The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×

Abstract

OBJECTIVE: Deficits in emotion processing are a hallmark of schizophrenia, with consequences for social functioning and subjective well-being. However, their specificity and characteristics have not been ascertained psychometrically. The authors’ purpose was to examine a differential deficit for processing emotional facets of the face compared to judgment of nonemotional features (age) and facial memory. The authors also sought to establish whether the deficit affects sensitivity or specificity of performance. METHOD: Participants were 20 patients with schizophrenia and 20 healthy subjects matched for age, gender, and parental education. The authors examined emotional discrimination abilities compared to age discrimination and recognition memory for faces with standardized faces displaying the universal emotions of happiness, sadness, anger, and fear. Percent correct in each condition and for each emotion were assessed as well as sensitivity (correct identification of a target emotion) and specificity (correct rejection of a nontarget emotion) for emotion recognition. RESULTS: Patients with schizophrenia were differentially impaired in the discrimination of emotional aspects of facial expressions compared to nonemotional aspects and memory. Within the emotional task, patients showed differential impairment in specificity and insensitivity to the emotion displayed. CONCLUSIONS: When identical stimuli were used across tasks, differential impairment was seen in patients with schizophrenia for processing emotional faces, although the nonemotional task proved harder for both groups. Impairment in the specificity of emotion identification may lead to misunderstanding of social communication and may underlie difficulties in social adjustment experienced by people with schizophrenia. Emotion discrimination tests could augment the neurobehavioral evaluation of patients.

Although emotion processing deficits have been implicated in schizophrenia since its original description (1), with symptoms including a “flat” and “inappropriate” affect, the extent and nature of emotion processing performance in schizophrenia are still unclear. Several studies have demonstrated that patients with schizophrenia have deficits in the processing of emotional facial expressions (2, 3), and these have included patients from a wide variety of cultural backgrounds (4). The deficits seem to affect mainly the ability to name and discriminate expressions (5, 6). Furthermore, performance correlates with symptom severity (79), which indicates that deficits in discriminating emotional aspects of facial expressions have clinical significance. Finally, functional imaging studies have shown abnormal activation in patients with schizophrenia in emotion processing tasks (10, 11). A comparison between effects of behavioral studies and functional imaging studies is made difficult by the differing nature of tasks. Behavioral studies use complex emotion discrimination tasks, whereas functional imaging studies use simple response modalities because of technical limitations and the need to probe specific neural systems. Few tasks are available that have been used in psychometric assessment and in functional imaging.

Notwithstanding the increasing evidence for emotion processing deficits in schizophrenia, controversy still exists as to whether a differential deficit can be established for processing emotion compared to nonemotional features (7, 12–17). Notably, neutral and happy faces showed different scan path patterns in patients (18). However, studies varied in the extent to which emotion discrimination and control tasks were equated, and the issue has been further complicated by using varying ranges of emotions and intensities. A differential deficit has been reported in recognition accuracy of patients for disgusted, fearful, and neutral expressions (8). However, different emotions were intermixed, and therefore, specificity of emotion discrimination could be based on only neutral expressions (8). Such a design does not permit powerful separation between deficits related to sensitivity and specificity.

In the present study, we sought to examine the ability of patients with schizophrenia, compared to healthy individuals, to discriminate emotional facial expressions. We used the same facial stimuli to examine performance on two nonemotional domains: age discrimination and recognition memory. Studies examining episodic memory in schizophrenia (1921) have consistently indicated deficits (22), including for face memory (23). Age discrimination has been used as a control task in earlier studies (7, 1011, 24–26). We aimed at comparing such deficits in nonemotional aspects of face processing to the deficit in identifying emotional expressions. An additional aim was to examine whether the deficit in facial emotion processing has specific performance characteristics with respect to sensitivity (being able to correctly identify a target emotion) and specificity (being able to correctly reject an expression that is not of the target emotion). Therefore, we used a blocked design in which the target emotion is different for each block and specificity can be determined for each emotion separately. Finally, the task was designed so that it could be administered with functional imaging by using blocked and event-related analysis. To equate and simplify response, all tasks required binary responding on a two-button forced-choice key.

Method

Subjects

The study was performed on 20 (10 male, 10 female) inpatients with a DSM-IV diagnosis of schizophrenia and 20 (10 male, 10 female) healthy volunteers who were matched to the patients by gender, age, and years of parental education (within 2 years of the mean). After the procedure was fully explained, written informed consent was obtained from all subjects. The mean age of the patients was 37.7 years (SD=12.6). They had a mean parental education of 9.3 years (SD=3.5). The local institutional review board approved the protocol.

Symptom severity was assessed with the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (27). Mean ratings were 13.0 (SD=7.1) on the positive symptom scale and 16.9 (SD=7.7) on the negative symptom scale. All patients were taking antipsychotic medication, with 12 receiving atypicals, four taking typicals, and three taking both agents. One patient’s neuroleptic medication was unknown because he was participating in a study using double-blind psychopharmacology. Patients with any other psychiatric disorder or neurological illness were excluded. The patients were free of recreational drugs and alcohol, as ascertained by regular urine drug screenings.

The healthy volunteers were recruited through local advertisements, followed by a detailed screening. Inclusion was based on the absence of any neurological disease or psychiatric disorder, including current substance abuse. The mean age of the healthy comparison subjects was 37.2 years (SD=11.4), and their mean parental education was 9.9 years (SD=3.0). A two-tailed comparison of means confirmed that there were no differences between the patients and the healthy participants in age (t=0.79, df=19, n.s.) or parental education (t=–0.37, df=19, n.s.).

Procedure

The participants were presented a modified version of the Facial Emotions for Brain Activation Test (25) with the PRESENTATION software package (Neurobehavioral Systems Inc., San Francisco). The stimuli consist of color photographs of actors and actresses of various ethnicities (African American, Caucasian, Asian, and Hispanic). The detailed task construction is reported elsewhere (25). Briefly, an original pool of photographs was presented to a group of raters (N=64) who judged the displayed emotion and the level of intensity of each picture. Pictures with the highest degree of identification accuracy were selected. In the emotion discrimination task, the photographs express either one of four different emotions (happiness, sadness, anger, fear) or no emotion (neutral). Disgust has been left out because it was difficult to judge for both patients and comparison subjects (8), perhaps because of ambiguous and equivocal facial expressions.

Task

The discrimination task consisted of four runs, one run for each emotion. The order of runs was randomly assigned across subjects. Each run consisted of 120 faces (32 targets, 32 nontargets, and 56 neutral targets). The presentation order was fixed within each run, but reaction side (a left or right button press for the correct answer) was randomly assigned across subjects, with each single stimulus presented for 3 seconds without an interstimulus interval. Within each run, blocks of face presentations and crosshairs (baseline) alternated. Each face presentation phase lasted 90 seconds, each crosshair phase was shown for 24 seconds. A 90-second block, while lengthier than needed for blocked analyses, was chosen to permit subsequent event-related (bottom-up) analysis of cerebral correlates for specific emotions. The 32 nontarget emotions had a slightly unequal distribution (N=11, N=11, N=10), varying across trials and affecting all emotion categories comparably. This was needed to keep the number of presentations of the single faces (emotional as well as neutral) equal during the emotion discrimination, i.e., all targets were presented exactly five times before the facial recognition phase. The participants had to decide whether the presented face was showing the respective target emotion or any other emotion/no emotion. They responded by pressing the left or right control button on a computer keyboard.

There were two cognitive control tasks. The age-discrimination task consisted of 120 stimuli divided into four blocks, with face and crosshair presentations in alternating order. Sixty-four of the 120 faces were emotional, and 56 were neutral. Again, the stimuli were presented for 3 seconds each with no interstimulus intervals. In this task, the subjects were asked to decide, by pressing a button, whether the presented face was older or younger than 30 years. A similar control task was proven adequate and effective in earlier studies (2427). Finally, a facial recognition task was administered in which the participants were shown the same faces presented in the discrimination tasks beforehand. However, the facial expressions in this task were all neutral. As in the discrimination tasks, faces and crosshair phases were presented in alternating order with the memory blocks appearing for 108 seconds and the crosshair appearing for 24 seconds. In this case, the subjects had to judge for each face whether or not they had seen it in the discrimination task before by pressing the right or left control button, respectively. The procedure consisted of 144 faces, including 96 old stimuli that had been shown before (targets) and 48 new faces (nontargets).

Statistical Analysis

The dependent measure of performance in emotion discrimination for each emotion, age discrimination, and recognition was percent correct responses. To analyze performance in emotion discrimination in further detail, we computed sensitivity (true positives/[true positives plus false negatives]) and specificity (true negatives/[true negatives plus false positives]) for each emotion condition. The true positives included the number of targets correctly recognized in each run, whereas the true negatives consisted of the number of nontargets correctly classified as such. False positive errors for each run (emotion) were calculated as the number of times a nontarget face was rated as a target out of the total number of nontargets per run. False negative errors were the number of targets falsely categorized as nontargets. Sensitivity is defined as the probability of correctly identifying a target, whereas specificity refers to the probability of correct rejection of a nontarget.

Because there was an unequal number of emotion discrimination (four) and nonemotional tasks (two), we performed generalized estimating equations modeling (SAS Proc Genmod, SAS Institute, Cary, N.C.) rather than multivariate analysis of variance. The generalized estimating equation is also more robust against violation of the sphericity assumption. Diagnosis and sex were grouping variables, and we tested for the significance of a diagnosis-by-emotional (four emotions) versus nonemotional (age discrimination, facial recognition) interaction term (with performance defined as percent correct). To contrast sensitivity and specificity within the emotional tasks, the generalized estimating equation tested for a diagnosis by sensitivity versus specificity interaction term with group (patients versus comparison subjects) and gender as between-subject factors and emotion as a within-subject factor. For the detailed analysis of false positives, suggested by the significant interaction, we performed an analysis of variance (ANOVA) (SAS Proc GLM, Cary, N.C.) on the false positive errors, with diagnosis and sex as between-subject factors and emotion as a within-subject factor.

Results

Figure 1 shows the means of patients and comparison subjects for the four emotion discrimination tasks and the two cognitive tasks. As can be seen, both male and female patients were impaired across tasks. However, the difference appears more pronounced for the emotional tasks. Indeed, the generalized estimating equation revealed a significant group effect (χ2=15.18, df=1, p<0.0001) and an interaction of diagnosis × emotional versus nonemotional tasks (χ2=21.06, df=2, p<0.0001). No main effects or interactions with gender were significant. Post hoc Scheffé comparisons within the emotion tasks revealed significant group differences for anger (diffcrit=6.34), fear (diffcrit=7.07), happiness (diffcrit=6.10), and sadness (diffcrit=6.33). Group differences in the percentage of correct responses were also significant for the recognition task (F=8.46, df=1, 36, p=0.006) and the age discrimination task (F=7.38, df=1, 36, p=0.01) (see also Table 1).

As Chapman and Chapman (28) have proposed, significant diagnosis-by-measure interactions need to be scrutinized for level of difficulty and true score variance before we can safely conclude the presence of a differential deficit. Patients may show impairment on one task because it is more difficult and not because it taps a different ability. To evaluate task difficulty, we compared the percent correct of the age and facial recognition tasks with the emotion discrimination task in healthy comparison subjects. Contrary to the alternative explanation of task difficulty artifacts, both control tasks were more difficult than the emotion discrimination task: age discrimination task (t=4.86, df=19, p=0.0001), facial recognition test (t=6.63, df=19, p=0.0001). This makes the finding of greater deficits in emotion discrimination more prominent because patients were more impaired on the easier emotion discrimination task, whereas they were able to process a nonemotional aspect and to memorize faces at a level more similar to that of the comparison subjects.

When we compared sensitivity and specificity (Table 1, Figure 2), it appeared that patients showed rates of sensitivity similar to those of the comparison subjects for the four emotions but performed worse on the specificity measures. The generalized estimating equation showed a main effect of diagnostic group (χ2=8.87, df=1, p<0.003) and a diagnosis × sensitivity versus specificity interaction (χ2=22.12, df=2, p<0.0001). Post hoc comparisons of means confirmed a significant difference between patients and healthy volunteers in specificity for anger (diffcrit=0.09), fear (diffcrit=0.08), happiness (diffcrit=0.07), and sadness (diffcrit=0.08). We also found a marginal gender-by-emotion interaction for sensitivity (p=0.03). Post hoc (Scheffé) tests indicated that across groups, men discriminated angry faces with a significantly higher sensitivity than women (diffcrit=0.09).

To further decompose the specificity effect, we performed an ANOVA on the false positive errors. The group difference was significant (F=17.12, df=1, 36, p=0.0002), indicating more false positives in patients regardless of emotion (happiness diffcrit=0.08, sadness diffcrit=0.11, fear diffcrit=0.09, anger diffcrit=0.09). There were no other effects.

Discussion

Consistent with earlier studies (17, 29, 30), we found impaired emotion discrimination performance in patients with schizophrenia compared to healthy persons. We also found with the same stimuli that patients, compared to comparison subjects, are relatively less impaired in processing nonemotional features of the face, specifically age, and in face memory. It is noteworthy that patients were more impaired on the emotion discrimination task even though the nonemotional task was harder. This result further underscores the need to study emotion processing deficits in schizophrenia, particularly in view of their correlates with clinical severity and course, as found in previous studies (7, 8, 31).

By presenting the specific emotions in blocks and revealing the target emotion for each block (i.e., at the beginning of each block, the participants were informed about the target emotion to be identified), the present design also permitted an evaluation of whether patients are primarily impaired in sensitivity or specificity for recognizing emotions. The results indicated that under these conditions, specificity is impaired rather than sensitivity. Thus, the patients encountered more difficulties when they had to decide whether a nontarget represented the respective target emotion. Impaired specificity of emotion discrimination could have severe consequences for social functioning. Our results indicate that when patients with schizophrenia are seeking to identify a target emotion, they will tend to misread that emotion even when it was not present on the observed expression. Such errors could lead to delusions, affective isolation, and withdrawal.

Several preceding studies did not find indications for impaired specificity but rather for reduced sensitivity (5, 6). Our earlier cross-cultural study (4) found lower sensitivity and specificity for German and American patients. The difference between the present paradigm and the other studies is that here we identified the target emotion for each block, eliminating the need to subclassify emotions and thereby perhaps facilitating sensitivity. Establishing differential deficits in emotional processing compared to nonemotional aspects of faces does not mean that patients will be more impaired for all emotions relative to all nonemotional processing tasks. Note, however, that even for the easily recognizable emotion of happiness, a performance deficit emerged in patients, whereas for age discrimination, which is a significantly more difficult task for comparison subjects in relation to emotion recognition, the deficit was less apparent. Age discrimination performance in patients was better than recognition of each negative emotion, whereas in comparison subjects, performance here was worse than for any negative emotion. Further research is needed to examine whether impairment of sensitivity or specificity of emotion in patients with schizophrenia depends on situational factors modulating expectancy.

Gender differences were indicated for sensitivity by a significant gender-by-emotion interaction. Men displayed a greater sensitivity for recognition of angry faces than women, irrespective of group. Because gender differences in emotion recognition have been reported before (32), this may point to a differential evolutionary significance of different emotions for men and women. Anger as a signal for aggression may entail a greater biological significance for men than women.

The present study has several limitations. Conclusions with respect to a specific versus generalized deficit need to be taken with extreme caution, and our results require replication and further placement in a larger context of deficits. It is extremely challenging to find adequate matched control tasks for emotion recognition tasks. Our control tasks matched the emotional task on stimulus material and task construction parameters, yet they proved slightly but significantly more difficult. The present paradigm is also limited in that it does not provide measures of differential patterns of misidentifications of target emotions. When a happy face is misclassified, is it more likely to be seen as neutral, angry, or sad? Such error patterns would be of interest, and earlier studies (8) have shown that misattribution of negative emotions to neutral faces was the main outcome measure differentiating patients with schizophrenia from comparison subjects. However, the present paradigm was designed to separate sensitivity from specificity by using a binary separation of target from foils. Thus, during each block, the participant indicates only whether the face has the target emotion; participants are not then asked to further specify the emotion if it is not the target. Adding a query for specific misattribution to the task would have compromised its psychometric integrity, and analysis of error patterns necessitates a different testing paradigm.

Another limitation of the study is that the order of two conditions (age discrimination and memory) was fixed, and the impact of task order needs to be determined. Furthermore, the recognition phase showed only neutral faces because it was used as a nonemotional control condition, and a proper balance of emotional and nonemotional faces during recognition will make the task prohibitively long. It would be useful to apply emotional and nonemotional faces for the facial recognition task. However, such an examination, although of clear merit, is beyond the scope of the present study. The study also lacks a condition maximizing specificity of search. Target emotions may be easier to recognize in the present task because the participant is required to focus on one target emotion only during each block. This is similar to tasks in which priming is used. The target emotion reflects the prime, and seeing a target emotion requires only a correct assignment. It would be interesting to compare performance of patients and comparison subjects when the requirement is a decision on the correct emotion for each stimulus without any prime. However, this may pose its own conceptual difficulties. There is no evidence for floor or ceiling effects in sensitivity performance in the present study. Thus, patients could have shown greater impairment in sensitivity. Furthermore, since happy faces were easier to recognize in both groups, the heterogeneity of responses within the emotion recognition task may have accentuated the differences in performance to the control tasks. However, because this was similar in both groups and group differences were the focus of interest in this study, we believe this factor does not affect our main conclusions.

The basis for deficits in facial emotion processing in schizophrenia is unclear. Deficient visual scanning has been hypothesized (18). Several studies applying chimeric faces, which are known to elicit a perceptual bias to the left hemiface in healthy persons, have found a significantly weaker bias in schizophrenia patients (33, 34). Given evidence that emotions are expressed more intensely in the left hemiface (35, 36), deficient facial scanning and perhaps eye-tracking mechanisms merit further examination.

The deficits found in emotion recognition likely relate to impairments observed in “theory of mind” tasks. Schizophrenia patients have deficits in theory of mind problems (37), requiring conscious interpretation of someone else’s mental state (empathy). Arguably, facial emotion recognition requires theory of mind. Further study can help establish whether these impairments are related to or are independent of general cognitive deficits and whether they reflect dysfunction in partly overlapping cerebral networks.

The present results encourage functional imaging studies probing neural systems for emotion processing that show abnormal activation in schizophrenia (10, 11), and the present task can provide a probe that yields informative performance data.

TABLE 1

Received Nov. 5, 2004; revision received April 26, 2005; accepted April 27, 2005. From the Departments of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, RWTH Aachen University; the Neuropsychiatry Section, Department of Psychiatry, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia; and the Institute of Medicine, Research Center Jülich, Jülich, Germany. Address correspondence and reprint requests to Dr. Schneider, Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, RWTH Aachen University, Pauwelsstr. 30, 52074 Aachen, Germany; (e-mail).Supported by the German Research Foundation DFG, Schn 362/13-1 and 13-2, the Research Center Jülich, the Federal Ministry of Education and Research (Brain Imaging Center West, 01GO0204), and NIH grant MH-60722.The authors thank Helen Greenland, Thilo Kellermann, Martina Klein, Nina Seiferth, Toni Stöcker, and Sabrina Weber for assistance.

Figure 1.

Figure 1. Performance on Emotion Recognition Task and Nonemotional Control Task by Comparison Subjects and Schizophrenia Patients

Figure 2.

Figure 2. Performance on Tasks Showing Sensitivity and Specificity of Emotion Recognition by Comparison Subjects and Schizophrenia Patients

References

1. Kraepelin E: Dementia Praecox and Paraphrenia. Translated by Barclay RM; edited by Robertson GM. Edinburgh, E & S Livingstone, 1919Google Scholar

2. Feinberg TE, Rifkin A, Schaffer C, Walker E: Facial discrimination and emotional recognition in schizophrenia and affective disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1986; 43:276–279Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

3. Heimberg C, Gur RE, Erwin RJ, Shtasel DL, Gur RC: Facial emotion discrimination, III: behavioral findings in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res 1992; 42:253–265Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

4. Habel U, Gur RC, Mandal MK, Salloum JB, Gur RE, Schneider F: Emotional processing in schizophrenia across cultures: standardized measures of discrimination and experience. Schizophr Res 2000; 42:57–66Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

5. Mueser KT, Penn DL, Blanchard JJ, Bellack AS: Affect recognition in schizophrenia: a synthesis of findings across three studies. Psychiatry 1997; 60:301–308Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

6. Penn DL, Combs DR, Ritchie M, Francis J, Cassisi J, Morris S, Townsend M: Emotion recognition in schizophrenia: further investigation of generalized versus specific deficit models. J Abnorm Psychol 2000; 109:512–516Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

7. Kohler CG, Bilker W, Hagendoorn M, Gur RE, Gur RC: Emotion recognition deficit in schizophrenia: association with symptomatology and cognition. Biol Psychiatry 2000; 48:127–136Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

8. Kohler CG, Turner TH, Bilker WB, Brensinger CM, Siegel SJ, Kanes SJ, Gur RE, Gur RC: Facial emotion recognition in schizophrenia: intensity effects and error pattern. Am J Psychiatry 2003; 160:1768–1774LinkGoogle Scholar

9. Schneider F, Gur RC, Gur RE, Shtasel DL: Emotional processing in schizophrenia: neurobehavioral probes in relation to psychopathology. Schizophr Res 1995; 17:67–75Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

10. Schneider F, Weiss U, Kessler C, Salloum JB, Posse S, Grodd W, Müller-Gärtner H-W: Differential amygdala activation in schizophrenia during sadness. Schizophr Res 1998; 34:133–142Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

11. Gur RE, McGrath C, Chan RM, Schroeder L, Turner T, Turetsky BI, Kohler C, Alsop D, Maldjian J, Ragland JD, Gur RC: An fMRI study of facial emotion processing in patients with schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 2002; 159:1992–1999LinkGoogle Scholar

12. Kerr SL, Neale JM: Emotion perception in schizophrenia: specific deficit or further evidence of generalized poor performance? J Abnorm Psychol 1993; 102:312–318Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

13. Kring AM, Neale JM: Do schizophrenia patients show a disjunctive relationship among expressive, experiential, and psychophysiological components of emotion? J Abnorm Psychol 1996; 105:249–257Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

14. Kring AM, Kerr S, Smith DA, Neale JM: Flat affect in schizophrenia does not reflect diminished subjective experience of emotion. J Abnorm Psychol 1993; 104:507–517CrossrefGoogle Scholar

15. Johnston PJ, Katsikitis M, Carr VJ: A generalised deficit can account for problems in facial emotion recognition in schizophrenia. Biol Psychol 2001; 58:203–227Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

16. Whittaker JF, Deakin JF, Tomenson B: Face processing in schizophrenia: defining the deficit. Psychol Med 2001; 31:499–507Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

17. Salem JE, Kring AM, Kerr SL: More evidence for generalized poor performance in facial emotion perception in schizophrenia. J Abnorm Psychol 1996; 105:480–483Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

18. Loughland CM, Williams LM, Gordon E: Visual scanpaths to positive and negative facial emotions in an outpatient schizophrenia sample. Schizophr Res 2002; 55:159–170Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

19. Saykin AJ, Gur RC, Gur RE, Mozley PD, Mozley LH, Resnick SM, Kester DB, Stafiniak P: Neuropsychological function in schizophrenia: selective impairment in memory and learning. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1991; 48:618–624Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

20. Tracy JI, Mattson R, King C, Bundick T, Celenza MA, Glosser G: A comparison of memory for verbal and non-verbal material in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 2001; 50:199–211Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

21. Van Oostrom I, Dollfus S, Brazo P, Abadie P, Halbecq I, Thery S, Marie RM: Verbal learning and memory in schizophrenic and Parkinson’s disease patients. Psychiatry Res 2003; 117:25–34Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

22. Tendolkar I, Ruhrmann S, Brockhaus A, Pukrop R, Klosterkötter J: Remembering or knowing: electrophysiological evidence for an episodic memory deficit in schizophrenia. Psychol Med 2002; 32:1261–1271Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

23. Gur RC, Ragland JD, Moberg PJ, Bilker WB, Kohler C, Siegel SJ, Gur RE: Computerized neurocognitive scanning, II: the profile of schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology 2001; 25:777–788Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

24. Gur RC, Schroeder L, Turner T, McGrath C, Chan RM, Turetsky BI, Alsop D, Maldjian J, Gur RE: Brain activation during facial emotion processing. Neuroimage 2002; 16:651–662Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

25. Gur RC, Sara R, Hagendoorn M, Marom O, Hughett P, Macy L, Turner T, Bajcsy R, Posner A, Gur RE: A method for obtaining 3-dimensional facial expressions and its standardization for use in neurocognitive studies. J Neurosci Methods 2002; 115:137–143Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

26. Gunning-Dixon FM, Gur RC, Perkins AC, Schroeder L, Turner T, Turetsky BI, Chan RM, Loughead JW, Alsop DC, Maldjian J, Gur RE: Age-related differences in brain activation during emotional face processing. Neurobiol Aging 2003; 24:285–295Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

27. Kay SR, Fiszbein A, Opler LA: The Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 1987; 13:261–276Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

28. Chapman LJ, Chapman JP: The measurement of differential deficit. J Psychiatr Res 1978; 14:303–311Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

29. Mandal MK, Jain A, Haque-Nizamie S, Weiss U, Schneider F: Generality and specificity of emotion-recognition deficit in schizophrenic patients with positive and negative symptoms. Psychiatry Res 1999; 87:39–46Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

30. Penn DL, Ritchie M, Francis J, Combs D, Martin J: Social perception in schizophrenia: the role of context. J Psychiatry Res 2002; 109:149–159CrossrefGoogle Scholar

31. Kohler CG, Gur RE, Gur RC: Recognition of facial emotions in neuropsychiatric disorders. CNS Spectr 2004; 9:267–274Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

32. Thayer JF, Johnson BH: Sex differences in judgement of facial affect: a multivariate analysis of recognition errors. Scand J Psychol 2000; 41:243–246Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

33. Gooding DC, Tallent KA: Schizophrenia patients’ perceptual biases in response to positively and negatively valenced emotion chimeras. Psychol Med 2002; 32:1101–1107Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

34. Kucharska-Pietura K, David AS, Dropko P, Klimkowski M: The perception of emotional chimeric faces in schizophrenia: further evidence of right hemisphere dysfunction. Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol Behav Neurol 2002; 15:72–78MedlineGoogle Scholar

35. Sackeim HA, Gur RC, Saucy MC: Emotions are expressed more intensely on the left side of the face. Science 1978; 202:433–435CrossrefGoogle Scholar

36. Indersmitten T, Gur RC: Emotion processing in chimeric faces: hemispheric asymmetry in expression and recognition of emotions. J Neurosci 2003; 23:3820–3825Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

37. Lee KH, Farrow TF, Spence SA, Woodruff PW: Social cognition, brain networks and schizophrenia. Psychol Med 2004; 34:391–400Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar