The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×

Abstract

Unlike most psychiatric diagnoses, posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) is defined in relation to a potentially etiologic event (the traumatic “stressor criterion”) that is fundamental to its conceptualization. The diagnosis of PTSD thus inherently depends on two separate but confounded processes: exposure to trauma and development of a specific pattern of symptoms that appear following the trauma. Attempts to define the range of trauma exposure inherent in the diagnosis of PTSD have generated controversy, as reflected in successive revisions of the criterion from DSM-III onward. It is still not established whether or not there are specific types of traumatic events and levels of exposure to them that are associated with a syndrome that is cohesive in clinical characteristics, biological correlates, familial patterns, and longitudinal diagnostic stability. On the other hand, the symptomatic description of PTSD is becoming more clear. Of three categories of symptoms associated with PTSD—intrusive memories, avoidance and numbing, and hyperarousal—avoidance and numbing appear to be the most specific for identification of PTSD. Research is now poised to answer questions about the relevance of traumatic events based on their relationship to symptomatic outcome. The authors recommend that future research begin with existing diagnostic criteria, testing and further refining them in accordance with the classic Robins and Guze strategy for validation of psychiatric diagnoses. In this process, diligent adherence to the criteria under examination is paramount to successful PTSD research, and changes in criteria are driven by empirical data rather than theory. Collaborations among trauma research biologists, epidemiologists, and nosologists to map the correspondence between the clinical and biological indicators of psychopathology are necessary to advance validation and further understanding of PTSD.

More than two decades ago, a major commentary on posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) opened with a statement about “a long-standing debate” over validity of the diagnosis (p. 267) (1) . The literature suggests the controversy has not cooled (26) . If anything, it appears to be intensifying with the push toward DSM-V (28) . Questions about the very existence of PTSD persist (5 , 711) . Although posttraumatic syndromes have been described for many centuries in various forms, PTSD is a relative newcomer in psychiatric diagnostic nomenclature with its debut in DSM-III in 1980. Subsequent changes in the diagnostic criteria have provoked heated debate, largely surrounding definition of the traumatic event that anchors the symptoms.

Establishing diagnostic criteria is an iterative process, beginning with a listing of clinical observations and revising these criteria as more empirical evidence is collected (12 , 13) . Traditional tests of the validity of these criteria entail demonstration of etiology, pathogenesis, illness course, including response to treatment, and familial patterns. Few diseases have one necessary and sufficient cause or pathognomonic indicators. Most, e.g., coronary artery disease, cancer, and Parkinson’s disease, result from a mixture of etiologies and complex pathogenesis (12 , 14) . Because the etiology and pathogenesis of most psychiatric disorders remain largely unknown, diagnostic validity in psychiatry is largely limited to follow-up studies of illness course, response to treatment, and family studies (12) .

Robins and Guze proposed a procedure for validation of psychiatric diagnoses nearly 40 years ago (15) . First adopted by DSM-III in 1980, this process revolutionized definitions of psychiatric disorders, most recently formalized in DSM-IV-TR. This validation procedure entails five investigative phases: 1) clinical description (including symptom profiles, demographic characteristics, and precipitating factors), 2) laboratory studies (including psychological tests and radiological, chemical, and postmortem findings), 3) separation from other disorders (through exclusion criteria), 4) follow-up studies (demonstrating diagnostic stability and treatment response), and 5) family studies (15) . A major strength of this approach is that it is agnostic and atheoretical regarding disease etiology. It avoids premature constraint to assumed etiologies and facilitates research exploring potential causal pathways. Diagnostic criteria have been validated by these methods for more than a dozen psychiatric disorders (e.g., schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, major depression, antisocial personality disorder, and alcohol use disorders). The remaining 200+ diagnoses in DSM-IV-TR, however, await completion of focused validation of their diagnostic criteria through these rigorous and systematic methods (14 , 16) .

PTSD is not among the diagnoses most completely validated by this process. This does not mean, however, that the diagnosis is necessarily invalid or that its criteria cannot be validated. To the contrary, we propose in this article that further validation research can suggest potential improvements based on empirical testing of proposed alternative diagnostic criteria yielding greater homogeneity in family patterns, longitudinal stability, biological indicators, and differentiation from other disorders.

Empirical Research on PTSD

In 1987, Breslau and Davis wrote, “There is as yet little empirical research on the validity of the diagnosis” of PTSD (p. 255) (17) . In 1988, a workshop convened by the National Institute of Mental Health outlined research needed to validate the diagnosis of PTSD (9) . It determined that existing criteria lacked validating evidence pertaining to characteristics of the traumatic stressor, coherence of the observed syndrome, and supporting evidence demonstrating a consistent longitudinal course, familial associations, and relationship to other syndromes.

Nearly two decades later, the Institute of Medicine (IOM) of the National Academy of Sciences reviewed existing evidence supporting diagnostic validity of PTSD through application of the five historical validation phases (18) . Although designed to be illustrative rather than comprehensive, the IOM report found considerable evidence supporting the first phase of clinical description. What was disappointing in our view was the limited research supporting the remaining four phases (biological markers, separation from other disorders, and follow-up and family studies).

The IOM report described inconsistent findings from major lines of investigation into PTSD. Hypothalamic pituitary adrenal (HPA) axis and brain imaging abnormalities appear contradictory and not necessarily specific to PTSD. Several studies indicate genetic predispositions, with PTSD heritability estimates ranging around 30% to 40%, but investigations of candidate genes, e.g., serotonin (5-HTT) and dopamine (DRD2, DAT), have yielded variable results. PTSD has been differentiated from other disorders only by expert conceptualization (19) but not in actual patients. Although apparent symptom overlap with other disorders may represent a potential threat to differentiation from other disorders required for diagnostic validation (3 , 4 , 7 , 2023) , methodological problems, especially nonadherence to established criteria, have limited this line of research. Comorbidity is typical for PTSD but also occurs with other valid diagnoses and does not automatically disqualify the diagnosis from validation (18) . Follow-up studies have examined the serial prevalence of PTSD over time, but few have presented findings on the longitudinal course of individual cases needed to determine diagnostic stability. Studies of familial associations in PTSD have been largely limited to family history obtained from probands rather than examining family members. PTSD research has demonstrated familial associations with other psychopathology but not PTSD.

The evidence for the other four diagnostic validation phases for PTSD is destined to remain limited until the evidence for the first phase—description of clinical characteristics—is more fully established. This first validation phase is, in our view, particularly important because it provides a specific set of defining clinical attributes as a foundation on which other phases (follow-up, family, exclusion, and biological studies) are based. Robins and Guze’s determination of valid diagnostic criteria for schizophrenia made possible a generation of research into its genetics and neurobiology. A similar solidification of the diagnosis of PTSD is needed to permit meaningful research into its causes and treatment.

The Trauma Criterion

The most controversial aspect of PTSD validity is paradoxically the organization of PTSD’s definition around a potentially causal event (the traumatic “stressor criterion”). Fundamental to PTSD conceptualization, this criterion creates formidable difficulties for clinicians and researchers alike (2 , 3 , 6 , 22 , 24 , 25) . PTSD differs from other psychiatric diagnoses by its inherent dependence on two distinct processes: first, exposure to trauma, and second, development of a specific pattern of symptoms in temporal and/or contextual relation to the traumatic event.

PTSD, as originally conceptualized and currently defined in DSM-IV-TR, occurs only in conjunction with trauma exposure (see summary of diagnostic criteria in Table 1 ). Criterion A specifies that the individual must have been sufficiently exposed to a stressor qualifying as traumatic to be a candidate for a PTSD diagnosis. Without sufficient exposure to a qualifying traumatic event, symptoms cannot be considered contributory to the diagnosis. Symptoms occurring in association with nonqualifying (possibly stressful but not classifiably traumatic) events (e.g., being fired, being sued) might alternatively be subsumed under the diagnosis of adjustment disorder (6) or could be part of depressive or anxiety disorders—or even simply distress that is not part of a recognizable psychiatric disorder.

By definition, PTSD may occur in association with a range of trauma types, e.g., natural disasters and terrorism, rape and other assaultive violence, military combat, and accidental injuries. Trauma types demonstrated most commonly associated with PTSD are rape, kidnapping, and torture (26) . The qualification of such extreme trauma is generally undisputed, but questions have arisen about whether other stressors should qualify, such as purely psychosocial events without immediate physical injury or threat (e.g., divorce, failing an important examination). Mounting pressure to broaden the definition of qualifying events has been countered by protestations over “bracket creep” (2 , 27) . Criteria have accordingly been altered in successive DSM editions. Previously defined as “markedly distressing to almost anyone,” a qualifying stressor now requires threat to “physical integrity”—i.e., to life or limb. Such trauma, however, does not have to be directly experienced; witnessing or “being confronted with” (hearing about a traumatic experience of a family member or other “close associate”) also qualifies. In addition, DSM-IV added a subjective component (response of intense fear, helplessness, or horror—criterion A2).

Leading trauma researchers have long recognized problems of confounding of descriptive characteristics (symptoms and other characteristics) with purported etiology (a traumatic stressor) (17 , 28 , 29) . Continuing debates over the trauma criterion have revolved around the type of qualifying traumatic event (and the requisite degree of exposure to it) and whether or not the definition should even require such an event, or even any stressor. One suggested solution is to simply remove the requirement of a stressful event from the definition. Two decades ago, researchers (17 , 28 , 29) recommended assessment of etiological agents and their effects independently to unconfound them operationally, allowing observable symptom patterns to clarify the types of events that are associated with PTSD. In this seemingly backward approach, the type of event would function as a dependent variable predicted from the associated symptoms.

Others, however, remain adamant that PTSD be defined around a traumatic event—the essence of what makes it distinctly PTSD (5 , 21 , 28 , 30) . Breslau and colleagues (21) noted that the link of symptoms to a specific traumatic event “transforms the list of PTSD symptoms into a distinct DSM disorder” (p. 574). They cautioned that severing the definitional link between the symptoms and a traumatic event distorts the integrity of the diagnosis. Breslau mused (30) , “Without exposure to trauma, what is posttraumatic about the ensuing syndrome?” (p. 927). We agree and whimsically propose that a syndrome following a nontraumatic stressor might more appropriately be named “poststressor stress disorder” and one associated with no identified stressor called “nonstressor stress disorder.”

Basing the definition of PTSD on a required traumatic event and an associated set of symptoms introduces causal complexities. The risk factors for exposure to a traumatic event may differ from those conferring the likelihood of psychiatric illness afterward. Attempts to assign causality to a syndrome defined in relation to two processes with different sets of risk factors are thus confounded. Breslau (30) noted, “Personality traits of neuroticism and extroversion, early conduct problems, a family history of psychiatric disorders, and preexisting psychiatric disorders are associated with increased risk for exposure to traumatic events” (p. 926). Thus, a portion of the psychopathology observed after trauma may simply represent an extension of the preexisting risk factors for exposure. We caution that a definition automatically assigning causality of the ensuing syndrome to the preceding traumatic event fails to allow alternate causal possibilities, oversimplifies relationships, and obscures the importance of scientific inquiry into causality.

Recently, Maier (31) concluded that current conceptualizations of PTSD widely embraced by professionals and the public alike promote inappropriate monocausal assumptions regarding traumatic events and associated mental health difficulties. Basing diagnostic definition on etiology, in his estimation, creates a tautology blurring pre-event, peri-event, and postevent factors. He stressed that defining PTSD etiologically deviates from the explicitly stated goals of diagnostic manuals to define diagnoses phenomenologically, avoiding etiological assumptions. Maier urged the field to shun simple causal models, instead promoting an understanding of PTSD as “a multifactorial disorder just like any other mental disorder, even when a single external factor still may be identified as particularly important and indispensable for the emergence of the disorder” (p. 105). We find these concerns vital to furthering PTSD research and establishing diagnostic validity.

Because causality is problematic to the definition of PTSD, yet the inclusion of a traumatic event is considered essential to PTSD, we propose an achievable solution through adoption of a descriptive approach to its definition that requires a traumatic event without invoking causal assumptions. DSM-IV-TR already does this, merely stating that the associated symptoms follow the event and refraining from assigning causality to the traumatic event: “The essential feature of posttraumatic stress disorder is the development of characteristic symptoms following exposure to an extreme traumatic stressor” (emphasis added) (p. 463). This definition allows research to proceed unimpeded to test etiological hypotheses from monocausal to complex causal models, ultimately enabling empirical findings rather than opinions to settle disputes. This subtle but important attention to wording of the relationship encourages research to examine untested causal assumptions. However, an inconsistency occurs two sentences later, with causality inadvertently ascribed: “The characteristic symptoms resulting from the exposure to the extreme trauma include…” (emphasis added). Although Maier considers the “A criterion” to be invalid and noncontributory to the diagnosis, for sociopolitical reasons, he ultimately recommended retaining the traumatic event within the definition of PTSD to preserve the obvious association of trauma with the symptoms. Thus, even in the minds of those who advocate definitional separation of stressor and symptom, the tie remains.

The Sept. 11, 2001, terrorist attacks introduced further complexities for interpretation of criterion A1 (32) . The burning Twin Towers could be viewed from a distance of miles; and both live and replayed television coverage brought graphic images into every home in real time and for days and weeks afterward. All of these September 11th experiences might conceivably be considered a form of witnessing. As written in DSM-IV-TR, the “witnessing” specification does not require one to be an eyewitness or even observe the scene from a circumscribed proximity. Numerous studies related to September 11th reported on the prevalence of PTSD among populations of Manhattan and Washington, DC, and surrounding areas, and in national samples as far away as Houston, Tex., and Los Angeles (3336) . Presumably, most of the people in these locations had not been personally present at the site and did not have a close friend or family member who was injured or killed or directly endangered by the attacks. Thus, they must have been considered PTSD candidates (and many were identified as having PTSD) through viewing television coverage of the attacks, hearing about it, or somehow perceiving that they too were endangered. The above studies carefully represented the findings not as PTSD but as “stress symptoms,” “symptoms of PTSD,” and “probable PTSD.” We agree with Silver and colleagues (36) , who cautioned that such symptoms do not necessarily imply psychopathology but may represent normal responses to an event of extreme proportions. The significance of PTSD “symptoms” outside the context of diagnosis is unclear.

Only 5% of a post-September 11th Manhattan sample assessed for PTSD resided south of Canal Street (within ∼1.5 miles of the World Trade Center); 37% “directly” witnessed the attacks, 11% said a family member or friend was killed in the attacks, and 11% reported involvement in rescue efforts (33) . These vague exposure categories did not differentiate, for example, between observing the attacks from the base of the towers and seeing them from several miles away or between losing a spouse and hearing that a distant friend or acquaintance had been killed in the attacks. Some 6% of people without direct exposures had “symptoms consistent with PTSD,” and those with exposures were about two or three times more likely to report such symptoms.

Empirical study is needed to test the associations between stressor types and related symptom patterns. Precise definitions for provisional categories of types of traumatic events and levels of exposure to them (e.g., direct threat to life or limb, eyewitness to an event, and indirect exposure through a loved one exposed to an event) must be adopted and operationalized to compare associated syndromes by exposure type, confirming and/or further refining the diagnostic criteria. Once this is accomplished, research can assess whether psychopathology following exposure to events qualifying as traumatic under DSM-IV-TR criteria can be differentiated from psychopathology in people without sufficient exposure or with exposure to a nontraumatic event they perceive as stressful. We agree with the admonishment by Weathers and Keane (5) that an excessively broad definition could “hinder research by increasing heterogeneity of participants” (p. 112). Although it remains to be tested, we suspect that nontraumatic stressors (e.g., being fired, being divorced, suffering a large financial loss) and distant exposures to traumatic events, such as hearing of injuries and deaths of far-away strangers, will not be found to yield an identical syndrome with similar prevalence to that following direct exposure to traumatic stressors such as surviving a plane crash.

Progress has been made toward investigating the effects of applying different criteria for traumatic events. Breslau and Kessler (24) empirically demonstrated an altered prevalence of traumatic events and PTSD resulting from DSM definition evolution. Changes between DSM-III-R and DSM-IV increased criterion A1-qualifying traumatic events by 59%, mostly related to hearing of the sudden unexpected death of a loved one. New application of the A2 criterion, however, limited the increase of criterion A (A1 and A2)-qualifying events to 22%. The A2 criterion added little to ultimate diagnosis, as very few people not endorsing intense fear, helplessness, or horror met the symptom threshold. Thus, it was suggested that the A2 criterion might be most useful as a screener to rule out cases unlikely to meet full criteria. Overall, 38% of DSM-IV-determined PTSD cases were attributable to definitional expansion by inclusion of new event types.

PTSD Symptoms

Although the trauma criterion has been controversial, research on the symptom criteria has made substantial progress. The diagnosis of PTSD requires assessment of 17 trauma-associated symptoms divided into three groups: B (requiring ≥ 1 intrusive memories of the event), C (requiring ≥ 3 avoidance and numbing responses), and D (requiring ≥ 2 hyperarousal symptoms). In DSM-III, PTSD included only the intrusion and avoidance/numbing symptom groups; the hyperarousal symptom group was not added until DSM-III-R in 1987. The group C criteria have been criticized as being relatively too stringent and restrictive for optimal definition of PTSD (37 , 38) . It has alternatively been argued that the group B and D criteria are relatively too sensitive to be of use in the differentiation of psychopathology from distress (38) .

Several research groups examined the relative contribution of symptom group criteria to the full diagnostic criteria for PTSD and concluded that the avoidance/numbing cluster (group C) is a strong determinant of PTSD (32 , 3742) . Traumatized individuals are usually two or more times as likely to meet groups B and D criteria than C criteria (37 , 38 , 40 , 41) . Breslau et al. (42) pointed out that relative to group B and D criteria, group C criteria present a higher threshold, effectively making group C a rate-limiting factor for the diagnosis. In part, this may be because the group C criteria require three-sevenths (43%) of avoidance/numbing symptoms, compared to group B, requiring one-fifth (20%) of intrusive reexperience symptoms, and group D, requiring two-fifths (40%) of hyperarousal symptoms. Based on the relative number of symptoms in each group, group B has a proportional advantage over groups C and D for fulfilling symptom group criteria. The relatively infrequent fulfillment of group C criteria, however, ultimately relates to individual symptom frequency. In traumatized populations, group C symptoms present less frequently than group B and D symptoms (28 , 37 , 40 , 41 , 43) .

Group C criteria may be more than just contributory to the diagnosis of PTSD as currently defined. Experts have observed that the C symptom group is “largely responsible” for the diagnosis (p. 516) (37) , “the critical one for a diagnosis of PTSD” (p. 16) (44) , and “central to the diagnosis of PTSD” (p. 59) (39) . Maes and colleagues (38) wrote that the diagnosis “relies only on the criterion C symptoms [and] symptoms belonging to criteria B and D are not needed” (p. 189). Previously, we observed that 94% of directly exposed Oklahoma City bombing survivors who met group C criteria (i.e., meeting ≥ 3 avoidance/numbing symptoms) met full DSM-III-R criteria, compared to (by definition) none of those not fulfilling group C criteria (40) . Further, symptom groups B and D by themselves, in the absence of fulfillment of group C criteria, did not predict PTSD. These and similar findings from bombing survivors in Nairobi, Kenya (45) , and studies of other researchers as noted above led us to conclude that, based on the organization of current diagnostic criteria and the manner of presentation of the symptoms in their trauma survivors, group C is a marker for PTSD.

Among Oklahoma City bombing survivors, we found that meeting group C criteria was also significantly associated with self-reports of preexisting psychopathology, postdisaster comorbidity, seeking mental health treatment, taking psychotropic medication, drinking alcohol to cope, and difficulties in functioning (40) . In the absence of group C, however, groups B and D did not predict these associated indicators of psychopathology. Group B and D symptoms were quite common, with about 80% of the sample meeting these symptom group criteria, compared to only 36% meeting C criteria and 34% meeting full PTSD criteria.

These findings have been confirmed by others. Breslau’s group (42) also found in community samples that meeting group C criteria after a traumatic event was associated with seeing a physician, taking medication for the disturbance, and functional impairment from posttraumatic symptoms. In a study of emergency room trauma survivors, Shalev’s group (46) also observed that group C symptoms predict psychiatric illness and comorbidity. In a study of injured survivors of a terrorist attack on a civilian bus, Shalev (47) further noted that the pervasiveness of B and D symptoms limited their specificity for PTSD. Given the saturation of B and D symptoms in traumatized populations and their lack of association with other indicators of psychopathology in the absence of group C among Oklahoma City bombing survivors, we concluded that the group B and D symptoms in general appear to represent normative responses which, by themselves, do not necessarily indicate psychopathology (40) . Thus, while the relatively common group B and D symptoms signify distress, the less prevalent group C cluster serves as a marker of psychopathology.

Relationships Between Traumatic Events and Symptoms

The relationship of symptoms to traumatic events, which persists in the thinking of most scholars of PTSD despite attempts to separate them, is useful for efforts to refine the specificity of the symptoms of PTSD. Symptoms not defined by their association with an event are destined to be nonspecific to an event of interest and to suffer inadvertent contamination from individuals who habitually endorse high levels of distress. Studies of symptoms without association with a specific event have been overwhelmingly unsuccessful in identifying a characteristic set of symptoms (4 , 7 , 2023) , demonstrating the essential nature of connecting symptoms to a traumatic event to define a coherent posttraumatic syndrome. Even the strongest association with an event, however, does not necessarily imply specific causality.

Taking care to evaluate posttraumatic symptoms according to criteria as currently specified by DSM-IV-TR may, on the surface, seem a subtle issue, but this distinction is critical to diagnostic validation and selection of the most homogeneous samples. Careful adherence to DSM-IV-TR will exclude unrelated symptoms not contextually tied to the event (e.g., requiring nightmares of the event rather than just any nightmares) or temporally tied to it (e.g., requiring new onset of insomnia, problems concentrating, or loss of interest after the event, rather than just the presence of these symptoms after the event).

Biological Observations

Confirming our own observations of the importance of relating traumatic events and subsequent symptoms (40) , Yehuda and McFarlane (23) clarified distinctions between psychopathology and normative distress following trauma and emphasized that most trauma survivors do not develop psychopathology. They concluded that demonstrated differences between the biology of PTSD and the biology of normative distress response support the concept of PTSD as a disorder distinct from distress. Biological findings they identified as distinguishing PTSD from normative distress are HPA axis abnormalities (decreased baseline cortisol and increased negative feedback regulation, suggesting HPA axis oversensitivity) and physiological, electrophysiological, and neurochemical aberrations in the regulation of sympathetic nervous system and other neuromodulatory systems.

These findings suggest that a vital step in advancing PTSD research is the convergence of nosological, epidemiological, and biological lines of investigation. Strong collaborations among traumatology biologists, epidemiologists, and nosologists will be needed to map the correspondence between the clinical and biological indicators of psychopathology and further differentiate them from normative responses to trauma. For example, epidemiologic evidence that avoidance and numbing represent a core aspect of psychopathology in PTSD suggests an approach to mapping biological findings, such as HPA axis and imaging abnormalities, to these clinical findings. In contrast, physiological correlates of ordinary stress response may be demonstrated in relation to intrusion and arousal alone. Differentiating the biology of normative trauma response and psychopathology following traumatic exposure through combined epidemiological and biological streams of investigation will be critical to the future understanding of PTSD.

The Demographics of PTSD

Just as biological studies form one test of the validity of PTSD, coherence across different demographics constitutes another potentially validating element. Population studies and disaster research have consistently shown that women have approximately twice the rates of PTSD of men (4850) .

PTSD has been documented in developed and undeveloped countries alike, in settings of war and mass violence and with endemic accidental trauma during peacetime. Attempts to compare PTSD in trauma survivors of different countries and continents have historically been limited by noncomparable research methods. Using structured diagnostic interviews, our team has documented remarkable cross-national similarities in PTSD between civilian groups in the United States (Oklahoma City) and Kenya (Nairobi) directly exposed to terrorist bombings (45) . The incidence of PTSD by gender was similar among survivors in the two countries (20%–30% in men and 40%–50% in women). In both sites, group C was strongly predictive of a diagnosis of PTSD. Both groups overwhelmingly depended on support from loved ones to help them cope. Secondarily, Nairobi, Kenya, survivors were inclined to seek comfort from their religious community, whereas Oklahoma City survivors favored medical treatment, medications, and alcohol for coping. Thus, the expression of PTSD itself was more similar than different in the two sites, but coping responses differed.

Conclusions

We draw several conclusions from existing research, including our own work. First, although methods of diagnostic validation have been clearly laid out, progress remains to be made toward validating the diagnosis beyond describing its core clinical characteristics. Second, although causal relationships are destined to be found between exposures to trauma and the development of PTSD, the specific nature of the causal pathways remains to be determined and is likely to be far more complex than a linear association. Much of the controversy in the literature may result from assuming simple causal relationships without conducting research examining causal relationships between trauma and symptom patterns. Only further multidisciplinary research can determine what types of events and level of exposure will yield a homogeneous syndrome that is consistent in clinical characteristics, biological correlates, familial patterns, and longitudinal diagnostic stability. Third, removing the trauma criterion from the diagnosis and examining nonspecific symptoms does not appear fruitful in yielding a coherent syndrome; the context of some type of trauma exposure is necessary to anchor the symptoms within the definition. Fourth, avoidance and numbing symptoms represent the core of psychopathology as currently written in DSM-IV-TR; intrusion and hyperarousal symptoms alone appear to represent emotional distress and by themselves do not differentiate distress from illness.

Diligence in faithful application of the criteria being studied is paramount to the success of future PTSD research. Previous research has often ignored or altered key features of the criteria, especially the required qualifying exposure to a defined traumatic event and assessment of symptoms specific to it. Simple symptom checklists are notorious for their potential to confuse psychopathology with normal reactions or other problems (3) . Their summed symptom scores and thresholds defining caseness fail to ensure fulfillment of the diagnostic algorithm. Basing putative diagnosis on number and intensity of symptoms endorsed rather than adherence to the algorithm jeopardizes the homogeneity of classification we seek. In this vein, Nemiah (51) thoughtfully emphasized the important difference between “PTSD symptoms” and “posttraumatic stress disorder,” which represent very different entities. While measuring symptoms may have useful applications, this alone cannot substitute for assessing full diagnostic criteria. What has already been accomplished dictates that we should continue to evaluate current PTSD criteria based on empirical research in order to decide whether and how to change them.

Received May 5, 2008; revisions received Aug. 16 and Oct. 3, 2008; accepted Oct. 3, 2008 (doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.08050644). From the VA North Texas Health Care System, Dallas; the University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center Department of Psychiatry and the Department of Surgery/Division of Emergency Medicine, Dallas; Massachusetts General Hospital, Boston; and Mt. Sinai School of Medicine, New York. Address reprint requests and correspondence to Dr. North, Program in Trauma and Disaster, Dallas VAMC, 4500 S. Lancaster Rd., Dallas, TX 75216; [email protected] (e-mail).

Dr. North reports grant funding from NIMH, NIAAA, and the VA and paid consulting relationships with Applied Research Associates and Cubic Corporation. Dr. Suris reports grant funding from the VA, Dallas VA Research Corporation, NARSAD, and the NIH National Center for Research Resources. Dr. Davis reports a paid consultation relationship with the Institute of Medicine. The opinions expressed in this article represent the authors’ opinions and not those of the Institute of Medicine. Dr. Smith reports no competing interests. The contents of this article do not represent the views of the Department of Veterans Affairs or the U.S. Government.

This work was supported by NIMH grant number 068853 to Dr. North.

The authors thank Dr. Barry Hong for his efforts in reading and commenting on a previous draft of this manuscript.

References

1. Horowitz MJ, Weiss DS, Marmar C: Diagnosis of posttraumatic stress disorder. J Nerv Ment Dis 1987; 175:267–268Google Scholar

2. McNally RJ: Progress and controversy in the study of posttraumatic stress disorder. Annu Rev Psychol 2003; 54:229–252Google Scholar

3. Rosen GM, Lilienfeld SO: Posttraumatic stress disorder: an empirical evaluation of core assumptions. Clin Psychol Rev 2007; 28:837–868Google Scholar

4. Spitzer RL, First MB, Wakefield JC: Saving PTSD from itself in DSM-V. J Anxiety Disord 2007; 21:233–241Google Scholar

5. Weathers FW, Keane TM: The criterion A problem revisited: controversies and challenges in defining and measuring psychological trauma. J Trauma Stress 2007; 20:107–121Google Scholar

6. Weathers FW, Keane TM: The crucial role of criterion A: a response to Maier’s commentary. J Trauma Stress 2007; 20:917–919Google Scholar

7. McHugh PR, Treisman G: PTSD: a problematic diagnostic category. J Anxiety Disord 2007; 21:211–222Google Scholar

8. Summerfield D: The invention of post-traumatic stress disorder and the social usefulness of a psychiatric category. BMJ 2001; 322:95–98Google Scholar

9. Robins LN: Steps toward evaluating post-traumatic stress reaction as a psychiatric disorder. J Appl Soc Psychol 1990; 20:1674–1677Google Scholar

10. Scott W: PTSD in DSM-III: a case in the politics of diagnosis and disease. Soc Problems 1990; 37:294–310Google Scholar

11. Sparr LF: Post-traumatic stress disorder: does it exist? Neurol Clin 1995; 13:413–429Google Scholar

12. Guze SB: Nature of psychiatric illness: why psychiatry is a branch of medicine. Compr Psychiatry 1978; 19:295–307Google Scholar

13. Susser E, Schwartz S: Prototypical cohort study, in Psychiatric Epidemiology: Searching for the Causes of Mental Disorders. Edited by Susser E, Schwartz S, Morabia A, Bromet EJ. New York, Oxford University Press, 2006, pp 91–107Google Scholar

14. Black KJ: Diagnosis, in Adult Psychiatry. Edited by Rubin EH, Zorumski CF. Malden, Mass, Blackwell Scientific, 2005, pp 15–23Google Scholar

15. Robins E, Guze SB: Establishment of diagnostic validity in psychiatric illness: its application to schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 1970; 126:983–987Google Scholar

16. Cloninger CR: Establishment of diagnostic validity in psychiatric illness: Robins and Guze’s method revisited, in The Validity of Psychiatric Diagnosis. Edited by Robins LN, Barrett JE. New York, Raven Press, 1989, pp 9–18Google Scholar

17. Breslau N, Davis GC: Posttraumatic stress disorder: the stressor criterion. J Nerv Ment Dis 1987; 175:255–264Google Scholar

18. National Academy of Sciences Institute of Medicine: Posttraumatic Stress Disorder: Diagnosis and Assessment. Washington, DC, National Academy Press, 2006Google Scholar

19. Keane TM, Taylor KL, Penk WE: Differentiating post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) from major depression (MDD) and generalized anxiety disorder (GAD). J Anxiety Disord 1997; 11:317–328Google Scholar

20. Bodkin JA, Pope HG, Detke MJ, Hudson JI: Is PTSD caused by traumatic stress? J Anxiety Disord 2007; 21:176–182Google Scholar

21. Breslau N, Chase GA, Anthony JC: The uniqueness of the DSM definition of post-traumatic stress disorder: implications for research. Psychol Med 2002; 32:573–576Google Scholar

22. Rosen GM, Taylor S: Pseudo-PTSD. J Anxiety Disord 2007; 21:201–210Google Scholar

23. Yehuda R, McFarlane AC: Conflict between current knowledge about posttraumatic stress disorder and its original conceptual basis. Am J Psychiatry 1995; 152:1705–1713Google Scholar

24. Breslau N, Kessler RC: The stressor criterion in DSM-IV posttraumatic stress disorder: an empirical investigation. Biol Psychiatry 2001; 50:699–704Google Scholar

25. Coyne JC, Thompson R: Posttraumatic stress syndromes: useful or negative heuristics? J Anxiety Disord 2007; 21:223–229Google Scholar

26. Breslau N, Kessler RC, Chilcoat HD, Schultz LR, Davis GC, Andreski P: Trauma and posttraumatic stress disorder in the community: the 1996: Detroit area survey of trauma. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1998; 55:626–632Google Scholar

27. Rosen GM: Traumatic events, criterion creep, and the creation of pretraumatic stress disorder. Scientific Rev of Ment Health Practice 2005; 3:39–42Google Scholar

28. Solomon SD, Canino GJ: Appropriateness of DSM-III-R criteria for post-traumatic stress disorder. Compr Psychiatry 1990; 31:227–237Google Scholar

29. Green BL: Defining trauma: terminology and generic stressor dimensions. J Appl Soc Psychol 1990; 20:1632–1641Google Scholar

30. Breslau N: Epidemiologic studies of trauma, posttraumatic stress disorder, and other psychiatric disorders. Can J Psychiatry 2002; 47:923–929Google Scholar

31. Maier T: Post-traumatic stress disorder revisited: deconstructing the A-criterion. Med Hypotheses 2006; 66:103–106Google Scholar

32. North CS, Pfefferbaum B: Research on the mental health effects of terrorism. JAMA 2002; 288:633–636Google Scholar

33. Galea S, Ahern J, Resnick H, Kilpatrick D, Bucuvalis M, Gold J, Vlahov D: Psychological sequelae of the Sept 11 terrorist attacks in New York City. N Engl J Med 2002; 346:982–987Google Scholar

34. Schlenger WE, Caddell JM, Ebert L, Jordan BK, Rourke KM, Wilson D, Thalji L, Dennis JM, Fairbank JA, Kulka RA: Psychological reactions to terrorist attacks: findings from the national study of Americans’ reactions to Sept 11. JAMA 2002; 288:581–588Google Scholar

35. Schuster MA, Stein BD, Jaycox L, Collins RL, Marshall GN, Elliott MN, Zhou AJ, Kanouse DE, Morrison JL, Berry SH: A national survey of stress reactions after the Sept 11, 2001, terrorist attacks. N Engl J Med 2001; 345:1507–1512Google Scholar

36. Silver RC, Holman EA, McIntosh DN, Poulin M, Gil-Rivas V: Nationwide longitudinal study of psychological responses to Sept 11. JAMA 2002; 288:1235–1244Google Scholar

37. Ehlers A, Mayou RA, Bryant B: Psychological predictors of chronic posttraumatic stress disorder after motor vehicle accidents. J Abnorm Psychol 1998; 107:508–519Google Scholar

38. Maes M, Delmeire L, Schotte C, Aleksander J, Creten T, Mylle J, Struyf A, Pison G, Rousseeuw PJ: Epidemiologic and phenomenological aspects of post-traumatic stress disorder: DSM-III-R diagnosis and diagnostic criteria not validated. Psychiatry Res 1998; 81:179–193Google Scholar

39. McMillen JC, North CS, Smith EM: What parts of PTSD are normal: intrusion, avoidance, or arousal? data from the Northridge, Calif, earthquake. J Trauma Stress 2000; 13:57–75Google Scholar

40. North CS, Nixon SJ, Shariat S, Mallonee S, McMillen JC, Spitznagel EL, Smith EM: Psychiatric disorders among survivors of the Oklahoma City bombing. JAMA 1999; 282:755–762Google Scholar

41. Norris FH: Epidemiology of trauma: frequency and impact of different potentially traumatic events on different demographic groups. J Consult Clin Psychol 1992; 60:409–418Google Scholar

42. Breslau N, Reboussin BA, Anthony JC, Storr CL: The structure of posttraumatic stress disorder: latent class analysis in two community samples. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2005; 62:1343–1351Google Scholar

43. Foa EB, Riggs DS, Gershuny BS: Arousal, numbing, and intrusion: symptom structure of PTSD following assault. Am J Psychiatry 1995; 152:116–120Google Scholar

44. Breslau N: The epidemiology of posttraumatic stress disorder: what is the extent of the problem? J Clin Psychiatry 2001; 62:16–22Google Scholar

45. North CS, Pfefferbaum B, Narayanan P, Thielman S, McCoy G, Dumont C, Kawasaki A, Ryosho N, Kim YS, Spitznagel EL: Comparison of post-disaster psychiatric disorders after terrorist bombings in Nairobi and Oklahoma City. Br J Psychiatry 2005; 186:487–493Google Scholar

46. Shalev AY, Freedman S, Peri T, Brandes D, Sahar T, Orr SP, Pitman RK: Prospective study of posttraumatic stress disorder and depression following trauma. Am J Psychiatry 1998; 155:630–637Google Scholar

47. Shalev AY: Posttraumatic stress disorder among injured survivors of a terrorist attack: predictive value of early intrusion and avoidance symptoms. J Nerv Ment Disord 1992; 180:505–509Google Scholar

48. Breslau N, Davis GC, Andreski P, Peterson EL, Schultz LR: Sex differences in posttraumatic stress disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1997; 54:1044–1048Google Scholar

49. Kessler RC, Sonnega A, Bromet E, Hughes M, Nelson CB: Posttraumatic stress disorder in the National Comorbidity Survey. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1995; 52:1048–1060Google Scholar

50. North CS: Trauma-related syndromes, in Adult Psychiatry. Edited by Rubin EH, Zorumski CF. Malden, Mass, Blackwell Scientific, 2005, pp 307–316Google Scholar

51. Nemiah JC: A few intrusive thoughts on posttraumatic stress disorder. Am J Psychiatry 1995; 152:501–503Google Scholar