerative disorder in which the rate of neuronal loss is maximal at the time of onset of overt symptoms.

If the characterization of schizophrenia as a progressive neurodevelopmental disorder is correct, it is instructive to compare and contrast it to two other exemplars of this class of disorders, Rett’s syndrome and autism. Rett’s syndrome is a rare sex-linked disorder with marked female preponderance, in which neurological function is normal at birth but then deteriorates, initially rapidly and then more slowly until a plateau of rather severe disability is reached at about age 4 (100). Most patients then survive well into adult life without any further worsening unless medical complications supervene. Brain and head size are normal at birth, but then brain growth lags severely behind what is expected, until by age 4 brain and head size stabilize at about 75% of normal size (101). It is believed that most of the lag in brain growth during this period is due to a lack of neuritic proliferation (100). Neuropathologically, there are no inflammatory changes and no gliosis (102), but there are subtle changes indicative of prenatal onset. The disorder is thought to be of genetic origin, but it does not fit any classic pattern of inheritance (100).

MRI volumetry in Rett’s syndrome shows a pattern of almost purely early volume loss. In a study of 20 Rett’s syndrome patients and 20 matched comparison subjects (mean ages, 9.7 and 9.0 years, respectively) the patients’ mean brain tissue volume was 297 cc less than that of the comparison subjects, and the mean extracerebral and ventricular CSF volumes were, respectively, 8.5 cc and 3.2 cc greater than those of the comparison subjects (101), meaning that 96% of the tissue loss is accounted for by a decrease in intracranial volume. This study was cross-sectional, and most of the patients were already beyond the active stage of the disorder, but serial measurements of head size make it clear that head size in Rett’s syndrome is normal at birth and the rate of growth begins to lag at about 2 months of age (103).

Autism also is marked by apparent normality at birth and often up to 2 years of age, at which time there is an arrest or even regression of speech function and social interaction, but, unlike the case in Rett’s syndrome, no major impairment of motor function. The disorder gradually stabilizes over the next several years, although there may be an impressive development of certain cognitive skills. In direct contrast to Rett’s syndrome, there is in a number of cases actually excessive brain volume or weight (104, 105). Neuropathological examinations of a limited number of brains from autistic patients show variable degrees of developmental abnormalities and localized increases in neuronal density but no generalized gliosis (105,106). Autism, like Rett’s syndrome, is thought to be genetic in origin, but it too does not fit any classic pattern of inheritance (107).

MRI volumetry in autism shows a distinct pattern of early volume gain. In one study (104), the patients’ mean brain tissue volume was 84.6 cc greater, and the extracerebral and ventricular CSF volumes were 6.8 cc and 7.7 cc greater, respectively, than those of comparison subjects, indicating that 85% of the gain in intracranial volume is accounted for by brain tissue growth. Here too the MRI study was cross-sectional, but serial head measurements again demonstrate normal head size at birth and subsequent excessive growth in a large subset (37%) of autistic individuals (108).

The important parallels suggesting that Rett’s syndrome, autism, and schizophrenia are all progressive neurodevelopmental disorders are threefold: first, all are characterized by deterioration from previous levels of functioning; second, all show a leveling off rather than a lifelong progression of clinical deterioration; and finally, all share an absence of reactive gliosis on postmortem neuropathological examination. The important differences, suggesting that each is a distinct member of a common class of disorders, are seen not only in the characteristic clinical manifestations but in three very different patterns of abnormality on MRI volumetry: pure early loss in Rett’s syndrome, pure early gain in autism, and either primarily late (62) or mixed early and late (52, 61) loss in schizophrenia.

CONCLUSIONS

If it is granted that there is excessive nonprogressive brain volume loss in schizophrenia, and if it is also assumed that absence of gliosis excludes all possible causes of postnatal brain volume loss, then a pathogenetic model of an early, static defect is both necessary and sufficient to account for the imaging and neuropathological evidence, but if there are pathogenetic mechanisms (i.e., apoptosis or neuritic pruning) that can produce excessive postnatal brain volume loss without gliosis, then absence of gliosis does not constrain our assumptions about either the time of onset or the duration of the observed volume loss, and a model of an early, static neurodevelopmental defect in schizophrenia is not necessary. Furthermore, if excessive brain volume loss has occurred (or continues to occur) after intracranial volume expansion is complete, a neurodevelopmental model of schizophrenia specifying an early, static defect is not sufficient. This conclusion does not depend on the disputed finding that measurable progression of excessive volume loss is observable with imaging after onset of overt clinical illness, and it does not require rejection of the possibility that the pathogenetic mechanism starts to exert its effects in the pre- or perinatal time period.

The alternative proposal that schizophrenia is a progressive developmental disorder does postulate that developmental mechanisms such as apoptosis and pruning can continue to go awry over many years without resulting in excessive gliosis, but if that assumption is made then the model readily accounts for both the neuropathological findings implicating prenatal pathology and the imaging findings of excessive extracerebral CSF space that must have resulted from brain volume loss occurring after brain volume expansion was complete.

Received May 1, 1997; revisions received Dec. 18, 1997, and June 8, 1998; accepted June 11, 1998. From the Departments of Medicine and Psychiatry, Texas A&M University School of Medicine and Central Texas Veterans Health Care System. Address reprint requests to Dr. Woods, Neurology Section, Central Texas Veterans Health Care System, 1901 South 1st St., Temple, TX 76502; (e-mail).

References

1. Miller R: Schizophrenia as a progressive disorder: relations to EEG, CT, neuropathological and other evidence. Prog Neurobiol 1989; 33:17–44Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

2. Keshavan MS, Anderson S, Pettegrew JW: Is schizophrenia due to excessive synaptic pruning in the prefrontal cortex? the Feinberg hypothesis revisited. J Psychiatr Res 1994; 28:239–265Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

3. DeLisi LE: Is schizophrenia a lifetime disorder of brain plasticity, growth and aging? Schizophr Res 1997; 23:119–129Google Scholar

4. Crow TJ: Schizophrenia as failure of hemispheric dominance for language. Trends Neurosci 1997; 20:339–343Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

5. Bruton CJ, Crow TJ, Frith CD, Johnstone EC, Owens DG, Roberts GW: Schizophrenia and the brain: a prospective clinico-neuropathological study. Psychol Med 1990; 20:285–304Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

6. Arnold SE, Trojanowski JQ: Recent advances in defining the neuropathology of schizophrenia. Acta Neuropathol 1996; 92:217–231Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

7. Woods BT, Wolf J: A reconsideration of the relation of ventricular enlargement to duration of illness in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 1983; 140:1564–1570LinkGoogle Scholar

8. Johnstone EC, Crow TJ, Frith CD, Husband J, Kreel L: Cerebral ventricular size and cognitive impairment in chronic schizophrenia. Lancet 1976; 2:924–926Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

9. Weinberger DR: Computed tomography (CT) findings in schizophrenia: speculation on the meaning of it all. J Psychiatr Res 1984; 18:477–490Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

10. Bogerts B, Hantsch J, Herzer M: A morphometric study of the dopamine-containing cell groups in the mesencephalon of normals, Parkinson patients, and schizophrenics. Biol Psychiatry 1983; 18:951–969MedlineGoogle Scholar

11. Roberts GW, Colter N, Lofthouse R, Bogerts B, Zech M, Crow TJ: Gliosis in schizophrenia: a survey. Biol Psychiatry 1986; 21:1043–1050Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

12. Benes FM, Davidson J, Bird ED: Quantitative cytoarchitectural studies of the cerebral cortex of schizophrenics. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1986; 43:31–35Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

13. Roberts GW, Colter N, Lofthouse R, Johnstone EC, Crow TJ: Is there gliosis in schizophrenia? investigation of the temporal lobe. Biol Psychiatry 1987; 22:1459–1468Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

14. Weinberger DR: Implications of normal brain development for the pathogenesis of schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1987; 44:660–669Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

15. Oppenheimer DR: Diseases of the basal ganglia, cerebellum, and motor neurons, in Greenfield’s Neuropathology, 4th ed. Edited by Adams JH, Corsellis JAN, Duchen LW. New York, John Wiley & Sons, 1984, pp 699–747Google Scholar

16. Stevens CD, Altshuler LL, Bogerts B, Falkai P: Quantitative study of gliosis in schizophrenia and Huntington’s chorea. Biol Psychiatry 1988; 24:697–700Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

17. Falkai P, Bogerts B, Rozumek M: Limbic pathology in schizophrenia: the entorhinal region—a morphometric study. Biol Psychiatry 1988; 24:515–521Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

18. Pakkenberg B: Pronounced reduction of total neuron numbers in mediodorsal thalamic nucleus and nucleus accumbens in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1990; 47:1023–1028Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

19. Nieto D, Escobar A: Major psychoses, in Pathology of the Nervous System. Edited by Minkler J. New York, McGraw-Hill, 1972, pp 2654–2665Google Scholar

20. Stevens JR: Neuropathology of schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1982; 39:1131–1139Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

21. Selemon LD, Rajkowska G, Goldman-Rakic PS: Abnormally high neuronal density in the schizophrenic cortex: a morphometric analysis of prefrontal area 9 and occipital area 17. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1995; 52:805–818Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

22. Cannon TD, Mednick SA, Parnas J, Schulsinger F, Praes­t­holm J, Vestergaard A: Developmental brain abnormalities in the offspring of schizophrenic mothers, I: contributions of genetic and perinatal factors. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1993; 50:551–564Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

23. Marcus J, Hans S, Auerbach JG, Auerbach AG: Children at risk for schizophrenia: the Jerusalem infant development study. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1993; 50:797–809Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

24. Geddes JR, Lawrie SM: Obstetric complications and schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Br J Psychiatry 1995; 167:786–793Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

25. Roessmann U, Gambetti P: Pathological reaction of astrocytes in perinatal brain injury: an immunohistochemical study. Acta Neuropathol 1986; 70:302–307Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

26. Roberts GW: Schizophrenia: a neuropathological perspective. Br J Psychiatry 1991; 158:8–17Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

27. Nasrallah HA, Olson SC, McCalley-Whitters M, Chapman S, Jacoby CG: Cerebral ventricular enlargement in schizophrenia: a preliminary follow-up study. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1986; 43:157–159Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

28. Illowsky BP, Juliano DM, Bigelow LB, Weinberger DR: Stability of CT scan findings: results of an 8-year follow-up study. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1988; 51:209–213Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

29. Vita A, Sacchetti E, Valvasori G, Cazzullo CL: Brain morphology in schizophrenia: a 2- to 5-year follow-up study. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1988; 78:618–621Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

30. Hoffman WF, Ballard L, Turner EH, Casey DE: Three-year follow-up of older schizophrenics: extrapyramidal syndromes, psychiatric symptoms, and ventricular brain ratio. Biol Psychiatry 1991; 30:913–926Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

31. Sponheim SR, Iocono WG, Beiser M: Stability of ventricular size after the onset of psychosis in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res Neuroimaging 1991; 40:21–29Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

32. Kemali D, Maj M, Galderisi S, Milici N, Salvati A: Ventricle-to-brain ratio in schizophrenia: a controlled follow-up study. Biol Psychiatry 1989; 26:753–756Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

33. Woods BT, Yurgelun-Todd D, Benes FM, Frankenburg FR, Pope HG Jr, McSparrow J: Progressive ventricular enlargement in schizophrenia: comparison to bipolar affective disorder and correlation with clinical course. Biol Psychiatry 1990; 27:341–352Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

34. Davis KL, Buchsbaum MS, Shihabuddin L, Spiegel-Cohen J, Metzger M, Frecska E, Keefe RS, Powchik P: Ventricular enlargement in poor-outcome schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 1998; 43:783–793Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

35. DeGreef G, Ashtari M, Bogerts B, Bilder RM, Jody DN, Alvir JMJ, Lieberman JA: Volumes of ventricular system subdivisions measured from magnetic resonance images in first-episode schizophrenic patients. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1992; 49:531–537Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

36. Jaskiw GE, Juliano DM, Goldberg TE, Hertzman M, Urow-Hamell E, Weinberger DR: Cerebral ventricular enlargement in schizophreniform disorder does not progress: a seven year follow-up study. Schizophr Res 1994; 14:23–28Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

37. Vita A, Giobbio GM, Dieci M, Garbarini M, Morganti C, Comazzi M, Inverniizzi G: Stability of cerebroventricular size from the appearance of the first psychotic symptoms to the later diagnosis of schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 1994; 35:960–962Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

38. DeLisi LE, Tew W, Shu-hong X, Hoff AL, Sakuma M, Kushner M, Lee G, Shedlack K, Smith AM, Grimson R: A prospective follow-up study of brain morphology and cognition in first-episode schizophrenic patients: preliminary findings. Biol Psychiatry 1995; 38:349–360Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

39. Gur RE, Cowell P, Turetsky BI, Gallacher F, Cannon T, Bilker W, Gur RC: A follow-up magnetic resonance imaging study of schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1998; 55:145–152Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

40. DeLisi LE, Sakuma M, Tew W, Kushner M, Hoff AL, Grimson R: Schizophrenia as a chronic active brain process: a study of progressive brain structural change subsequent to the onset of schizophrenia. Psychiatr Res 1997; 74:129–140Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

41. Rapoport JL, Giedd JN, Kumra S, Jacobsen L, Smith A, Lee P, Nelson J, Hamburger SD: Childhood onset schizophrenia: progressive ventricular change during adolescence. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1997; 54:897–903Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

42. Pakkenberg B, Moller A, Gundersen HJG, Mouritzen Dam A: The absolute number of nerve cells in substantia nigra in normal subjects and in patients with Parkinson’s disease estimated with an unbiased stereological method. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1991; 54:30–33Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

43. Anglade P, Vyas S, Hirsch EC, Agid Y: Apoptosis in dopaminergic neurons of the human substantia nigra during normal aging. Histol Histopathol 1997; 12:603–610MedlineGoogle Scholar

44. Fearnley JM, Lees AJ: Ageing and Parkinson’s disease: substantia nigra regional selectivity. Brain 1991; 114:2283–2301Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

45. Jones P, Murray RM: The genetics of schizophrenia is the genetics of neurodevelopment. Br J Psychiatry 1991; 158:615–623Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

46. Weinberger DR: From neuropathology to neurodevelopment. Lancet 1995; 346:552–557Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

47. Murray RM, Lewis SW: Is schizophrenia a neurodevelopmental disorder? BMJ 1987; 295:681–682Google Scholar

48. Benes FM: The role of stress and dopamine-GABA interactions in the vulnerability for schizophrenia. J Psychiatr Res 1997; 31:257–275Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

49. Feinberg I: Schizophrenia: caused by a fault in programmed synaptic elimination during adolescence? J Psychiatr Res 1982/1983; 17:319–334Google Scholar

50. Andreasen NC, Flashman L, Flaum M, Arndt S, Swayze V II, O"Leary DS, Ehrhardt JC, Yuh WTC: Regional brain abnormalities in schizophrenia measured with magnetic resonance imaging. JAMA 1994; 272:1763–1769Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

51. Gur RE, Mozley PD, Shtasel DL, Cannon TD, Gallacher F, Turetsky B, Grossman R, Gur RC: Clinical subtypes of schizophrenia: differences in brain and CSF volume. Am J Psychiatry 1994; 151:343–350LinkGoogle Scholar

52. Woods BT, Yurgelun-Todd D: Brain volume loss in schizophrenia: when does it occur and is it progressive? Schizophr Res 1991; 5:202–204Google Scholar

53. Zipursky RB, Lim KO, Sullivan EV, Brown BW, Pfefferbaum A: Widespread cerebral gray matter volume deficits in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1992; 49:195–205Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

54. Lim KO, Harris D, Beal M, Hoff AL, Minn K, Csernansky JG, Faustman WO, Marsh L, Sullivan EV, Pfefferbaum A: Gray matter deficits in young onset schizophrenia are independent of age of onset. Biol Psychiatry 1996; 40:4–13Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

55. Jernigan TL, Zisook S, Heaton RK, Moranville JT, Hesselink JR, Braff DL: Magnetic resonance imaging abnormalities in lenticular nuclei and cerebral cortex in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1991; 48:881–890Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

56. Gur RE, Mozley D, Resnick SM, Shtasel D, Kohn M, Zimmerman R, Herman G, Atlas S, Grossman R, Erwin R, Gur RC: Magnetic resonance imaging in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1991; 48:407–412Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

57. Harvey I, Ron MA, Du Boulay G, Wicks D, Lewis SW, Murray RM: Reduction of cortical volume in schizophrenia on magnetic resonance imaging. Psychol Med 1993; 23:591–604Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

58. DeLisi LE, Hoff AL, Schwartz JE, Shields GW, Halthore SN, Gupta SM, Henn FA, Anand AK: Brain morphology in first-episode schizophrenic-like psychotic patients: a quantitative magnetic resonance imaging study. Biol Psychiatry 1991; 29:159–175Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

59. Shenton ME, Kikinis R, Jolesz FA, Pollak SD, LeMay M, Wible CG, Hokama H, Martin J, Metcalf D, Coleman M, McCarley RW: Abnormalities of the left temporal lobe and thought disorder in schizophrenia: a quantitative magnetic resonance imaging study. N Engl J Med 1992; 327:604–612Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

60. Wible CG, Shenton ME, Hokama H, Kikinis R, Jolesz FA, Metcalf D, McCarley RW: Prefrontal cortex and schizophrenia: a quantitative magnetic resonance imaging study. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1995; 52:279–288Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

61. Woods BT, Yurgelun-Todd D, Goldstein J, Seidman LJ, Tsuang MT: MRI brain abnormalities in chronic schizophrenia: one process or more? Biol Psychiatry 1996; 40:585–596Google Scholar

62. Nopoulos P, Torres I, Flaum M, Andreasen NC, Ehrhardt JC, Yuh WTC: Brain morphology in first-episode schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 1995; 152:1721–1723LinkGoogle Scholar

63. Cannon TD, Mednick SA, Parnas J, Schulsinger F, Praes­t­holm J, Vestergaard A: Developmental brain abnormalities in the offspring of schizophrenic mothers, II: structural brain characteristics of schizophrenia and schizotypal personality disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1993; 51:955–962CrossrefGoogle Scholar

64. Ward KE, Friedman L, Wise A, Schulz SC: Meta-analysis of brain and cranial size in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 1996; 22:197–213Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

65. Galaburda AM: Neurology of developmental dyslexia. Curr Opin Neurobiol 1993; 3:237–242Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

66. Bilder RM, Wu H, Bogerts B, Degreef G, Ashtari M, Alvir JMJ, Snyder PJ, Lieberman JA: Absence of regional hemispheric volume asymmetries in first-episode schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 1994; 151:1437–1447LinkGoogle Scholar

67. Benes FM: Myelination of cortical-hippocampal relays during late adolescence. Schizophr Bull 1989; 15:585–593Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

68. Lipska BK, Swerdlow NR, Geyer MA, Jaskiw GE, Braff DL, Weinberger DR: Neonatal excitotoxic hippocampal damage in rats causes post-pubertal changes in prepulse inhibition of startle and its disruption by apomorphine. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1995; 122:35–43Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

69. Lipska BK, Weinberger DR: Genetic variation in vulnerability to the behavioral effects of neonatal hippocampal damage in rats. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1995; 92:8906–8910Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

70. Sams-Dodd F, Lipska BK, Weinberger DR: Neonatal lesions of the rat ventral hippocampus result in hyperlocomotion and deficits in social behavior in adulthood. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1997; 132:303–310Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

71. Olney JW, Farber NB: Glutamate receptor dysfunction and schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1995; 52:998–1007Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

72. Goldman PS: Functional development of the prefrontal cortex in early life and the problem of neuronal plasticity. Exp Neurol 1971; 32:366–387Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

73. Goldman-Rakic PM, Isseroff A, Schwartz ML, Bugbee NM: The neurobiology of cognitive development, in Handbook of Child Psychiatry, vol 2: Infancy and Developmental Psychology. Edited by Haith MM, Campos JJ. New York, John Wiley & Sons, 1983, pp 281–344Google Scholar

74. Goldman PS: Recovery of function after CNS lesions in infant monkeys. Neurosci Res Prog Bull 1974; 12:217–222MedlineGoogle Scholar

75. Kennard MA: Cortical reorganization of motor function. Arch Neurol 1942; 48:227–240CrossrefGoogle Scholar

76. Basser LS: Hemiplegia of early onset and the faculty of speech with special reference to the effects of hemispherectomy. Brain 1962; 85:427–460Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

77. Volpe JJ: Neurology of the Newborn, 3rd ed. Philadelphia, WB Saunders, 1995Google Scholar

78. Hebb DO: The effect of early and late brain injury upon test scores and the nature of normal adult intelligence. Proceedings of the American Philosophical Society 1942; 85:275–292Google Scholar

79. Woods BT, Teuber H-L: Early onset of complementary specialization of cerebral hemispheres in man. Trans Am Neurol Assoc 1973; 98:59–63Google Scholar

80. Schneider GE: Is it really better to have your brain lesion early? a revision of the “Kennard principle.” Neuropsychologia 1979; 17:557–583Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

81. Kolb B, Whishaw IQ: Plasticity in the neocortex: mechanisms underlying recovery from early brain damage. Prog Neurobiol 1989; 32:235–276Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

82. Pakkenberg B: Total nerve cell number in neocortex in chronic schizophrenics and controls estimated using optical disectors. Biol Psychiatry 1993; 34:768–772Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

83. Huttenlocher PR: Morphometric study of human cerebral cortex development. Neuropsychologia 1990; 28:517–527Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

84. Bredesen DE: Neural apoptosis. Ann Neurol 1995; 38:839–851Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

85. Vaux DL: Toward an understanding of the molecular mechanisms of physiological cell death. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1993; 90:786–789Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

86. McGeer PL, McGeer EG, Suzuki JS: Aging and extrapyramidal function. Arch Neurol 1977; 34:33–35Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

87. Portera-Cailliau C, Price DL, Martin LJ: Excitotoxic neuronal death in the immature brain is an apoptosis-necrosis morphological continuum. J Comp Neurology 1997; 378:70–87MedlineGoogle Scholar

88. Thomas LB, Gates DJ, Richfield EK, O’Brien TF, Schweitzer JB, Steindler DA: DNA end labeling (TUNEL) in Huntington’s disease and other neuropathological conditions. Exp Neurol 1995; 133:265–272Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

89. Hengartner MO, Ellis RE, Horvitz HR: Caenorhabditis elegans gene ced-9 protects cells from programmed cell death. Nature 1992; 356:494–499Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

90. Stellar H: Mechanisms and genes of cellular suicide. Science 1995; 267:1445–1449Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

91. Michaelidis TM, Sendtner M, Cooper JD, Airaksinen MS, Holtmann B, Meyer M, Thoenen H: Inactivation of bcl-2 results in progressive degeneration of motoneurons, sympathetic and sensory neurons during early postnatal development. Neuron 1996; 17:78–89CrossrefGoogle Scholar

92. Deckwerth TL, Elliott JL, Knudson CM, Johnson EM, Snider WD, Korsmeyer SJ: BAX is required for neuronal death after trophic factor deprivation and during development. Neuron 1996; 17:401–411Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

93. Raff MC, Barres BA, Burne JF, Coles HS, Ishizaki Y, Jacobson MD: Programmed cell death and the control of cell survival: lessons from the nervous system. Science 1993; 262:695–700Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

94. Akbarian S, Kim JJ, Potkin SG, Herrick WP, Bunney WE Jr, Jones EG: Maldistribution of interstitial neurons in prefrontal white matter of the brains of schizophrenic patients. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1996; 53:425–436Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

95. Akbarian S, Bunney WE Jr, Potkin SG, Wigal SB, Hagman JO, Sandman CA, Jones EG: Altered distribution of nicotinamide-adenine dinucleotide phosphate-diaphorase cells in frontal lobe of schizophrenics implies disturbances of cortical development. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1993; 50:169–177Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

96. Akbarian S, Vinuela A, Kim JJ, Potkin SG, Bunney WE Jr, Jones EG: Distorted distribution of nicotinamide-adenine dinucleotide phosphate-diaphorase neurons in temporal lobe of schizophrenics implies anomalous cortical development. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1993; 50:178–187Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

97. Pfefferbaum A, Mathalon DH, Sullivan EV, Rawles JM, Zipursky RB, Lim KO: A quantitative magnetic resonance imaging study of changes in brain morphology from infancy to late adulthood. Arch Neurol 1994; 51:874–887Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

98. Giedd JN, Snell JW, Lange N, Rajapakse JC, Casey BJ, Kozuch PL, Vaituzis AC, Vauss YC, Hamburger SD, Kaysen D, Rapoport JL: Quantitative magnetic resonance imaging of human brain development: ages 4–18. Cereb Cortex 1996; 6:551–560Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

99. Jernigan TL, Trauner DA, Hesselink JR, Tallal PA: Maturation of human cerebrum observed in vivo during adolescence. Brain 1991; 114:2037–2049Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

100. Naidu S: Rett syndrome: a disorder affecting early brain growth. Ann Neurol 1997; 42:3–10Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

101. Subramaniam B, Naidu S, Reiss AL: Neuroanatomy in Rett syndrome: cerebral cortex and posterior fossa. Ann Neurol 1997; 48:399–407CrossrefGoogle Scholar

102. Bauman ML, Kemper TL, Arin DM: Pervasive neuroanatomic abnormalities of the brain in three cases of Rett’s syndrome. Neurology 1995; 45:1581–1586Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

103. Naidu S, Hyman S, Harris EL, Narayanan V, Johns D, Castora F: Rett syndrome: studies of natural history and search for a genetic marker. Neuropediatrics 1995; 26:63–66Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

104. Piven J, Arndt S, Bailey J, Havercamp S, Andreasen NC, Palmer P: An MRI study of brain size in autism. Am J Psychiatry 1995; 152:1145–1149LinkGoogle Scholar

105. Bailey A, Luthert P, Dean A, Harding B, Janota I, Montgomery M, Rutter M, Lantos P: A clinicopathological study of autism. Brain 1998; 121:889–905Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

106. Bauman ML: Neuropathology of autism, in Movement Disorders in Neurology and Neuropsychiatry. Edited by Joseph AB, Young RR. Boston, Blackwell Scientific, 1992, pp 662–666Google Scholar

107. Folstein SE, Piven J: Etiology of autism: genetic influences. Pediatrics 1991; 87(May suppl):767–773Google Scholar

108. Lainhart JE, Piven J, Wzorek M, Landa R, Santangelo SL, Coon H, Folstein SE: Macrocephaly in children and adults with autism. J Am Acad Child Adol Psychiatry 1997; 36:282–290Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar