The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×
ArticleFull Access

Longitudinal Assessment of Premorbid Cognitive Functioning in Patients With Schizophrenia Through Examination of Standardized Scholastic Test Performance

Abstract

OBJECTIVE: Studies suggest the presence of premorbid cognitive impairment in patients with schizophrenia, yet the onset and course of these deficits remain unclear. The purpose of this study was to investigate the longitudinal course of premorbid cognitive functioning in individuals with schizophrenia by using prospective data obtained from scholastic test results. METHOD: Scores from grades 4, 8, and 11 on the Iowa Tests of Basic Skills and the Iowa Tests of Educational Development were obtained for 70 subjects who later developed schizophrenia. The mean percentile rank of the subjects’ scores in the areas of vocabulary, reading comprehension, language, mathematics, sources of information, and an overall composite category were compared with state norms. RESULTS: The subjects scored below the 50th percentile for each category in all three grades, but only language, reading, sources of information, and composite scores from grade 11 were significantly below state norms. With regard to longitudinal course, there was a significant linear decrease in language scores over time. Scores from grade 11 were positively correlated with WAIS-R IQ, verbal fluency, and Rey Auditory Verbal Learning scores at first illness episode. Scores from grade 11 were not significantly correlated with age at illness onset or ratings of disorganized, psychotic, or negative symptoms. CONCLUSIONS: Scholastic test scores at grades 4 and 8 were nonsignificantly below average in this group of children who later developed schizophrenia. However, test scores dropped significantly between grades 8 and 11. This corresponds to ages 13–16 years, or the onset of puberty. Poor or declining scholastic performance may be a precursor to the cognitive impairment seen during the first episode of illness.

Cognitive impairment in patients with schizophrenia is ubiquitous and is considered to be core to the pathophysiology of the illness (1). Reports of cognitive impairment in schizophrenia date back to the pioneering efforts of Kraepelin (2) and Bleuler (3), yet the course of these deficits over time is still unclear. Cognitive impairment has been found during the first episode of illness in schizophrenia (410), and there is evidence that these impairments continue into the chronic stages of the illness (4, 7, 11, 12).

Moreover, several studies have reported cognitive deficits that significantly predate the onset of the illness. Jones and colleagues (13) found that children from a 1946 birth cohort who later developed schizophrenia had lower mean scores on educational tests than did comparison subjects at ages 8, 11, and 15. The greatest deficits registered were in verbal, nonverbal, and mathematics tests, and the smallest deficits were in vocabulary and reading tests. Although cross-sectional in design, it appeared that these deficits worsened over time, with the strongest findings seen with the 15-year-old subjects.

Ambelas (14) investigated childhood records of patients with schizophrenia who had been seen at a child guidance center and compared these records with those of children seen at the center who did not develop schizophrenia. The children who went on to develop schizophrenia had lower IQs and impaired performance in the areas of speech, language, and reading. These were children who had been seen at a child-care center and thus would have been exhibiting clinical symptoms or behavioral problems and therefore are special cases. Similarly, studies of “high-risk children,” i.e., the offspring of people with schizophrenia, have reported impairments in attention and memory (15).

Two studies have investigated cognitive performance in large numbers of healthy male adolescents by using assessments obtained at the time of military conscription (16, 17). Compared with subjects who did not develop psychiatric illness, the male subjects who were later diagnosed with schizophrenia had lower test scores with significant deficits in social functioning, organizational ability, and intellectual ability (17). In addition, there was a linear relationship between risk of schizophrenia and intellectual functioning (16, 17).

Although premorbid cognitive deficits in schizophrenia appear to be well documented, many questions still remain. For instance, to our knowledge no study to date has longitudinally evaluated premorbid cognition to assess its course over time. Also unanswered is whether these premorbid cognitive deficits are related to clinical measures of postonset illness, such as severity of symptoms or age at illness onset.

More than 30 years ago, the Department of Education at the University of Iowa pioneered the development of two standardized cognitive test batteries to be administered to all students in Iowa and many students nationwide. The Iowa Tests of Basic Skills (18) are given to students in kindergarten through grade 8, and the Iowa Tests of Educational Development (19) are given to students in grades 9 through 12. The purpose of the present study was to investigate premorbid cognitive functioning in patients with schizophrenia by examining their scores on these scholastic tests. The objectives of the study were to 1) document the extent and pattern of cognitive abilities; 2) assess the longitudinal course of cognitive abilities by evaluating change in scores over three periods (grades 4, 8, and 11); and 3) investigate the relationship between premorbid cognitive measures and “postmorbid” cognitive measures as well as positive, disorganized, and negative symptoms.

Method

Subjects

Subjects were 70 patients obtained from two different sources: admissions to the Mental Health Clinical Research Center and those who were participating in a prospective longitudinal study (20). Subjects who were diagnosed with DSM-IV schizophrenia after a structured interview (the Comprehensive Assessment of Symptoms and History [21]) were then asked if they had attended school during grades 4–11 in Iowa. Those who had were asked to provide written consent to allow release of their Iowa Tests of Basic Skills and Iowa Tests of Educational Development records. All subjects who were asked to release their scores agreed to do so. Fifty-seven were male, and 13 were female. The mean age of the patients at admission was 28.0 years (SD=6.9); the mean duration of illness was 7.8 years (SD=6.7).

The test scores for grades 4, 8, and 11 were then collected from the University of Iowa’s Department of Education and merged with demographic information and symptom ratings from the Mental Health Clinical Research Center. Data for 70 subjects with scores for at least one of the test years and Comprehensive Assessment of Symptoms and History data were used in the analyses.

Standardized Test Measures

State percentile ranks for the Iowa Tests of Basic Skills and the Iowa Tests of Educational Development were used in all analyses. The percentile rank reveals the student’s relative position or rank among a group of Iowa students who were in the same grade and who were tested at the same time of year. The state percentile ranks were used instead of the national percentile ranks so as to minimize confounds from curricular sequence, emphasis, and breadth of coverage. These curricular differences vary across the nation but are more homogeneous within the state of Iowa. Furthermore, students from the state of Iowa generally rank higher than the national average (22).

Six educational domains were created by the Iowa Department of Education when designing the standardized test batteries to measure the academic skills of students. Because the state norms are supplied for these domains, it was deemed necessary and worthwhile to use the domains provided.

Vocabulary

The vocabulary domain of both the Iowa Tests of Basic Skills and the Iowa Tests of Educational Development provides a measure of reading vocabulary. Students read a target word in context and then select the word or phrase that most closely conveys the same meaning as the target word.

Reading comprehension

This test assesses how well students can comprehend written material. In the Iowa Tests of Basic Skills, the passages to be read vary in length from a few lines to a full page and consist of fiction, poetry, social studies, science, fables, interviews, biographies, and other nonfiction. Three levels of meaning are measured: factual, inferential, and evaluative. In the Iowa Tests of Educational Development, the passages are 275 to 700 words in length and consist of five levels of meaning: factual, inferential, nonliteral, generalizing themes and ideas, and recognizing literary techniques and tone.

Language

The language tests measure fundamental skills in the use of standard written English and do not directly measure the student’s ability to write well. The Iowa Tests of Basic Skills focuses on the student’s ability to apply the guidelines for spelling, capitalization, punctuation, and usage. Usage questions ask students to evaluate which of the proposed revisions would make the passage better organized, more focused, or complete. For the Iowa Tests of Educational Development, the six areas tested are spelling, capitalization and punctuation, usage and grammar, sentence structure, organization of ideas, and expression.

Mathematics

In the Iowa Tests of Basic Skills, the mathematics component consists of two sections. The first section involves concepts and estimations. The concepts assessed include number system and numeration theory, arithmetic, geometry, measurement, fraction/decimals/percents, probability/statistics, and equations/inequalities/number sentences. Estimations include estimates using rounding, order of magnitude, front-ending, compatible numbers, and compensation. The second section examines problem solving and data interpretation. The emphasis on solving multiple-step problems increases with test level. Data interpretation ranges from reading amounts to interpreting data from a graph or a table. The mathematics section in the Iowa Tests of Educational Development measures a student’s ability to solve quantitative problems and to use appropriate mathematical reasoning. The questions require ability to perform basic arithmetic, estimations, data interpretation, and logical thinking.

Sources of information

In the Iowa Tests of Basic Skills, this domain assesses a student’s ability to use maps, diagrams, and reference materials. The Iowa Tests of Educational Development evaluates a student’s ability to use books, dictionaries, encyclopedias, maps, government sources, and periodical guides.

Composite

The composite score for both the Iowa Tests of Basic Skills and the Iowa Tests of Educational Development represents the mean of the total scores for the domains of reading ([vocabulary + reading comprehension]/2), language, mathematics, and sources of information.

Symptoms and Cognitive Test Measures

Symptoms were assessed upon admission and scored by using the Comprehensive Assessment of Symptoms and History (21). The symptoms were classified as disorganized (bizarre behavior, positive thought disorder, inappropriate affect), psychotic (hallucinations and delusions), or negative (alogia, flattened affect, anhedonia, avolition).

Cognitive assessment occurred during a subject’s intake hospitalization at the Mental Health Clinical Research Center. Patients were tested when they were clinically stable and able to participate fully. In many cases, this was after treatment with neuroleptic medications. Patients were administered a comprehensive battery of tests that typically took 4 hours to complete. The battery included the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (23), from which the immediate recall, delayed recall, and recognition scores were taken; the Rey-Osterreith Complex Figure Test (24, 25), from which the copy, immediate recall, delayed recall, and saving scores were taken; the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (12), from which the number of categories was taken; the Word Fluency Test (26), from which the number of words generated in 3 minutes in response to the letters C, F, and L were taken; the Stroop Color and Word Test (27), from which the interference score was taken; and the Grooved Pegboard Test (28). In addition, general IQs were obtained by using the WAIS-R (29).

Statistical Analysis

First, we looked at the mean percentile rank of the patients compared with the expected median population value in Iowa (i.e., the 50th percentile). Simple one-group F tests were used for this analysis and are reported as effect sizes (Cohen’s d).

Next, the data were analyzed by using a random regression model. This model was chosen because it allows for missing observations and gives estimations based on both the subject and group data (30). Two models are presented. The first is an analog of a repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA), which tested whether there were differences among grades 4, 8, and 11. Another model was used to test for a simple linear decline in scores over time. Preliminary analyses indicated that a first-order auto-regressive model provided the best fit for our covariance structure and was used consistently.

There were 24 subjects with missing data for either 1 or 2 years, 16 of whom were missing data for the 11th grade. In order to assess the possibility that data were missing completely at random, demographic items from the Comprehensive Assessment of Symptoms and History for the group of patients with missing data were compared with those for two groups: 1) patients with no missing data, and 2) patients with scores for the 11th grade. These groups did not differ significantly in terms of level of education achieved, parental socioeconomic status, age at onset of illness, age at first outpatient care, age at first hospitalization, or premorbid adjustment scale score (for ages 6–12 or ages 13–21). There were no significant differences between the two groups for any of the scores on the Iowa Tests of Basic Skills for grades 4 or 8. These findings would support the notion that the data are missing completely at random.

In addition, we interviewed the faculty at the University of Iowa Department of Education about the process that led to missing data in their files. Much of the missing data throughout the state was produced by random events such as scheduling conflicts, lost records from entire schools, etc. These factors are unrelated to the subject and are not associated with the disorder that the child would face later in life. Thus, we feel comfortable that the data are missing completely at random.

Preliminary investigation revealed that there were no significant differences between male and female subjects on any of the test scores. Therefore, the male and female subjects were treated as one group throughout the analyses. Preliminary investigation also revealed that there were no significant differences between test scores of patients with illness onset before age 20 compared with those with illness onset at age 20 or older.

Pearson’s correlations were used to assess associations between variables from the standardized tests and the symptom ratings from the Comprehensive Assessment of Symptoms and History as well as neuropsychological data.

Results

The subjects’ mean percentile rank on the domains of the scholastic standardized tests for grades 4, 8, and 11 appear in Figure 1. In a purely descriptive manner, it is evident that these children who later developed schizophrenia fell below the state norms on every category for all three grades.

Analysis of differences between the median percentile rank of the state population of Iowa and the mean percentile rank of our patient group revealed that the patients’ 4th and 8th grade scores did not significantly differ from state norms for any of the variables. This suggests that in grades 4 and 8, the patients were not significantly below average for any of the categories tested on the Iowa Tests of Basic Skills. However, as seen in Figure 1, the grade 11 scores of the patients were significantly below the median percentile rank for reading, language, sources of information, and composite scores. The effect sizes for these differences in reading (d=0.36), language (d=0.41), sources of information (d=0.35), and composite (d=0.32) scores were just below a “medium” effect (d=0.50) in Cohen’s terms. The mean scores for these domains were 9.2 (SD=30.1), 12.4 (SD=29.8), 9.1 (SD=31.7), and 8.6 (SD=29.9) percentiles below average, respectively.

In addition, within the patient group there was a significant difference among the three grades for the language and sources of information scores (Figure 1). Further analyses of these data revealed that the sources of information scores did not differ significantly between grades 4 and 8 or between grades 4 and 11, but the scores for grade 11 were significantly lower than those for grade 8. For the language scores, there was no significant difference in scores between grades 4 and 8, but the grade 11 scores were significantly lower than those of both grade 4 and grade 8.

Finally, there was a significant linear decline in language scores across time (F=5.71, df=1, 108, p<0.04). This means that the language scores in grades 4, 8, and 11 became steadily worse.

Since none of the scores from grades 4 or 8 significantly differed from state norms, only the grade 11 scores from the Iowa Tests of Educational Development were used in the correlations with symptom and neuropsychological data. Because of the conservative nature of Bonferroni corrections, these were not used to adjust for the number of correlations. Instead, only those correlations significant at the 0.01 level or below will be discussed.

There were no significant correlations between Iowa Tests of Educational Development domain scores and age at onset of illness (r=0.06 to 0.26) or ratings of disorganized (r=0.07 to 0.24), psychotic (r=–0.04 to –0.22), or negative (r=–0.02 to –0.15) symptoms at the time of hospital admission.

The results of the Pearson’s correlations between the neuropsychological tests carried out at the Mental Health Clinical Research Center following the onset of schizophrenia and grade 11 scores on the Iowa Tests of Educational Development are presented in Table 1. There was a significant positive correlation between the scores for each domain and WAIS-R verbal IQ and between the scores for each domain (except language and reading) and WAIS-R performance IQ. There was a significant positive correlation between Word Fluency performance and the vocabulary, reading, language, and composite scores. This means that higher scores on the Iowa Tests of Educational Development were associated with a greater number of words produced in 60 seconds. There was a significant positive correlation between performance on the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test and all domain scores except vocabulary. This means that higher standardized test scores were related to a greater number of words correctly recalled across all trials. There were no significant correlations between Iowa Tests of Educational Development scores at grade 11 and performance on any other neuropsychological test.

Discussion

The results of this study provide evidence in support of previous findings that children who later develop schizophrenia manifest intellectual differences from peers before illness onset. These results are unique in that the patient groups included both male and female subjects, the premorbid scores were prospective and longitudinal (reflecting the childhood and adolescent stages of their lives), and multiple areas of knowledge were examined instead of using a general “intelligence” score.

For each domain of the scholastic standardized tests at each grade, the mean percentile rank for the children who later developed schizophrenia was below the 50th percentile (median state rank). However, these scores were not statistically significantly below average at grades 4 or 8. It was not until grade 11 that scores for patients fell significantly below Iowa state norms. Moreover, although the grade 11 composite score was significantly lower than the norm, some areas of cognitive functioning were more severely impaired than others. For example, grade 11 vocabulary and mathematics scores were not significantly below state norms.

Closer inspection of the pattern of scores over time reveals that in every category except language, there was actually an increase in scores in the interval between grades 4 and 8. Then, in the next interval between grades 8 and 11, four of the six measures (including composite scores) precipitously dropped to below the scores obtained in grade 4 and significantly below the state norm. Grade 8 language scores were only slightly below those of grade 4 but are unique in that they did not follow the pattern of increasing by grade 8. It is this pattern for the language scores that manifested as a statistically significant linear decline over time.

The current findings provide evidence that cognitive impairment is present before symptom onset in subjects who later develop schizophrenia. A drop in scores from grade 8 to grade 11 is not normally seen in the Iowa standardized test scores. The tests are designed so that an individual’s scores from 1 year will predict any other year. While it is true that any child can have an “off year” and decline more than would be expected, the significant decline in the scores of the students who later develop schizophrenia suggests more than just an off year. For the children who later developed schizophrenia, something was occurring between the ages of 13 (8th grade) and 16 (11th grade) that led to cognitive decline.

Of course, one thing that is occurring between ages 13 and 16 for many subjects is adolescence. This is by no means a coincidence, as more and more evidence suggests that adolescence is a time of robust structural and functional brain changes. Moreover, these changes may underlie the pathophysiology of schizophrenia. Several studies have provided evidence for a process of “pruning” in which excess or nonuseful neural connections are trimmed, leading to a decrease over time in synaptic density (31, 32), glucose metabolism (33), and cortical gray matter volume (34). Feinberg (35) was the first to suggest that schizophrenia could be due to abnormal pruning. A longitudinal study of childhood-onset schizophrenia has shown that the “normal” decrease in cortical gray matter over the ages 13–17 (seen in healthy comparison subjects) was four times greater in the patient group, leading to a significant decrement in regional cortical volume, which suggests an “overpruning” (36). These brain changes are documented for the same period of time in which the current study shows a significant academic decline, indicating that “overpruning” of the cortex may be directly related to changes in cognitive function.

One interpretation of the current findings is that the cognitive deficits seen in the 11th grade may be due to “prodrome” or early phase of the illness rather than true premorbid deficit. However, there was no correlation between age at illness onset and scores on the Iowa Tests of Educational Development. This suggests that patients with lower scores were not necessarily closer to illness onset than patients with higher scores and supports the notion that the cognitive deficits are truly premorbid rather than prodromal or early illness. On the other hand, the distinction among premorbid, prodrome, and early illness may be artificial. The current study suggests that specific cognitive functions such as language slowly decline over time beginning in childhood. If this suggestion is correct, then the definition of illness onset becomes decidedly more complex.

The cognitive impairment seen after the diagnosis of schizophrenia is made appears to be much stronger than the premorbid cognitive deficit seen with scholastic standardized test results. The decline in language may be the first impairment to surface, and other cognitive abilities may decrease between the mid-to-late teens and the onset of psychosis. Another possibility is that the cognitive impairment seen in the first episode of schizophrenia is different from that examined by scholastic aptitude tests. Scores on the Iowa Tests of Educational Development were associated with WAIS-R verbal and performance IQs, but they were not significantly associated with all of the neuropsychological test results. Scholastic cognition may be different from neuropsychological cognition. It is likely that the cognitive domains such as reasoning and abstraction (Wisconsin Card Sorting Test), visuospatial skills (Rey-Osterreith Complex Figure Test), and inhibition (Stroop Color and Word Test) are using different cognitive skills than those used in language expression, sources of information, or reading.

One limitation of this study is the small size. There were only 70 participants with schizophrenia to compare with state norms. A second shortcoming was the lack of highly significant results. The power to detect effects, given the study group size, may not be extremely high. Power considerations only apply to negative effects. Not all our findings proved significant, but a substantial amount of significant results exists, suggesting that something is not normal in premorbid cognitive functioning in subjects who later develop schizophrenia. Further data collection for a larger patient group is currently underway.

A third limitation is that our group of patients was not a random sample. The group was a select group of individuals who developed schizophrenia in adulthood. There may be a number of factors associated with our group of children that may make them nonrepresentative, including socioeconomic status, season of birth, family history, low birth weight, etc. Since schizophrenia is likely a disease with heterogeneous etiology, it may be impossible to produce a truly matched comparison group.

Although the results of the current study provide evidence of premorbid cognitive impairment in individuals with schizophrenia, some students who went on to develop schizophrenia scored in the 90th percentile or above. Thus, not all patients diagnosed with schizophrenia show premorbid cognitive impairment, just as not all children whose standardized tests scores decline in grade 11 are destined to develop schizophrenia.

This initial investigation has provided evidence that examining scholastic achievement tests is a worthwhile investigative measure of premorbid cognitive functioning in patients with schizophrenia. Future studies are needed to explore a more fine-grained evaluation of scores between grades 8 and 11, to examine the relationship between scores and structural and functional brain measures, and to determine whether an aggregate of measures can be used to identify high-risk children who will ultimately develop schizophrenia.

TABLE 1

Received May 4, 2001; revision received Jan. 22, 2002; accepted Feb. 6, 2002. From the Mental Health Clinical Research Center, Department of Psychiatry, University of Iowa Hospitals and Clinics, Iowa City. Address reprint requests to Dr. Fuller, Maryland Psychiatric Research Center, University of Maryland, P.O. Box 21247, Baltimore, MD 21228; (e-mail). Supported in part by NIMH grants MH-31593 and MH-40856; an NIMH Clinical Research Center grant (MH-43271); the Nellie Ball Trust Research Fund, Iowa State Bank and Trust Company, Trustee; and an NIMH Research Scientist Award (MH-00625). The authors thank H.D. Hoover from the Iowa Testing Programs for his advice and assistance and Sue Oliver for data collection.

Figure 1.

Figure 1. Standardized Scholastic Test Performancea in Grades 4, 8, and 11 Relative to State Norms for 70 Subjects Who Later Developed Schizophrenia

aGrade 4 and 8 results represent performance on the Iowa Tests of Basic Skills; grade 11 results represent performance on the Iowa Tests of Educational Development.

bGrade 11 score significantly lower than median percentile rank (F=5.89, df=1, 45, p<0.05).

cGrade 11 score significantly lower than median percentile rank (F=7.80, df=1, 45, p<0.01). Repeated measures ANOVA revealed a significant difference in scores among the three grades (F=3.18, df=2, 107, p<0.05), with further analyses showing that the grade 11 scores were significantly lower than the scores in grade 4 (F=5.04, df=1, 45, p<0.05) and grade 8 (F=4.97, df=1, 45, p<0.05).

dGrade 11 score significantly lower than median percentile rank (F=5.63, df=1, 45, p<0.05). Repeated measures ANOVA revealed a significant difference in scores among the three grades (F=3.66, df=2, 107, p<0.03), with further analyses showing that the grade 11 scores were significantly lower than the scores in grade 8 (F=6.40, df=1, 46, p=0.01).

eGrade 11 score significantly lower than median percentile rank (F=4.77, df=1, 45, p<0.05).

References

1. Andreasen N, Paradiso S, O’Leary D: “Cognitive dysmetria” as an integrative theory of schizophrenia: a dysfunction in cortical-subcortical-cerebellar-circuitry? Schizophr Bull 1998; 24:203-218Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

2. Kraepelin E: Dementia Praecox and Paraphrenia. Translated by Barclay RM; edited by Robertson GM. Edinburgh, E & S Livingstone, 1919Google Scholar

3. Bleuler E: Dementia Praecox or the Group of Schizophrenias (1911). Translated by Zinkin J. New York, International Universities Press, 1950Google Scholar

4. Bilder R, Lipschultz-Broch L, Reiter G, Geisler S, Mayerhoff D, Lieberman J: Intellectual deficits in first-episode patients with schizophrenia: evidence for progressive deterioration. Schizophr Bull 1992; 18:437-448Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

5. Hoff AL, Riordan H, O’Donnell DW, Morris L, DeLisi LE: Neuropsychological functioning of first-episode schizophreniform patients. Am J Psychiatry 1992; 149:898-903LinkGoogle Scholar

6. Hoff A, Riordan H, O’Donnell D, Stritzke P, Neale C, Boccio A, Anand A, DeLisi L: Anomalous lateral sulcus asymmetry and cognitive function in first-episode schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 1992; 18:257-270Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

7. Saykin A, Gur R, Gur R, Mozley P, Mozley L, Resnick S, Kester D, Stafiniak P: Neuropsychological function in schizophrenia: selective impairment in memory and learning. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1991; 48:618-624Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

8. Censits D, Ragland D, Gur R, Gur R: Neuropsychological evidence supporting a neurodevelopmental model of schizophrenia: a longitudinal study. Schizophr Res 1997; 24:289-298Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

9. Mohamed S, Paulsen J, O’Leary D, Arndt S, Andreasen N: Generalized cognitive deficits in schizophrenia: a study of first-episode patients. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1999; 56:749-754Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

10. Gold S, Arndt S, Nopolous P, O’Leary DS, Andreasen NC: Longitudinal study of cognitive function in first-episode and recent-onset schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 1999; 156:1342-1348AbstractGoogle Scholar

11. Goldberg E, Seidman L: Higher cortical functions in normals and in schizophrenia: a selective review, in Neuropsychology, Psychophysiology and Information Processing: Handbook of Schizophrenia, vol 5. Edited by Steinhauser S, Gruzelier J, Zubin J. Amsterdam, Elsevier, 1991, pp 553-597Google Scholar

12. Heaton R, Paulsen J, McAdams L, Kuck J, Zisook S, Braff D, Harris J, Jeste D: Neuropsychological deficits in schizophrenics: relationship to age, chronicity, and dementia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1994; 51:469-476Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

13. Jones P, Rodgers B, Murray R, Marmot M: Child developmental risk factors for adult schizophrenia in the British 1946 birth cohort. Lancet 1994; 344:1398-1402Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

14. Ambelas A: Preschizophrenics: adding to the evidence, sharpening the focus. Br J Psychiatry 1992; 160:401-404Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

15. Erlenmeyer-Kimling L, Rock D, Roberts SA, Janal M, Kestenbaum C, Cornblatt B, Adamo UH, Gottesman II: Attention, memory, and motor skills as childhood predictors of schizophrenia-related psychoses: the New York High-Risk Project. Am J Psychiatry 2000; 157:1416-1422LinkGoogle Scholar

16. David A, Malmberg A, Brandt L, Allebeck P, Lewis G: IQ and risk for schizophrenia: a population-based cohort study. Psychol Med 1997; 27:1311-1323Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

17. Davidson M, Reichenberg A, Rabinowiz J, Weiser M, Kaplan Z, Mark M: Behavioral and intellectual markers for schizophrenia in apparently healthy male adolescents. Am J Psychiatry 1999; 156:1328-1335AbstractGoogle Scholar

18. Hoover HD, Hieronymus AN, Dunbar SB, Frisbie DA: Iowa Tests of Basic Skills. Itasca, Ill, Riverside Publishing, 1996Google Scholar

19. Forsyth RA, Ansley TN, Feldt LS, Alnot SD: Iowa Tests of Educational Development. Itasca, Ill, Riverside Publishing, 2001Google Scholar

20. Flaum MA, Andreasen NC, Arndt S: The Iowa prospective longitudinal study of recent-onset psychoses. Schizophr Bull 1992; 18:481-490Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

21. Andreasen NC, Flaum M, Arndt S: The Comprehensive Assessment of Symptoms and History (CASH): an instrument for assessing diagnosis and psychopathology. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1992; 49:615-623Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

22. Deeter T, Prine D: ITBS and ITED Assessment Results 1994-1995: Focus on Standardized Testing. Des Moines, Iowa, Des Moines Public Schools, 1996, p 27Google Scholar

23. Rey A: L’examen clinique en psychologie. Paris, Presses Universitaires de France, 1964Google Scholar

24. Osterrieth PA: Le test de copie d’une figure complex: contribution à l’étude de la perception et de la memoire. Arch Psychol 1944; 30:286-350Google Scholar

25. Rey A: L’examen psychologique dans les cas d’encephalopathietraumatique. Arch Psychol 1941; 28:286-340Google Scholar

26. Benton A, Hamsher K: Multilingual Aphasia Examination. Iowa City, University of Iowa Press, 1976Google Scholar

27. Golden CJ: Stroop Color and Word Test: A Manual for Clinical and Experimental Uses. Wood Dale, Ill, Stoelting Co, 1978Google Scholar

28. Matthews C, Klove H: Instructional Manual for the Adult Neuropsychology Test Battery. Madison, University of Wisconsin Medical School, 1964Google Scholar

29. Wechsler D: Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—Revised, Manual. Cleveland, Psychological Corp, 1981Google Scholar

30. Gibbons R, Hedeker D, Elkin I, Waternaux C, Kraemer H, Greenhouse J, Shea M, Imber S, Sotsky S, Watkins J: Some conceptual and statistical issues in analysis of longitudinal psychiatric data: application to the NIMH Treatment of Depression Collaborative Research Program dataset. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1993; 50:739-750Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

31. Huttenlocher P: Synaptic density in human frontal cortex: developmental changes and effects of aging. Brain Res 1979; 163:195-205Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

32. Huttenlocher P, Dabholkar A: Regional differences in synaptogenesis in human cerebral cortex. J Comp Neurol 1997; 387:167-178Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

33. Chugani HT, Phelps ME, Mazziotta JC: Positron emission tomography study of human brain functional development. Ann Neurol 1987; 22:487-497Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

34. Giedd J, Blumenthal J, Jeffries N, Castellanos F, Liu H, Zijdenbos A, Paus T, Evans A, Rapoport J: Brain development during childhood and adolescence: a longitudinal MRI study. Nat Neurosci 1999; 2:861-863Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

35. Feinberg I: Schizophrenia: caused by a fault in programmed synaptic elimination during adolescence? J Psychiatr Res 1982-1983; 17:319-334Google Scholar

36. Rapoport J, Giedd J, Blumenthal J, Hamburger S, Jeffries N, Fernandez T, Nicolson R, Bedwell J, Lenane M, Zijdenbos A, Paus T, Evans A: Progressive cortical change during adolescence in childhood-onset schizophrenia: a longitudinal magnetic resonance imaging study. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1999; 56:649-654Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar