The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×
ArticleFull Access

A Neuropsychological Investigation of Prefrontal Cortex Involvement in Acute Mania

Abstract

OBJECTIVE: Mania has received little attention from a contemporary neuropsychological perspective despite its clear resemblance to the disinhibition syndrome sometimes seen after frontal brain injury, particularly injury to the inferior aspect of the prefrontal cortex. The purpose of this investigation was to describe the neuropsychological profile of severe acute mania by using a range of tasks selected primarily for the detection of localized neural disruption within the prefrontal cortex. METHOD: Fifteen acutely manic inpatients were compared with 30 nonpsychiatric subjects on tasks from the Cambridge Automated Neuropsychological Test Battery (Tower of London, spatial working memory, intradimensional-extradimensional attentional shift, and rapid visual information processing tasks) and on the Iowa Gambling Task, Stroop Color and Word Test, a verbal fluency task, and the California Verbal Learning Test. RESULTS: Discriminant function analysis identified deficits in sustained attention (on the rapid visual information processing task) and verbal learning (on the California Verbal Learning Test) as the best indicators of manic performance, rather than deficits on any of the tests of executive functioning. The model correctly classified 91% of subjects overall and 87% of manic subjects. Manic patients did not resemble patients with ventromedial prefrontal cortex damage in their performance on the Iowa Gambling Task. CONCLUSIONS: Acute mania is characterized by core deficits in verbal memory and sustained attention against a background of milder impairments in functions that are traditional measures of prefrontal cortex integrity (attentional set shifting, planning, working memory). The data do not implicate ventral prefrontal cortex disruption as a locus of pathology in acute mania. Verbal memory and sustained attention deficits may relate differentially to the state and trait characteristics of bipolar disorder.

The extremes of manic excitement, pressure of speech, grandiosity, and lack of judgment can appear as profound, but transient, disturbances in the behavior of otherwise well-organized and successful individuals. It seems unequivocal that disturbance of brain function underpins these characteristics of mania. Neuropsychological paradigms offer fresh insight into the links between symptoms, elemental cognitive functions, and the underlying neural substrates of abnormal mental states. If we assume a core abnormality in mania, then there should be consistent effects on some functional domains and perhaps more variable effects on neuropsychological functions that are less closely yoked to the core disturbance of mood and activation.

The renewed emphasis on identifying elemental psychological functions (1) has led to the development of improved tests, especially tests of functions believed to be subserved by the prefrontal cortex, such as attention, working memory, planning, set learning, set shifting, and decision making. The effects of lesions provide a starting point for what we should expect of neuropsychological investigation in mood disorder. Lesions of the left frontal cortex have been reported to be associated with depressed mood, and lesions of the right frontal cortex with mood elevation (2). A second emphasis has been on lesions of the inferior medial prefrontal cortex regardless of laterality, which appear to result in a disinhibition syndrome characterized by impulsivity, distractibility, and poor decision making (3). These components of the frontal lobe syndrome are not clearly associated with disturbances of subjective or objective mood, but the overlap in apparent symptoms suggests that mania may have an inferior prefrontal component.

Imaging studies completed in bipolar disorder patients in the manic phase have suggested prefrontal involvement without giving clear evidence of laterality (4). Neuroimaging research in mania is difficult because of the lack of cooperation from patients, and the data currently available are highly preliminary. Our own within-subject comparison of patients before and after lithium withdrawal suggested that the symptoms of mania are associated with increased perfusion in the orbital and supragenual medial frontal cortex (5).

Specific neuropsychological investigations of patients with mania are just as limited at present. The existing findings suggest a pattern of generalized deficit across mnemonic and executive domains in mania (69). However, the clinical state of the manic groups has often been inadequately described, and studies have commonly relied on coarse tests that conflate several cognitive processes, such as the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (10, 11). In the neuropsychological study of mania reported here, we capitalized on the more recent development of tests for specific aspects of executive function by using items from the Cambridge Automated Neuropsychological Test Battery (12) and the Iowa Gambling Test (3). The latter test is believed to be specifically sensitive to damage in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex.

Method

Subjects

The study subjects were all inpatients with mania at the time of testing. They were identified through routine visits to the wards at a university-affiliated psychiatric hospital and consultation with ward staff. Thirty-one patients considered suitable by ward staff were approached. Four patients were no longer considered sufficiently manic to be included, and three patients refused to participate. Testing was abandoned for nine patients who agreed to participate, typically because the patients could not be engaged on the tests long enough to derive meaningful data. The remaining 15 patients completed the protocol.

The patients ranged in age from 18 to 60 years and met DSM-IV criteria for bipolar I disorder, manic episode. A history of head injury was an exclusion criterion. Four subjects had a history of substance abuse within the past 6 months (polydrug abuse for two, and cannabis abuse for two) but were drug free at the time of testing. Two subjects had received ECT previously but not within the past year.

Fourteen of the 15 patients were receiving neuroleptic medication at the time of testing. In addition to taking neuroleptic medication, four patients were receiving lithium, valproate, carbamazepine, or a combination of these medications. Three patients were receiving the anticholinergic procyclidine, and six patients were receiving a benzodiazepine, typically lorazepam.

Thirty healthy comparison subjects were recruited by using a psychology subject panel, advertisements, and word of mouth. The comparison subjects had no psychiatric history and no first-degree relatives with bipolar diagnoses. The study was approved by the local research ethics committee, and after complete description of the study to the subjects, written informed consent was obtained.

Procedure

An important consideration in testing the manic patients was how soon after admission to approach the patients. All nine of the patients for whom testing was abandoned were approached within 1 week of admission. Ten of the 15 subjects were tested within 1 month of admission, and the remaining five patients were in their second or third month in the hospital and had suffered a fluctuating pattern of mood disturbance.

Testing of the manic patients was often split over two consecutive days, in which case an effort was made to test the patients at roughly the same time each day. The subjects’ mood was formally assessed with the Young Mania Rating Scale (13) and the Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (14). Three measures were used to assess intelligence level in the two groups: years of education; score on the National Adult Reading Test (15), a measure of premorbid verbal IQ; and score on the block design subtest of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS-R) (16), an approximation of performance IQ.

The subjects were tested with an extensive neuropsychological test battery that included both computerized and pen-and-paper tasks. The pen-and-paper tasks included a verbal fluency task (naming words beginning with the letters C, F, and L and naming animals and household items) (17), the Stroop Color and Word Test (17), and the California Verbal Learning Test (18). The computerized tasks were the Iowa Gambling Task (3) and four tasks from the Cambridge Automated Neuropsychological Test Battery (12): the Tower of London task (a test of executive function) and tasks testing spatial working memory, intradimensional-extradimensional attentional shift, and rapid visual information processing. The computerized tests were run on a portable Datalux 486 personal computer with a 10 1/2 inch touch-screen monitor. Brief descriptions of the tasks and the variables of interest are given in Table 1. The tests were administered in a fixed order.

Statistical Analyses

The demographic and clinical characteristics of the manic patient group and the comparison group were compared by using t tests for independent samples. Neuropsychological data were tested for conformity to a normal distribution by using the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test. Data for the intradimensional-extradimensional attentional shift task and the rapid visual information processing response bias failed to conform to a normal distribution, and these results were log transformed to normalize them (the raw data are presented in Table 2). Effect sizes were calculated for untransformed neuropsychological data by using the formula (m1–m2)/s, where m1 and m2 are the means for the two groups, and s is the pooled standard deviation (19). A stepwise discriminant function analysis was performed to examine the strongest neuropsychological discriminators of the manic and comparison groups, with prior probabilities adjusted in accordance with the differing group sizes. A threshold of F=3.84 was used to enter variables in the model, and F=2.71 was used to remove variables. Finally, Pearson’s correlations were calculated to test for associations between the neuropsychological and clinical variables.

Results

The demographic and clinical characteristics of the two groups are shown in Table 2. The two groups were closely matched with respect to age and years of education; most had a university-level education. However, the patient group had significantly lower scores than the comparison group on the National Adult Reading Test and the WAIS-R block design test. These variables were included in the discriminant function analysis, but they failed to enter the model. All of the manic patients had ratings above 12 on the Young Mania Rating Scale (mean=21.1, SD=6.4). Only one patient was experiencing a first episode of mania. The other patients had a mean of 2.8 previous hospitalizations for manic illness (SD=2.6, range=0–9), and a mean length of time since initial diagnosis of 106.1 months (SD=117.8).

The subjects’ neuropsychological performance is summarized in Table 3. The mean values indicate poorer performance in the manic study group on every dependent variable, although the effect sizes vary considerably. Missing data complicated the discriminant function analysis: three manic subjects refused to complete the spatial working memory task (the final test in the battery), and one manic subject refused to complete the Tower of London test (the penultimate test). To include these subjects and tests in the analysis, two separate analyses were performed with the missing values replaced in different ways: 1) by using the mean values for the two study groups combined (a more conservative method for detecting discrimination) and 2) by using the mean values for the manic study group (a less conservative method). Both analyses indicated the same discriminant function: only two variables reached inclusion criteria for the model. The F values presented in Table 3 are from the second analysis. At step 1, the California Verbal Learning Test learning score entered (Wilks’s lambda=0.49, F=45.65, df=1, 43, p<0.00001), correctly classifying 84.4% of the subjects overall and 73.3% of the manic subjects. At step 2, the accuracy score for the rapid visual information processing task entered (Wilks’s lambda=0.41, F=7.79, df=2, 42, p<0.00001). The discriminant model correctly classified 91.1% of the subjects overall and 86.7% of the manic subjects. No additional variables met entry criteria at step 3.

A closer examination (post hoc) of results for the California Verbal Learning Test and the rapid visual information processing task revealed a significant interaction of learning trial (five levels, within subjects) and diagnosis (two levels, between subjects) on the California Verbal Learning Test (repeated measures analysis of variance [ANOVA]: F=5.51, df=4, 172, p=0.001) such that the manic subjects displayed less improvement with list repetition relative to comparison subjects. This finding suggests impairment in the encoding or consolidation of information into memory in the manic patients. For the rapid visual information processing task, which lasts 7 minutes, a repeated measures ANOVA revealed that the interaction of time (seven levels, within subjects) and diagnosis (two levels, between subjects) was not significant, i.e., the decrement in performance over time was no more pronounced in the manic subjects than in the comparison subjects.

Neuropsychological Performance and Medication

The effects of medication on performance in the manic group were analyzed in two ways. First, as 14 of 15 subjects were receiving neuroleptic medication, it was possible to examine correlations between neuroleptic dose (in chlorpromazine equivalents) and neuropsychological performance. Increasing neuroleptic dose was associated (albeit nonsignificantly) with improved performance on the rapid visual information processing task (r=0.36, df=13, p=0.19) but was not associated with California Verbal Learning Test performance (r=–0.04, df=13, p=0.96). Second, in light of the established effects of benzodiazepines and anticholinergic drugs on cognition (20, 21), performance on the California Verbal Learning Test and the rapid visual information processing task were examined in a subgroup of manic patients who were free of these medications (N=6). For this subgroup, the mean California Verbal Learning Test learning score was 47.0 (SD=10.5) and the mean target sensitivity score for the rapid visual information processing task was 0.88 (SD=0.04). These values were very similar to the scores for the manic study group overall (compare with Table 3). It is therefore unlikely that the deficits in verbal learning and sustained attention could be explained by medication status in the manic study group.

Neuropsychological Performance and Clinical Variables

Correlation coefficients were calculated for the association between neuropsychological performance and mood ratings on the Young and Hamilton mood rating scales for the manic patients. The score on the Young Mania Rating Scale correlated with impairment only on the intradimensional-extradimensional attentional shift task (r=0.64, df=13, p<0.01). No neuropsychological variables correlated significantly with either number of hospitalizations or duration of illness (all p>0.10).

Discussion

The manic phase of bipolar disorder is characterized by impaired performance across a range of tasks assessing executive, mnemonic, and attentional functioning. In the present study, a discriminant function analysis demonstrated that two measures, verbal learning (the California Verbal Learning Test) and sustained attention (the rapid visual information processing task), were particularly powerful indicators of the deficit in mania, correctly classifying 87% of manic subjects and 91% of subjects overall. The results of comparisons between the manic patients and the healthy subjects on the remaining five tests had effect sizes in the range of 0.5–1.0, representing moderate to large effects, but these measures did not significantly contribute to the classification of the manic subjects, suggesting that they do not represent core markers of the manic state.

A central hypothesis of our investigation was that the performance on the Iowa Gambling Task of the manic subjects would resemble that of patients with lesions to the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (3). Although the manic subjects on average selected more cards from the risky decks than did the comparison subjects, they nonetheless chose more cards from the safe decks than from the risky decks overall and therefore did not closely resemble the patients with ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions, who virtually ignore the safe decks. The differences between the manic subjects and the comparison subjects on the Iowa Gambling Task may be more readily explainable in terms of delayed acquisition, quite feasibly as a result of poor sustained attention or learning. Lesions to ventral aspects of the prefrontal cortex are also associated with impaired reversal learning (22), an aspect of attentional processing assessed with the intradimensional-extradimensional attentional shift task. The manic subjects were impaired at several stages of this task, and it is suggested that this profile may be better accounted for by distractibility and impaired set learning than by a specific deficit in reversal learning. In conclusion, we were unable to demonstrate that the ventral prefrontal cortex was a major locus of dysfunction in acute mania.

Lateralized dysfunction of the prefrontal cortex has frequently been proposed as an underlying process in pathological affective states. The present data on sustained attention are consistent with a right prefrontal cortex abnormality in the manic state. Functional neuroimaging studies with the Continuous Performance Test, including the rapid visual information processing task, have consistently revealed a right-lateralized frontoparietal network of activation (23, 24), and evidence from human subjects with lesions in this area corroborates the view that right frontal but not left frontal lesions disrupt sustained attention (25). Furthermore, the task that showed the smallest effect size in the present study was verbal fluency, which has typically been associated with a left-lateralized prefrontal locus (17, 26). Other diverse lines of evidence support right prefrontal involvement in mania. Secondary mania after brain injury is more commonly associated with right frontal lesions than with left frontal lesions (2). A preliminary study employing transcranial magnetic stimulation has reported that right but not left frontal transcranial magnetic stimulation may relieve symptoms of mania (27). Finally, right-sided brain lesions in rats may have preferential effects on noradrenergic and dopaminergic neurotransmission, selectively resulting in behavioral overactivity (28) and attenuated physiological activation to stress (29).

It is less likely, however, that a core impairment in verbal learning in the manic state is also compatible with a right-lateralized prefrontal abnormality. The manic subjects showed pronounced impairment at the retrieval stages on the California Verbal Learning Test, but they also showed encoding difficulties. Their rate of learning was reduced, and their recognition ability was deficient. Encoding impairments on memory tasks are often interpreted in terms of medial temporal lobe dysfunction, while retrieval deficits may localize more frontally (30). Altered temporal cortex metabolism has been reported in functional neuroimaging studies in mania, consistent with this account (31, 32). However, imaging studies of mnemonic processing have highlighted contributions of the prefrontal cortex to both encoding (left-lateralized) and retrieval (right-lateralized) (33). Furthermore, in light of the verbal nature of the California Verbal Learning Test, difficulties on the test suggest disruption of left hemisphere processes. The neural substrates of the mnemonic impairment in acute mania therefore require further elucidation, potentially through functional neuroimaging.

The manic study group was small, because recruiting and testing, as always, was difficult. The majority of previous studies had patient group sizes in the range of 14–20 subjects (610, 34). The manic subjects in the present study constituted a severely symptomatic group, with an average Young Mania Rating Scale score above 20, and suffered from few depressive symptoms. These data therefore reflect the effects of pure mania. Others have provided preliminary evidence of subtle neuropsychological differences between patients with mixed and pure mania (35). In our study, 15 manic subjects completed the research. An additional nine subjects were fully cooperative, but meaningful data could not be obtained from them. It is almost an additional defining feature of severe acute mania that patients cannot be engaged in formal testing.

Sustained attention and mnemonic impairments have been noted in previous studies in manic groups. Sax and colleagues (34) have reported a correlation in manic patients between sustained attention deficit and prefrontal and hippocampal volumes measured by magnetic resonance imaging. This correlation raises the interesting possibility that trait dysfunction in sustained attention may exist in bipolar disorder, as structural changes should be temporally stable. Preliminary data from our laboratory that have resulted from testing euthymic patients with bipolar disorder have indicated this is the case. Indeed, of the tasks employed in the present investigation, only sustained attention, measured with the rapid visual information processing task, remained impaired during the euthymic phase of bipolar disorder, when the analysis controlled for low levels of affective symptoms (36). The trait nature of the attentional abnormality does not lend itself to a simple interpretation, since the postulated changes in monoamine function in mania might be expected to modulate attention as amphetamine does in normal subjects (37).

Memory impairments have also been described in earlier reports in acute mania (6, 8, 9), and verbal memory impairments have been robustly described in schizophrenia and unipolar depression (38, 39). In contrast to the clear relevance of sustained attention to distractibility in mania, the relationship between memory impairment and the symptoms of mania is not immediately obvious. The reversibility of memory impairment in the euthymic state indicates that it is a state-related marker and implies a distinct neural substrate from that supporting sustained attention.

Significant impairments in manic groups relative to healthy comparison subjects on executive measures such as the Tower of London task (8, 9), the Stroop Color and Word Test (7), and the Cambridge Automated Neuropsychological Test Battery spatial working memory task (9) have previously been reported. Although our findings support a weakness in executive processing in mania, executive tasks did not classify manic subjects as reliably as the California Verbal Learning Test and rapid visual information processing tasks. Consistent with this proposal, McGrath et al. (7) reported significant impairment on a summary measure of executive (frontal) performance but results only approaching significance on three of the four individual tests (the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, the Trail Making Test, and the verbal fluency task). The smaller effect sizes on executive tests in manic subjects may be explained in various ways, e.g., the prefrontal cortex may be only distantly related to the core pathology in mania, or it may be disrupted in only a subset of patients. The issue of “process purity” may also be important here: sustained attention and learning are component processes in many neuropsychological tests and thus deficits in these processes may lead to apparent generalized impairment.

In summary, deficits in verbal learning and sustained attention were capable of discriminating manic and normal comparison subjects better than measures of executive processing. A reduced capacity for sustained attention appears to be a trait abnormality in patients with bipolar I disorder and a clue perhaps to a key stable abnormality in the phenotype. Reduced episodic memory performance is very prominent in mania and depression, and its putative neurobiology is a neglected target for understanding of the state transitions in severe mood disorder.

TABLE 1
TABLE 2
TABLE 3

This research was presented at the Summer Meeting of the British Association for Psychopharmacology, Cambridge, U.K., July 16–19, 2000. Received July 19, 2000; revisions received Feb. 2 and March 13, 2001; accepted April 16, 2001. From the Departments of Psychiatry and Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford, U.K. Address reprint requests to Dr. Goodwin, University Department of Psychiatry, Warneford Hospital, Oxford OX3 7JX, U.K.; (e-mail). Supported by a Medical Research Council studentship to Mr. Clark. The authors thank Dr. A. Bechara and Prof. A. Damasio for use of the Iowa Gambling Task.

References

1. Shallice T: From Neuropsychology to Mental Structure. New York, Cambridge University Press, 1988Google Scholar

2. Robinson RG, Boston JD, Starkstein SE, Price TR: Comparison of mania and depression after brain injury: causal factors. Am J Psychiatry 1988; 145:172-178LinkGoogle Scholar

3. Bechara A, Damasio AR, Damasio H, Anderson SW: Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition 1994; 50:7-15Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

4. Goodwin GM: Neuropsychological and neuroimaging evidence for the involvement of the frontal lobes in depression. J Psychopharmacol 1997; 11:115-122Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

5. Goodwin GM, Cavanagh JT, Glabus MF, Kehoe RF, O’Carroll RE, Ebmeier KP: Uptake of 99mTc-exametazime shown by single photon emission computed tomography before and after lithium withdrawal in bipolar patients: associations with mania. Br J Psychiatry 1997; 170:426-430Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

6. Bulbena A, Berrios GE: Cognitive function in the affective disorders: a prospective study. Psychopathology 1993; 26:6-12Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

7. McGrath J, Scheldt S, Welham J, Clair A: Performance on tests sensitive to impaired executive ability in schizophrenia, mania and well controls: acute and subacute phases. Schizophr Res 1997; 26:127-137Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

8. Murphy FC, Sahakian BJ, Rubinsztein JS, Michael A, Rogers RD, Robbins TW, Paykel ES: Emotional bias and inhibitory control processes in mania and depression. Psychol Med 1999; 29:1307-1321Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

9. Sweeney JA, Kmiec JA, Kupfer DJ: Neuropsychologic impairments in bipolar and unipolar mood disorders on the CANTAB neurocognitive battery. Biol Psychiatry 2000; 48:674-685Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

10. Morice R: Cognitive inflexibility and pre-frontal dysfunction in schizophrenia and mania. Br J Psychiatry 1990; 157:50-54Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

11. Tam WC, Sewell KW, Deng HC: Information processing in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder: a discriminant analysis. J Nerv Ment Dis 1998; 186:597-603Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

12. Elliott R, Sahakian BJ, McKay AP, Herrod JJ, Robbins TW, Paykel ES: Neuropsychological impairments in unipolar depression: the influence of perceived failure on subsequent performance. Psychol Med 1996; 26:975-989Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

13. Young RC, Biggs JT, Ziegler VE, Meyer DA: A rating scale for mania: reliability, validity and sensitivity. Br J Psychiatry 1978; 133:429-435Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

14. Hamilton M: A rating scale for depression. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1960; 23:56-62Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

15. Nelson HE, O’Connell A: Dementia: the estimation of premorbid intelligence using the National Adult Reading Test. Cortex 1978; 14:234-244Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

16. Wechsler D: Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—Revised. New York, Psychological Corp, 1981Google Scholar

17. Lezak MD: Neuropsychological Assessment. New York, Oxford University Press, 1995Google Scholar

18. Delis DC, Kramer JH, Kaplan E, Ober BA: California Verbal Learning Test. New York, Psychological Corp, 1987Google Scholar

19. Cohen J: Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences, 2nd ed. Hillsdale, NJ, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1988Google Scholar

20. Spohn HE, Strauss ME: Relation of neuroleptic and anticholinergic medication to cognitive functions in schizophrenia. J Abnorm Psychol 1989; 98:367-380Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

21. Curran HV: Benzodiazepines, memory and mood: a review. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1991; 105:1-8Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

22. Rolls ET, Hornak J, Wade D, McGrath J: Emotion-related learning in patients with social and emotional changes associated with frontal lobe damage. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1994; 57:1518-1524Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

23. Paus T, Zatorre RJ, Hofle N, Caramanos Z: Time-related changes in neural systems underlying attention and arousal during the performance of an auditory vigilance task. J Cogn Neurosci 1997; 9:392-408Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

24. Coull JT, Frackowiak RS, Frith CD: Monitoring for target objects: activation of right frontal and parietal cortices with increasing time on task. Neuropsychologia 1998; 36:1325-1334Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

25. Wilkins AJ, Shallice T, McCarthy R: Frontal lesions and sustained attention. Neuropsychologia 1987; 25:359-365Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

26. Phelps EA, Hyder F, Blamire AM, Shulman RG: FMRI of the prefrontal cortex during overt verbal fluency. Neuroreport 1997; 8:561-565Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

27. Grisaru N, Chudakov B, Yaroslavsky Y, Belmaker RH: Transcranial magnetic stimulation in mania: a controlled study. Am J Psychiatry 1998; 155:1608-1610LinkGoogle Scholar

28. Robinson RG: Differential behavioral and biochemical effects of right and left hemispheric cerebral infarction in the rat. Science 1979; 205:707-710Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

29. Sullivan RM, Gratton A: Lateralized effects of medial prefrontal cortex lesions on neuroendocrine and autonomic stress responses in rats. J Neurosci 1999; 19:2834-2840Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

30. Squire LR: Memory and Brain. Oxford, UK, Oxford University Press, 1987Google Scholar

31. O’Connell RA, Van Heertum RL, Luck D, Yudd AP, Cueva JE, Billick SB, Cordon DJ, Gersh RJ, Masdeu JC: Single-photon emission computed tomography of the brain in acute mania and schizophrenia. J Neuroimaging 1995; 5:101-104Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

32. Migliorelli R, Starkstein SE, Teson A, de Quiros G, Vazquez S, Leiguarda R, Robinson RG: SPECT findings in patients with primary mania. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 1993; 5:379-383Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

33. Tulving E, Kapur S, Craik FI, Moscovitch M, Houle S: Hemispheric encoding/retrieval asymmetry in episodic memory: positron emission tomography findings. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1994; 91:2016-2020Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

34. Sax KW, Strakowski SM, Zimmerman ME, DelBello MP, Keck PE Jr, Hawkins JM: Frontosubcortical neuroanatomy and the continuous performance test in mania. Am J Psychiatry 1999; 156:139-141LinkGoogle Scholar

35. Sax KW, Strakowski SM, McElroy SL, Keck PE Jr, West SA: Attention and formal thought disorder in mixed and pure mania. Biol Psychiatry 1995; 37:420-423Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

36. Clark L, Iversen SD, Goodwin GM: Sustained attention deficit in bipolar disorder. Br J Psychiatry (in press)Google Scholar

37. Jacobs D, Silverstone T: Dextroamphetamine-induced arousal in human subjects as a model for mania. Psychol Med 1986; 16:323-329Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

38. Saykin AJ, Gur RC, Gur RE, Mozley PD, Mozley LH, Resnick SM, Kester DB, Stafiniak P: Neuropsychological function in schizophrenia. Selective impairment in memory and learning. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1991; 48:618-624Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

39. Austin MP, Mitchell P, Wilhelm K, Parker G, Hickie I, Brodaty H, Chan J, Eyers K, Milic M, Hadzi PD: Cognitive function in depression: a distinct pattern of frontal impairment in melancholia? Psychol Med 1999; 29:73-85Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar