The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×
ArticleFull Access

Project Among African-Americans to Explore Risks for Schizophrenia (PAARTNERS): Evidence for Impairment and Heritability of Neurocognitive Functioning in Families of Schizophrenia Patients

Abstract

Objective

Neurocognitive impairments in schizophrenia are well replicated and widely regarded as candidate endophenotypes that may facilitate understanding of schizophrenia genetics and pathophysiology. The Project Among African-Americans to Explore Risks for Schizophrenia (PAARTNERS) aims to identify genes underlying liability to schizophrenia. The unprecedented size of its study group (N=1,872), made possible through use of a computerized neurocognitive battery, can help further investigation of the genetics of neurocognition. The current analysis evaluated two characteristics not fully addressed in prior research: 1) heritability of neurocognition in African American families and 2) relationship between neurocognition and psychopathology in families of African American probands with schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder.

Method

Across eight data collection sites, patients with schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder (N=610), their biological relatives (N=928), and community comparison subjects (N=334) completed a standardized diagnostic evaluation and the computerized neurocognitive battery. Performance accuracy and response time (speed) were measured separately for 10 neurocognitive domains.

Results

The patients with schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder exhibited less accuracy and speed in most neurocognitive domains than their relatives both with and without other psychiatric disorders, who in turn were more impaired than comparison subjects in most domains. Estimated trait heritability after inclusion of the mean effect of diagnostic status, age, and sex revealed significant heritabilities for most neurocognitive domains, with the highest for accuracy of abstraction/flexibility, verbal memory, face memory, spatial processing, and emotion processing and for speed of attention.

Conclusion

Neurocognitive functions in African American families are heritable and associated with schizophrenia. They show potential for gene-mapping studies.

Neurocognitive impairments in schizophrenia are well established (1) and considered candidate endophenotypes that may facilitate understanding of genetics and pathophysiology (2). Attention, verbal memory, and working memory domains may meet key criteria as schizophrenia endophenotypes (3), including association with the illness, trait-like features, impairment in biological relatives, and heritability (4, 5). However, two important aspects of endophenotype candidacy need further elucidation.

First, it is unclear whether heritability estimates of endophenotypes, and their utility in the schizophrenia gene hunt, transfer across populations. Because the genetic basis of schizophrenia in the African American population likely derives from both Africa and Europe, studies of Europeans may not yield endophenotypes that are informative for genetics in African Americans (6). Although significant heritability of some neurocognitive domains has been reported in schizophrenia (2, 7), it appears that no studies have examined heritability in African American families. Heritability estimates of general cognitive ability can vary according to experimental design and study assumptions (8, 9). A meta-analysis of general cognitive ability (8) estimates its heritability to be 48%. Results from the few studies of African Americans (e.g., reference 10) are consistent with heritable variation for general cognitive ability. However, little is known about heritability of specific neurocognitive domains, particularly in African American schizophrenia patients and their families.

Second, endophenotype criteria specify cosegregation with illness in families (4). Thus, against a backdrop of impaired performance in relatives, individuals with schizophrenia and related disorders are more likely to exhibit the endophenotype than relatives with nonpsychotic disorders or no psychopathology. Meta-analytically derived effect sizes (11, 12) suggest that, on average, schizophrenia patients have greater neurocognitive impairment than their biological relatives, who are more impaired than comparison participants. However, the relationship between neurocognition and nonpsychotic psychopathology in relatives is not well delineated (11, 13). Because some neurocognitive impairments are observed in other nonpsychotic groups (e.g., mood disorders) (14, 15), estimates of neurocognitive deficits in relatives may be affected by inclusion of relatives with psychopathology, especially if compared to psychopathology-free controls (11). Yet inclusion of relatives with nonpsychotic illness is common in schizophrenia family studies, and there is no consensus on methods of investigating the relationship between neurocognition and psychopathology in such families (13).

Three strategies have been employed: 1) inclusion of all relatives without evaluating relationships between neurocognition and psychopathology, 2) a priori exclusion of relatives with psychopathology, and 3) post hoc exclusion of relatives with psychopathology to determine whether differences between relatives and comparison subjects are upheld. Because other disorders may share genetic susceptibility (16) and neurocognitive impairment (17) with schizophrenia, a risk of strategy 1 is that relatives with psychopathology may contribute to a misinterpretation of deficits as specifically associated with schizophrenia genetic liability, thus obscuring potential overlap among disorders. Conversely, strategy 2 risks excluding genetically informative individuals. Strategy 3 strengthens conclusions regarding impaired neurocognition in psychiatrically healthy relatives but does not explicitly inform understanding of neurocognition in relatives with nonpsychotic psychopathology. A solution is to subgroup relatives according to psychopathology and directly compare across disorders, but this approach requires large study groups. The only large-scale (N=214) study of which we are aware that employed this strategy (7) showed that family members with schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder and those with bipolar disorders were comparably impaired on several traditional neurocognitive tasks, whereas relatives with major depressive disorder were less impaired. Although informative, that study did not investigate other forms of psychopathology in relatives, and it examined a specific population (Latino) and thus requires replication in other populations.

The Project Among African-Americans to Explore Risks for Schizophrenia (PAARTNERS) addresses the aforementioned gaps. PAARTNERS is an eight-site investigation aimed at identifying schizophrenia liability genes. The large study group (N=1,872) and standardized clinical and neurocognitive data collection provide a unique opportunity for investigating heritability and the relationship between psychopathology and neurocognition in families. PAARTNERS employed a computerized neurocognitive battery adapted for use in large-scale multisite studies, and we believe that it provides the most comprehensive neurocognitive assessment in African Americans thus far (6).

Two related hypotheses were tested: 1) neurocognitive domains identified as candidate schizophrenia endophenotypes are heritable in African American families and 2) African American individuals with schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder and their relatives—both affected and unaffected with other forms of psychopathology—exhibit lower neurocognitive functioning than community comparison subjects, with the greatest impairment observed in individuals with psychotic disorders.

Method

PAARTNERS methods are detailed elsewhere (6) and will be outlined here. After complete description of the study, written informed consent was obtained. Research protocols were approved by each site's institutional review board for use of human subjects.

Participants

PAARTNERS recruited families of probands with schizophrenia, the depressed subtype of schizoaffective disorder, and the bipolar subtype of schizoaffective disorder. The probands were self-identified African Americans with at least a 6-month period (current or past) of meeting criteria for the psychotic disorder without substance abuse or dependence. Family members were selected to meet criteria for at least one of the following pedigree structures: affected sibling pair (proband and one or more siblings diagnosed with schizophrenia, schizoaffective-depressed, or schizoaffective-bipolar; N=70), trio (proband and either two parents or at least one additional sibling if a parent was unavailable; N=436), or multiplex (proband and one or more affected first-degree relatives and eight or more additional first- to fourth-degree relatives; N=28). In families with trios or affected sibling pairs, if a parent was unavailable, half-siblings were tested if they were children of the missing parent. Relatives unaffected with schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder had to be at least 15 years old, and probands and relatives with schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder had to be at least 18. During semistructured interviews with the 1,538 probands and relatives regarding ethnicity, 94.7% of the family members (N=1,457) identified one or more African American ancestors. The majority (3.9%, N=60) of the remaining participants had information on only 1 forbear, and no participants indicated that their ancestors were all of European descent.

Healthy community comparison subjects also self-identified as African American were group matched by geographical region to the schizophrenia patients and were targeted to include individuals in the same demographic groups (sex, age, maternal education) as the patients and relatives. They were initially identified and screened by a professional survey company employing a telephone list constructed by using zip codes in the eight geographical regions of the collaborating sites. Individuals thus identified were then contacted by the respective local sites for final eligibility determination and scheduling. To supplement this group, local recruitment of comparison subjects was also conducted at most sites through established mechanisms.

All participants were required to be proficient in English, provide informed consent, and not have mental retardation or physical conditions restricting their ability to participate in neurocognitive testing. Comparison subjects were excluded for current or past significant head injury, neurological or severe systemic illness that may affect neurocognitive functioning, current or past psychosis, another axis I disorder with less than 1 month of psychiatric stability, substance abuse in the past 1 month, substance dependence not in remission for 6 months, current or past treatment with antipsychotic agents, ECT in the past 6 months, or a history of psychosis in a first-degree family member or an unknown family history (e.g., due to adoption).

Diagnostic Assessment

DSM-IV diagnoses were assigned by using a lifetime best-estimate final diagnosis approach incorporating the Diagnostic Interview for Genetic Studies (18), the Family Interview for Genetic Studies (19), medical record review, and consensus review of all materials by at least two investigators. Quality assurance of diagnostic procedures was described previously (6).

Computerized Neurocognitive Battery

The computerized neurocognitive battery, developed and validated (20, 21) for large-scale studies, was administered by using clickable icons on desktop or laptop computers in a fixed order. The PAARTNERS battery, described in detail elsewhere (6), uses 14 tasks to assess 10 neurocognitive domains: 1) abstraction and mental flexibility—Abstraction and Working Memory Task (21, 22) and Penn Conditional Exclusion Test (23); 2) attention—Penn Continuous Performance Test, Number and Letter Version (24), Letter-N-Back, 0-back condition (25); 3) working memory—Letter-N-Back, 1- and 2-back conditions (25); 4) verbal memory—Penn List Learning Task, Computerized Penn Word Memory Test (26); 5) face memory—Penn Face Memory Test (26); 6) spatial memory—Visual Object Learning Test (27); 7) language—Penn Verbal Reasoning Test (20, 28); 8) spatial processing—Computerized Judgment of Line Orientation (20); 9) sensorimotor processing—Computerized Finger-Tapping Task (29) and Motor Praxis Test (29); and 10) emotion processing—Penn Emotion Recognition Test (30) and Penn Emotion Discrimination Task (31). Administration time was approximately 120 minutes, including rest. Scoring was automated, as was downloading to a central repository at the University of Pennsylvania. For each domain except sensorimotor processing, two summary functions were calculated: accuracy (number of correct responses) and speed (response time for correct answers). For sensorimotor processing, only the speed summary function was calculated.

Statistical Approach

Heritability

For the heritability analyses, the number of individuals with measurements varied between 1,271 and 1,432. Because heritability analysis requires complete valid pedigrees, for any incomplete pedigrees, intervening but unassessed relatives were added by using pedigree information collected from participating family members. Within our pedigrees, 1,432 individuals were assessed and 765 were added to generate complete pedigrees containing 2,197 individuals. Heritability for each neurocognitive domain was estimated by mixed-model analysis of variance. The model included fixed effects for site and diagnosis, covariates for sex and age, and random effects for individuals and residual variation. Estimates for genetic and residual components of variance were determined by means of restricted maximum likelihood (32). We applied the average information (33) algorithm, as implemented in AIREMLF90 (http://nce.ads.uga.edu/~ignacy/newprograms.html), because it guarantees faster convergence to a solution than does the more traditional expectation-maximization algorithm (34). Approximate standard errors for heritability estimates were determined by using the formulas of Gilmour et al. (35). A procedure based on Fisher's z transformation (36, 37) was used to obtain 95% confidence intervals for heritability estimates.

Group differences

Genetically related individuals are expected to show positive correlation for neurocognitive performance reflecting shared environmental and genetic variation. To account for relatedness, we used generalized estimating equation and mixed linear models with a family effect. Because the results were similar for the two types of models, only results from the generalized estimating equation models are reported. These specify the correlation structure among family members to correct for correlations among relatives. For each neurocognitive task, the correlation of siblings' scores was first estimated by fitting a generalized estimating equation model to data from only nominal full siblings. The estimated correlation from full siblings was taken as the approximation of the correlation for all relative pairs. While this correlation is expected to overestimate the correlation for other kinds of relative pairs, it provides a conservative correction for significance testing. For each function and domain, a significant type 3 generalized estimating equation analysis was followed by least squares mean difference tests to detect pairwise differences between groups. Effect sizes (d) were calculated and interpreted according to Cohen's guidelines: 0.2=small, 0.5=moderate, 0.8=large.

To evaluate the relationship between diagnostic status and neurocognitive measures, we employed two distinct encodings of diagnosis. First, we used a hierarchical, mutually exclusive encoding roughly approximating DSM-IV axis I classification. Under this traditional scheme, each individual is categorized into only one of seven diagnostic groups based on DSM-IV criteria, as in Table 1. This scheme does not allow for comorbidity of disorders; for example, a participant with both schizophrenia and a substance-related disorder would be categorized in the schizophrenia group and the substance disorder would be effectively ignored. Thus, we also employed a dichotomous (Bernoulli) encoding in which each individual received a value of 1 (diagnosis given) or 0 (diagnosis not given) for each of 10 diagnostic categories. This dichotomous coding scheme provides for actual DSM-IV diagnostic categories that are not mutually exclusive, allowing for a fuller evaluation of the association between "secondary" disorders (such as substance-related disorders) and neurocognition. The hypothetical participant would thus receive a value of 1 for schizophrenia, 1 for substance-related disorder, and if there are no other comorbid disorders, 0 for all other categories.

Table 1. Demographic Characteristics of Patients With Schizophrenia or Schizoaffective Disorder, Relatives, and Comparison Subjects (N=1,872)
Table 1. Demographic Characteristics of Patients With Schizophrenia or Schizoaffective Disorder, Relatives, and Comparison Subjects (N=1,872)
Enlarge table

Preparatory Analyses

For neurocognitive tests with skewed distributions of raw scores, log or Box-Cox transformations were performed to approximate normality. Scores were then standardized to z-scores by using the comparison subjects' mean and standard deviation. When a domain comprised multiple tests, a z-score average was computed. Because the original speed measure was time (milliseconds), we reversed the sign of the z-scores for speed to standardize the interpretation (higher value is better).

Results

For neurocognitive tests with skewed distributions of raw scores, log or Box-Cox transformations were performed to approximate normality. Scores were then standardized to z-scores by using the comparison subjects' mean and standard deviation. When a domain comprised multiple tests, a z-score average was computed. Because the original speed measure was time (milliseconds), we reversed the sign of the z-scores for speed to standardize the interpretation (higher value is better).

Demographic Characteristics

Table 1 presents the current PAARTNERS study group (491 families and 334 comparison subjects) with neurocognitive data. Because the participant groups differed in sex distribution and age (p<0.001), these variables were used as covariates in all models.

Heritability

Accuracy measures in all domains were significantly heritable; most estimates were greater than 0.30 (Figure 1). Notably, large heritabilities were observed in domains particularly implicated in schizophrenia, including abstraction/flexibility (h2=0.46), verbal memory (h2=0.50), and face memory (h2=0.44). The heritability estimates of the accuracy of spatial processing (h2=0.53), emotion processing (h2=0.44), and language (h2=0.43) were also high, although these abilities have not been widely investigated as candidate endophenotypes in schizophrenia. In the domain of attention, the heritability of accuracy was low (h2=0.13), but for speed it was high (h2=0.40). Other than attention, heritability estimates of speed were generally lower (h2 range=0.09–0.30) than those for accuracy (h2 range=0.13–0.53).

Figure 1. Heritability Estimates for Neurocognitive Functioning in African American Patients With Schizophrenia or Schizoaffective Disorder and in Relativesa

aTotal number of subjects for these analyses ranged from 1,271 to 1,432, while standard errors ranged from 0.04 to 0.05.

*p<0.05. **p<0.01. ***p<0.0001.

Neurocognition and Hierarchical Diagnoses

Analysis of variance indicated that, with the exception of language speed, the neurocognitive domains differed among the diagnostic groups. Given the large number of variables and groups, the results are depicted in two ways. Figure 2 provides a visual representation of mean z-scores for each diagnostic group. The complementary Table 2 separately gives the effect size of each pairwise group comparison for accuracy (above diagonal) and speed (below diagonal), as well as the results of significance tests comparing the group means. Entries in Table 2 are identified in this text by row and column as in a spreadsheet (e.g., row 1 in column G is referred to as 1G).

Figure 2. Mean Scoresa for Accuracy and Speed on Neurocognitive Tests for African American Patients With Schizophrenia or Schizoaffective Disorder and Relativesb

aStandardized to z-scores by using means and standard deviations for community comparison subjects.

bFamily members with substance-related disorders did not have schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder, major depressive disorder, or bipolar disorders. Those with "other disorders" had axis I or II diagnoses without schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder, major depressive disorder, bipolar disorders, or substance-related disorders. Unaffected family members had no psychopathology.

Table 2. Pairwise Effect Sizes and Least Squares Mean Difference Results for Neurocognitive Functioning in Patients With Schizophrenia or Schizoaffective Disorder, Relatives, and Comparison Subjects (N=1,872), Following Type 3 Generalized Estimating Equation Models of Differences Among Seven Diagnostic Groupsa
Table 2. Pairwise Effect Sizes and Least Squares Mean Difference Results for Neurocognitive Functioning in Patients With Schizophrenia or Schizoaffective Disorder, Relatives, and Comparison Subjects (N=1,872), Following Type 3 Generalized Estimating Equation Models of Differences Among Seven Diagnostic Groupsa
Enlarge table

Accuracy

The z-scores for accuracy are shown in the upper part of Figure 2. Least-squares mean differences indicate that the participants with schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder were less accurate than the community comparison subjects in all neurocognitive domains, with effect sizes ranging from small (–0.31) to large (–0.87) (Table 2, cell 1G in all domains).

The group with schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder was also less accurate than the unaffected relatives, relatives with major depressive disorder, and those with substance-related disorders in nearly all neurocognitive domains (Table 2, 1B, 1D, and 1F in all domains). Participants with schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder were less accurate than relatives with bipolar disorders (N=27; Table 2, 1C) in abstraction/flexibility, verbal memory, face memory, language, and emotion processing. In contrast, no significant difference was seen between schizophrenia/schizoaffective and bipolar disorder in working memory (d=–0.02) and spatial processing (d=–0.04), indicating some performance similarities in these groups. Like the bipolar group, the "other" group was quite small (N=28), but unlike the bipolar group, it differed from the schizophrenia/schizoaffective group only in attention and abstraction/flexibility (Table 2, 1E). The heterogeneous "other" group includes individuals with psychotic disorders; hence, it is expected to share some neurocognitive features with schizophrenia.

There were few significant differences in accuracy among relatives in the various diagnostic categories, reflecting similar levels of performance accuracy in relatives regardless of diagnosis (Table 2, 2C–F, 3D–F, 4E–F, 5F).

Across all diagnostic groups, relatives had significantly less accuracy than the community comparison subjects in spatial memory, working memory, spatial processing, and emotion processing (Table 2, 2G to 6G in each domain). All relatives except the bipolar group had impaired accuracy in abstraction/flexibility, verbal memory, and language. The relationship between group membership and performance accuracy was the weakest for face memory, where only relatives with bipolar disorder differed from community subjects (Table 2, 3G), and their performance was actually better than that of the community comparison subjects. An important finding was that the relatives who were unaffected with any disorder were less accurate than the comparison subjects in all domains except face memory, indicating that impairments are observable even in psychiatrically healthy relatives (Table 2, 6G).

Speed

Group comparisons for speed are displayed in the lower portion of Figure 2 and in the results below the diagonal for each domain in Table 2. Except for language, the patients with schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder were slower than the community comparison subjects across all domains (Table 2, 7A), most markedly in sensorimotor processing speed (d=–1.05).

The patients were also slower than the relatives with major depression in all domains except language (Table 2, 2A), and they were slower than relatives with substance-related disorders in all domains except language and working memory (Table 2, 4A). In contrast, they did not differ in speed from relatives with "other" disorders (Table 2, 5A), and they differed from relatives with bipolar disorders on only verbal memory and sensorimotor dexterity (Table 2, 3A).

As with accuracy, relatives with most forms of psychopathology were comparable in response times to each other and to unaffected relatives (Table 2, 3B, 4B–C, 5B–D, 6B) with a few exceptions.

Fewer differences between relatives and the community comparison subjects were observed in speed (Table 2, 7B–F) than in accuracy. The most consistent differences across groups were found for sensorimotor speed and spatial processing (where all groups were slower than the comparison subjects) and for emotion processing (where all relative groups except "other" showed slower response times than the comparison subjects). In addition, unaffected relatives were slower than comparison subjects (Table 2, 7F) in abstraction/flexibility, verbal memory, and spatial memory, while relatives with bipolar disorder (Table 2, 7C) and "other" disorders (Table 2, 7E) were slower in abstraction/flexibility and attention.

Neurocognition and Dichotomous Diagnoses

Analysis of variance related the 10 dichotomous encodings of diagnoses to the neurocognitive measures. In the form of a heat map, Figure 3 graphically shows the relationship between neurocognitive performance and the diagnostic categories not required to be mutually exclusive. Figure 3 shows that the schizophrenia diagnosis is most significantly associated with lower accuracy on the neurocognitive measures, followed by schizoaffective-depressed, schizoaffective-bipolar, and then the remaining diagnoses. An important observation is that although substance-related disorders are associated with cognitive impairments, schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorder have stronger associations, indicating that substance disorders do not account for the neurocognitive impairments observed in schizophrenia. The dichotomous coding analyses thus reveal a direct connection between neurocognitive accuracy and schizophrenia, even after accounting for diagnostic comorbidity.

Figure 3. Heat Mapa Showing Analysis of Variance Results for Accuracy and Speed on Neurocognitive Tests of African American Patients With Schizophrenia or Schizoaffective Disorder and Relatives in 10 Non-Mutually-Exclusive Diagnostic Categoriesb

aHeat scores were assigned on the basis of the p values and signs of differences between individuals with the diagnosis and the group mean. Heat was calibrated into p value bins: <0.001 is represented by dark red and light yellow, mapping to worst and best performance, respectively; 0.001 to <0.01 is represented by the next lighter shade of red and the next darker shade of yellow, mapping to somewhat better or worse performance; and so on. To arrange the heat scores, the difference between estimated means for each diagnostic group was calculated, producing a distance metric. The diagnostic groups were then ordered by their means, such that the group most similar to schizophrenia was placed next to schizophrenia and the least similar group was placed furthest away from schizophrenia in the graph. The neurocognitive domains were arrayed in a similar fashion.

bThe 10 dichotomous categories were coded as present or absent and included schizophrenia (N=507); schizoaffective disorder, depressed subtype (N=55); schizoaffective disorder, bipolar subtype (N=47); major depressive disorder (N=170); bipolar disorders I and II (N=27); substance-related disorders (N=435); other mood disorders (bipolar not otherwise specified [NOS], dysthymic disorder, depressive disorder NOS, mood disorder due to general medical condition, substance-induced mood disorder, and mood disorder NOS) (N=100); "nonorganic" psychotic disorders (delusional disorder, shared psychotic disorder, brief psychotic disorder, and psychotic disorder NOS) (N=20); "organic" psychotic disorders (alcohol-induced psychotic disorder, substance-induced psychotic disorder, and psychotic disorder due to general medical condition, with delusions and with hallucinations) (N=11); and unaffected (N=559).

cSignificant differences between individuals with diagnoses coded as present and absent are as follows for each domain (df=1 in all cases). Abstraction/flexibility: schizophrenia (χ2=109.73), schizoaffective-depressed (χ2=18.12), schizoaffective-bipolar (χ2=11.08), substance-related disorders (χ2=6.49), psychotic-"nonorganic" (χ2=5.57), psychotic-"organic" (χ2=4.14), unaffected (χ2=13.20). Attention: schizophrenia (χ2=88.55), schizoaffective-depressed (χ2=19.48), schizoaffective-bipolar (χ2=11.64), major depressive disorder (χ2=7.41), unaffected (χ2=10.95). Verbal memory: schizophrenia (χ2=134.99), schizoaffective-depressed (χ2=15.73), schizoaffective-bipolar (χ2=13.66), substance-related disorders (χ2=5.07), unaffected (χ2=5.94). Face memory: schizophrenia (χ2=35.48), major depressive disorder (χ2=4.27), bipolar disorders (χ2=4.11). Spatial memory: schizophrenia (χ2=75.91), schizoaffective-depressed (χ2=19.59), schizoaffective-bipolar (χ2=9.23), substance-related disorders (χ2=9.84), psychotic-"organic" (χ2=5.31), unaffected (χ2=5.05). Working memory: schizophrenia (χ2=55.00), schizoaffective-depressed (χ2=9.05), schizoaffective-bipolar (χ2=14.47), bipolar disorders (χ2=3.91), psychotic-"organic" (χ2=6.13), unaffected (χ2=13.84). Language: schizophrenia (χ2=43.33), schizoaffective-depressed (χ2=6.92), schizoaffective-bipolar (χ2=7.21), major depressive disorder (χ2=5.64), substance-related disorders (χ2=10.91), unaffected (χ2=7.87). Spatial processing: schizophrenia (χ2=57.51), schizoaffective-depressed (χ2=15.41), schizoaffective-bipolar (χ2=7.72), substance-related disorders (χ2=16.35), unaffected (χ2=11.49). Emotion processing: schizophrenia (χ2=122.93), schizoaffective-depressed (χ2=12.01), schizoaffective-bipolar (χ2=13.40), major depressive disorder (χ2=4.58), psychotic-"nonorganic" (χ2=5.28), unaffected (χ2=12.68).

dSignificant differences between individuals with diagnoses coded as present and absent are as follows for each domain (df=1 in all cases). Abstraction/flexibility: schizophrenia (χ2=36.20), schizoaffective-depressed (χ2=15.88), bipolar disorders (χ2=6.76), substance-related disorders (χ2=6.99), psychotic-"nonorganic" (χ2=4.60), unaffected (χ2=16.85). Attention: schizophrenia (χ2=49.92), schizoaffective-depressed (χ2=19.70), schizoaffective-bipolar (χ2=14.56), bipolar disorders (χ2=3.91), psychotic-"nonorganic" (χ2=4.14), psychotic-"organic" (χ2=5.06). Verbal memory: schizophrenia (χ2=65.14), schizoaffective-depressed (χ2=8.27), schizoaffective-bipolar (χ2=6.20), psychotic-"nonorganic" (χ2=4.91). Face memory: schizophrenia (χ2=18.43). Spatial memory: schizophrenia (χ2=30.29), schizoaffective-depressed (χ2=9.07), schizoaffective-bipolar (χ2=5.55), substance-related disorders (χ2=8.26), psychotic-"nonorganic" (χ2=6.45), unaffected (χ2=8.89). Working memory: schizophrenia (χ2=19.82), schizoaffective-depressed (χ2=19.98), schizoaffective-bipolar (χ2=6.20). Spatial processing: schizophrenia (χ2=49.04), schizoaffective-depressed (χ2=4.97), schizoaffective-bipolar (χ2=10.83), unaffected (χ2=16.03). Sensorimotor processing: schizophrenia (χ2=196.93), schizoaffective-depressed (χ2=35.22), schizoaffective-bipolar (χ2=24.99), major depressive disorder (χ2=13.76), substance-related disorders (χ2=8.07), psychotic-"nonorganic" (χ2=6.94), psychotic-"organic" (χ2=5.41), unaffected (χ2=38.00).

The schizophrenia and schizoaffective categories were associated with the slowest speeds, but although substance-related disorders were associated with lower accuracy, they were not as strongly associated with impaired speed.

These results underscore the relationship between schizophrenia diagnosis and neurocognitive performance, with the schizophrenia diagnosis more highly associated with reduced accuracy and speed than nonschizophrenia disorders. Relatives with schizoaffective and other disorders share many of these features, but not to the same extent. These results highlight the utility of considering multiple diagnoses per individual, rather than restricting analyses to mutually exclusive, primary diagnostic categories.

Medical History

Because PAARTNERS participants were not excluded for medical conditions that may affect neurocognition, we evaluated the relationship between neurocognitive performance and medical history. We introduced into our models three levels of medical conditions, based on relationship to neurocognition: medically healthy (N=745), minor medical condition (e.g., hypertension, minor head injury, diabetes; N=969), and major medical condition (e.g., meningitis, stroke, significant head injury, thyroid disease; N=62). The remaining individuals had an unknown medical history (missing or incomplete information, N=96) and were excluded from further medical history analyses. Generalized estimating equation analyses performed with each of the neurocognitive domain scores as the dependent variable revealed that medical history was a significant predictor of attention accuracy and the speed of verbal memory, spatial memory, spatial processing, and sensorimotor processing. However, in each case where diagnosis was a significant predictor, it remained so. Moreover, the amount of variance in neurocognitive measures that was attributable to diagnosis did not differ substantially after medical history was included in the model.

Discussion

To our knowledge, PAARTNERS has recruited and assessed the largest group of African American families of probands with schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder to date. Heritability estimates for all neurocognitive abilities were significant, with the highest for the accuracy of spatial processing, verbal memory, abstraction/flexibility, face memory, and emotion processing and for the speed of attention (Figure 1). These results are consistent with prior work robustly implicating deficits in verbal memory, abstraction/flexibility, and attention as candidate endophenotypes of schizophrenia (2). In contrast, spatial processing and emotion processing have not been widely regarded as candidate endophenotypes of schizophrenia. Nonetheless, both domains show high heritability and differentiate individuals with schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder and their family members from community subjects. These findings justify further consideration of these abilities as candidate endophenotypes (2).

Two multisite studies have supported the heritability of particular measures of the University of Pennsylvania computerized neurocognitive performance battery in families of schizophrenia probands that were predominantly European American (Consortium on the Genetics of Schizophrenia; 38, 39) or wholly European American (multigenerational investigation; 3). The results of the current investigation support the hypothesis that neurocognitive abilities, especially those implicated as candidate endophenotypes in schizophrenia, are heritable in African American families as well.

Consistent with the endophenotype criterion that the characteristic cosegregate with illness in families (4, 5), a diagnosis of schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder had a profound impact on neurocognitive performance, other psychotic and bipolar disorders had a more modest effect, while family members with nonpsychotic disorders only occasionally differed from unaffected family members in neurocognitive performance accuracy. Differences between schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorder were also observed (Figure 3). Schizophrenia was associated with more impaired neurocognitive accuracy and speed than schizoaffective disorder. Presently, there is no consensus on the genetic relationship between schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorder, but the literature suggests potential genetic overlap (16). Future analyses of PAARTNERS data will seek to examine differences among family members with these disorders to gauge genetic implications.

Individuals with schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder and their relatives with bipolar disorder had similar results for accuracy of working memory and spatial processing and for performance speed across neurocognitive domains (Figure 2), but they differed substantially on accuracy for several other domains, such as verbal and face memory. Evidence for similar neurocognitive functioning in some domains is consistent with the suggestion that schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorder share some endophenotypic markers with bipolar disorder (14) and that they have shared genetic susceptibility (16). This result should be interpreted cautiously because the bipolar group was small. Moreover, all relatives were genetically related to probands with schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder, and a lifetime history of bipolar disorder in one of these family members could be etiologically or phenomenologically different from bipolar disorder in other familial contexts.

Although significantly heritable, accuracy of face memory did not differentiate relatives from community subjects, a finding inconsistent with our prior work with predominantly European American groups (3, 40, 41). Surprisingly, the small number of relatives with bipolar disorder were significantly more accurate on face memory than community comparison subjects. A recent study using the same face processing measures supports an "other-race effect" in face processing among European Americans and African Americans (42), but this would not seem to account for the better performance in relatives, especially because all participants in the current study were African American. Moreover, the schizophrenia patients exhibited the expected impairments. Future efforts will be aimed at examining face memory performance across relatives from racially diverse families to better understand this effect.

Relatives of patients with schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder, regardless of axis I psychopathology, were less accurate than community comparison subjects in most neurocognitive domains, and they were slower in spatial and emotion processing and sensorimotor speed. Notably, this was true even for psychiatrically healthy relatives. In addition, analyses of group differences that included only unaffected relatives age 30 or older (119 young relatives dropped) and analyses including only unaffected relatives age 25 or older (83 young relatives dropped) did not yield substantive changes in results (data not shown), indicating that the observed impairment in relatives is not attributable to a preponderance of young relatives in premorbid or prodromal stages of schizophrenia. Moreover, other, nonpsychiatric medical conditions did not interact with psychiatric status to predict performance. These results suggest that neurocognitive performance in schizophrenia families is attributable to an individual's status as a biological relative, and thus to presumed genetic liability for schizophrenia, rather than to personal psychopathology, including substance-related disorders or other medical conditions. These results, together with the evidence for heritability and cosegregation of neurocognitive abilities with the schizophrenia diagnosis, extend support for neurocognitive abilities as candidate endophenotypes of schizophrenia in African American families.

Presented in part at the 11th International Congress on Schizophrenia Research, Colorado Springs, March 28 to April 2, 2007, and at the 63rd Annual Meeting of the Society of Biological Psychiatry, Washington, D.C., May 1–3, 2008. Received Sept. 11, 2008; revision received Sept. 21, 2009; accepted Nov. 3, 2009. From the Department of Psychiatry, University of Pennsylvania; the Department of Epidemiology, the Department of Psychiatry, and the Department of Human Genetics, University of Pittsburgh; the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, University of California at Davis, Sacramento; the Department of Epidemiology and International Health and the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Neurobiology, University of Alabama at Birmingham; the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Duke University Medical Center, and the Department of Research, Durham VA Medical Center, Durham, N.C.; the Department of Psychiatry and Human Behavior, University of Mississippi Medical Center, Jackson; the Department of Preventive Medicine, Institute for Global Health, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, Tenn.; the Department of Psychiatry, Morehouse School of Medicine, Atlanta; the Department of Psychiatry, University of Tennessee College of Medicine, Memphis; the Department of Neurology, University of Virginia, Charlottesville; the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Medical University of South Carolina, Charleston; and the Philadelphia VA Medical Center. Address correspondence and reprint requests to Dr. Calkins, Schizophrenia Research Center and Brain Behavior Laboratory, Neuropsychiatry Section, Department of Psychiatry, University of Pennsylvania School of Medicine, 9 Maloney, 3600 Spruce St., Philadelphia, PA 19104; (e-mail).

Dr. Ruben Gur receives grant support through collaborations of the University of Pennsylvania with AstraZeneca, Pfizer, and Merck and may receive royalties from future commercial use of the Penn Computerized Neurocognitive Battery. Dr. Ragland may receive royalties from future commercial use of the Penn Computerized Neurocognitive Battery. Ms. Richard may receive royalties from future commercial use of Penn Computerized Neurocognitive Battery. Dr. Nimgaonkar received a research grant from Lundbeck on an unrelated topic when this article was in preparation. Dr. Raquel Gur receives grant support through collaborations of the University of Pennsylvania with AstraZeneca and Pfizer and may receive royalties from future commercial use of the Penn Computerized Neurocognitive Battery. The remaining authors report no financial relationships with commercial interests.

Supported by NIMH RO1 grants MH-66006, MH-66278, MH-066049, MH-66181, MH-66121, MH-066005, MH-66050, MH-66263, and MH-66004 and NIMH grant K08-MH-79364.

Acknowledgments

The following research faculty and staff contributed at the eight sites: University of Alabama (central administrative site)—Roberta May; Charlie Swanson, Jr., M.D.; Laura Montgomery-Barefield, M.D.; Tolulope Aduroja, M.D.; Ryan Coleman; Rakesha Garner; Lee Prichett, R.N.; Thomas Kelley, R.N.; and Marguerite Ryan Dickson, M.S.; Duke—Joseph P. McEvoy, M.D.; and Linda Blalock, R.N.; University of Mississippi—Karen Richardson, M.S.; Morehouse School of Medicine—Deirdre Evans-Cosby, M.D.; George W. Woods, M.D.; Kendaly Meadows, R.N.; Sandra Cummings M.S.W.; Cara Stephens L.C.S.W.; and Kent Baker; Medical University of South Carolina—Shirley Hendrix, Cynthia Gilliard, Wanda Smalls-Smith, and Steven McLeod-Bryant; University of Pennsylvania—Felipe Da Silva; Alexandra Duncan-Ramos, M.S.; Jarrod Gutman; CarlaAnn Henry; Paul Hughett, Ph.D.; Farzin Irani, Ph.D.; Jennifer Jackson Greene, M.S.; Stephen J. Kanes, M.D., Ph.D.; Christian Kohler, M.D.; David Rice; Devon Seward; Steven Siegel, M.D., Ph.D.; Bruce Turetsky, M.D.; and Robert Witalec; University of Pittsburgh—Joel Wood, B.S.; Mary Miller, L.P.N.; and Frank Fleischer, M.B.A.; University of Tennessee—Kristin Beizai, M.D.; Marie Tobin, M.D.; Alyssa English, M.D.; Richard Sanders, B.S.; Shelia A. Dempsey, A.D.N.; Martha Velez, C.P.S.; Marianne Smith, B.S., M.A.; Martha Garriott, M.S., N.C.C.; Nancy Fowler; Derrick W. Allen, M.S.S.W.; Phyllis Meyer, B.S., P.A.; and Lynn Heustess, B.S.

The authors thank the study participants and research faculty and staff for their time and effort, and they thank the Western Mental Health Center and Dr. Thomas Hobbs for their support and contribution to recruitment in Ensley and Birmingham, Ala.

References

1 Aleman A , Hijman R , de Haan EHF , Kahn RS : Memory impairment in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Am J Psychiatry 1999; 156:1358–1366 AbstractGoogle Scholar

2 Gur RE , Calkins ME , Gur RC , Horan WP , Nuechterlein KH , Seidman LJ , Stone WS : The Consortium on the Genetics of Schizophrenia: neurocognitive endophenotypes. Schizophr Bull 2007; 33:49–68 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

3 Gur RE , Nimgaonkar VL , Almasy L , Calkins ME , Ragland JD , Pogue-Geile MF , Kanes S , Blangero J , Gur RC : Neurocognitive endophenotypes in a multiplex multigenerational family study of schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 2007; 164:813–819 LinkGoogle Scholar

4 Gottesman II , Gould TD : The endophenotype concept in psychiatry: etymology and strategic intentions. Am J Psychiatry 2003; 160:636–645 LinkGoogle Scholar

5 Iacono WG : Identifying genetic risk for psychopathology, in The Science of Clinical Psychology: Accomplishments and Future Directions. Edited by Routh DKDeRubeis RJ. Washington, DC, American Psychological Association, 1998, pp 3–22 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

6 Aliyu MH , Calkins ME , Swanson JCL , Lyons PD , Savage RM , May R , Wiener H , Devlin B , Nimgaonkar VL , Ragland JD , Gur RE , Gur RC , Bradford LD , Edwards N , Kwentus J , McEvoy JP , Santos AB , McCleod-Bryant S , Tennison C , Go RCP : Project Among African-Americans to Explore Risks for Schizophrenia (PAARTNERS): recruitment and assessment methods. Schizophr Res 2006; 87:32–44 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

7 Glahn DC , Almasy L , Blangero J , Burk GM , Estrada J , Peralta JM , Meyenberg N , Castro MP , Barrett J , Nicolini H , Raventós H , Escamilla MA : Adjudicating neurocognitive endophenotypes for schizophrenia. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 2007; 144B:242–249 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

8 Devlin B , Daniels M , Roeder K : The heritability of IQ. Nature 1997; 388:468–471 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

9 Plomin R : Genetics, genes, genomics and g. Mol Psychiatry 2003; 8:1–5 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

10 Rowe DC , Cleveland HH : Academic achievement in blacks and whites: are the developmental processes similar? Intelligence 1996; 23:205–228 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

11 Snitz BE , Macdonald AW , Carter CS : Cognitive deficits in unaffected first-degree relatives of schizophrenia patients: a meta-analytic review of putative endophenotypes. Schizophr Bull 2006; 32:179–194 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

12 Heinrichs RW , Zakzanis KK : Neurocognitive deficits in schizophrenia: a quantitative review of the evidence. Neuropsychology 1998; 12:426–445 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

13 Hoff AL , Svetina C , Maurizio AM , Crow TJ , Spokes K , DeLisi LE : Familial cognitive deficits in schizophrenia. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 2005; 133B:43–49 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

14 Glahn DC , Bearden CE , Niendam TA , Escamilla MA : The feasibility of neuropsychological endophenotypes in the search for genes associated with bipolar affective disorder. Bipolar Disord 2004; 6:171–182 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

15 Weiser M , Reichenberg A , Rabinowitz J , Knobler HY , Lubin G , Yazvitzky R , Nahon D , Gur RC , Davidson M : Cognitive performance of male adolescents is lower than controls across psychiatric disorders: a population-based study. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2004; 110:471–475 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

16 Craddock N , O'Donovan MC , Owen MJ : Genes for schizophrenia and bipolar disorder? implications for psychiatric nosology. Schizophr Bull 2006; 32:9–16 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

17 Daban C , Martinez-Aran A , Torrent C , Tabares-Seisdedos R , Balanza-Martinez V , Salazar-Fraile J , Selva-Vera G , Vieta E : Specificity of cognitive deficits in bipolar disorder versus schizophrenia: a systematic review. Psychother Psychosom 2006; 75:72–84 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

18 Nurnberger JI , Blehar MC , Kaufmann CA , York-Cooler C , Simpson SG , Harkavy-Friedman J , Severe JB , Malaspina D , Reich T : Diagnostic Interview for Genetic Studies: rationale, unique features, and training: NIMH Genetics Initiative. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1994; 51:849–859 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

19 NIMH Genetics Initiative: Family Interview for Genetic Studies (FIGS). Rockville, Md, National Institute of Mental Health, 1992 Google Scholar

20 Gur RC , Ragland JD , Moberg PJ , Turner TH , Bilker WB , Kohler C , Siegel SJ , Gur RE : Computerized neurocognitive scanning, I: methodology and validation in healthy people. Neuropsychopharmacology 2001; 25:766–788 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

21 Gur RC , Ragland JD , Moberg PJ , Bilker WB , Kohler C , Siegel SJ , Gur RE : Computerized neurocognitive scanning, II: the profile of schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology 2001; 25:777–788 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

22 Glahn DC , Cannon TD , Gur RE , Ragland JD , Gur RC : Working memory constrains abstraction in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 2000; 47:34–42 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

23 Kurtz MM , Ragland JD , Moberg PJ , Gur RC : The Penn Conditional Exclusion Test: a new measure of executive-function with alternate forms for repeat administration. Arch Clin Neuropsychology 2004; 19:191–201 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

24 Kurtz MM , Ragland JD , Bilker W , Gur RC , Gur RE : Comparison of the continuous performance test with and without working memory demands in healthy controls and patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 2001; 48:307–316 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

25 Ragland JD , Turetsky BI , Gur RC , Gunning-Dixon F , Turner TH , Schroeder L , Chan R , Gur RE : Working memory for complex figures: an fMRI comparison of letter and fractal N-back tasks. Neuropsychology 2002; 16:370–379 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

26 Gur RC , Jaggi JL , Ragland JD , Resnick SM , Shtasel D , Muenz L , Gur RE : Effects of memory processing on regional brain activation: cerebral blood flow in normal subjects. Int J Neurosci 1993; 72:31–44 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

27 Glahn DC , Gur RC , Ragland JD , Gur RE : Reliability, performance characteristics, and construct validity and initial application of the Visual Object Learning Test (VOLT). Neuropsychology 1997; 11:602–612 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

28 Gur RC , Gur RE , Obrist WD , Skolnick BE , Reivich M : Age and regional cerebral blood flow at rest and during cognitive activity. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1987; 44:617–621 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

29 Gur RC , Richard J , Hughett P , Calkins ME , Macy L , Bilker WB , Brensinger C , Gur RE : A cognitive neuroscience based computerized battery for efficient measurement of individual differences: standardization and initial construct validation. J Neurosci Methods (in press) Google Scholar

30 Kohler CG , Turner TH , Bilker WB , Brensinger CM , Siegel SJ , Kanes SJ , Gur RE , Gur RC : Facial emotion recognition in schizophrenia: intensity effects and error pattern. Am J Psychiatry 2003; 160:1768–1774 LinkGoogle Scholar

31 Erwin RJ , Gur RC , Gur RE , Smailis J , Skolnick BE : Facial emotion discrimination, 1: task construction and behavioral findings in normal subjects. Psychiatry Res 1992; 42:231–240 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

32 Patterson HD , Thompson JR : Recovery of inter-block information when block sizes are unequal. Biometrika 1971; 58:545–554 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

33 Misztal I , Tsuruta S , Strabel T , Auvray B , Druet T , Lee D : BLUPF90 and related programs (BGF90), in Proceedings of the 7th World Congress on Genetics Applied to Livestock Production (CD-ROM communication 01-11). Montpellier, France, 2002, p 7 Google Scholar

34 Dempster AP , Laird NM , Rubin DB : Maximum likelihood from incomplete data via the EM algorithm. J R Stat Soc Series B Stat Methodol 1977; 39:1–38 Google Scholar

35 Gilmour AR , Cullis BR , Welham SJ , Thompson R : ASREML Reference Manual. Orange, New South Wales, Australia, New South Wales Agriculture, 2002 Google Scholar

36 Fisher R : Statistical methods for research workers. Edinburgh, Oliver and Boyd, 1925 Google Scholar

37 Donner A , Wells G : A comparison of confidence interval methods for the intraclass correlation coefficient. Biometrics 1986; 42:401–412 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

38 Calkins ME , Dobie DJ , Cadenhead KS , Olincy A , Freedman R , Green MF , Greenwood TA , Gur RE , Gur RC , Light GA , Mintz J , Nuechterlein KH , Radant AD , Schork NJ , Seidman LJ , Siever LJ , Silverman JM , Stone WS , Swerdlow NR , Tsuang DW , Tsuang MT , Turetsky BI , Braff DL : The Consortium on the Genetics of Endophenotypes in Schizophrenia: "model" recruitment, assessment and endophenotyping methods for a multi-site collaboration. Schizophr Bull 2007; 33:33–48 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

39 Greenwood TA , Braff DL , Light GA , Cadenhead KS , Calkins ME , Dobie DJ , Freedman R , Green MF , Gur RE , Gur RC , Mintz J , Nuechterlein KH , Olincy A , Radant AD , Seidman LJ , Siever LJ , Silverman JM , Stone WS , Swerdlow NR , Tsuang DW , Tsuang MT , Turetsky BI , Schork NJ (The Consortium on the Genetics of Schizophrenia): Initial heritability analyses of endophenotypic measures for schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2007; 64:1242–1250 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

40 Calkins ME , Gur RC , Ragland JD , Gur RE : Face recognition memory deficits and visual object memory performance in patients with schizophrenia and their relatives. Am J Psychiatry 2005; 162:1963–1966 LinkGoogle Scholar

41 Conklin HM , Calkins ME , Anderson CW , Dinzeo TJ , Iacono WG : Recognition memory for faces in schizophrenia patients and their first-degree relatives. Neuropsychologia 2002; 40:2314–2324 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

42 Pinkham AE , Sasson NJ , Calkins ME , Richard J , Hughett P , Gur RE , Gur RC : The other-race effect in face processing among African American and Caucasian individuals with schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 2008; 165:639–645 LinkGoogle Scholar