The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×
Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1176/ajp.155.7.969

Abstract

OBJECTIVE: The authors tested the hypotheses that medical illness burden is independently associated with depression and that this association is moderated by neuroticism. METHOD: Multiple regression techniques were used to determine the independent associations of medical burden and neuroticism with depression in a group of 196 subjects, 60 years of age and older, recruited from primary care settings. RESULTS: Medical burden and neuroticism were independently associated with major depression, depressive symptoms, and psychiatric dysfunction. CONCLUSIONS: These findings support models in which medical disorders may contribute directly to depression. At the same time, the role of neuroticism in later-life depression warrants further study. (Am J Psychiatry 1998; 155:969–971)

Depression in later life is a major public health problem, often associated with prominent medical comorbidity (1). Medical illnesses may contribute to depressive pathogenesis through direct effects on brain function or through psychological or psychosocial mechanisms. As an example of the latter, personality trait neuroticism by definition implies emotional vulnerability to stress; persons high in neuroticism might experience greater depressive symptoms in the face of increased medical burden (2). While recent studies have examined the role of categorically defined personality disorders in later-life depression (3, 4), only one examined dimensional personality traits (5). No study has examined whether neuroticism moderates the association between medical illness and later-life depression.

We tested the hypotheses that 1) medical illness burden is associated with depression independent of neu~roticism and 2) there is an interaction such that subjects with higher neuroticism have a greater association of medical burden and depression. Our subjects were drawn from primary care sites because of the public health importance of understanding psychopathology and medical comorbidity in these settings.

METHOD

Subjects were recruited from private internal medicine offices or a family medicine clinic (described previously in reference 6 and also by Lyness et al. in an unpublished manuscript). All patients ages 60 years and over who gave formal verbal informed consent (procedures approved by the University of Rochester's Research Subjects Review Board) were eligible to participate. Stratified sampling on a self-report depression inventory was used to oversample patients with significant depressive symptoms, but patients across the full range of screening scores were included. In-depth assessments were conducted by trained master's level raters using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R (7) and the 24-item Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (8). Neuroticism was assessed by the NEO-Five Factor Inventory (9), a 60-item self-report questionnaire with demonstrated reliability and long-term stability. Its neuroticism factor, similar to other scales used in published depression studies, includes items assessing proneness to affects and ideation found in depression. Medical illness severity was assessed by the Cumulative Illness Rating Scale (10), on the basis of physician-investigator (J.M.L.) review of each patient's primary care chart and all other available records. Other measures included the self-reported Geriatric Depression Scale (11) and the Global Assessment of Functioning Scale from DSM-III-R.

We used multiple logistic and linear regression techniques to examine the independent associations of medical illness burden and neuroticism with the dependent measures of depressive diagnosis (current or any history of major depression), depressive symptoms (Hamilton depression scale and Geriatric Depression Scale), and psychiatric function (Global Assessment of Functioning Scale), while controlling for age, gender, and education. To determine whether any association of medical illness with the Hamilton depression scale was due merely to physical symptoms, the Hamilton depression scale also was divided into two 12-item subscales assessing affective/psychological and somatic/neurovegetative symptoms, respectively. To test for interactions of the Cumulative Illness Rating Scale and neuroticism, polynomial interaction terms were created by computing powers and cross-products. Both second- (e.g., x2, xy, y2) and third- (e.g., x3, x2y, xy2, y3) order terms were included (second alone and then both combined) as independent variables in separate regression analyses; only the second-order cross-product interaction term is reported because the third-order terms did not yield additional information. Logarithmic or squared transformations of data were used when necessary to stabilize the variances. One-tailed p values were used because of the a priori directional hypotheses.

RESULTS

A total of 196 subjects completed all study measures. Their mean age was 70.4 years (SD=7.1, range=60–89); 119 (61%) were women. The mean Hamilton depression scale score was 8.1 (SD=6.4, range=0–32), and the Cumulative Illness Rating Scale score was 6.0 (SD=2.9, range=0–16). Twenty-eight subjects did not complete the NEO-Five Factor Inventory or other study measures. They did not differ statistically from the study subjects in age, gender distribution, Hamilton depression scale score, or Cumulative Illness Rating Scale score, although they had less education (mean=11.6 years, SD=3.1, versus mean=13.3, SD=2.8) (t=2.63, df=30.7, p=0.01).

Table 1 shows the results from the first set of regression analyses. Both the Cumulative Illness Rating Scale and neuroticism were significantly and independently associated with all outcome variables, except the Cumulative Illness Rating Scale was not associated with history of major depression.

Turning to the regressions that examined interactions, the Cumulative Illness Rating Scale-neuroticism cross-product interaction term was significantly independently associated with current major depression (χ2=3.75, df=1, p=0.03) and with history of major depression (χ2=5.67, df=1, p=0.008), such that higher Cumulative Illness Rating Scale score and greater neuroticism were associated with a greater likelihood of depressive disorder. (The Hosmer-Lemeshow goodness-of-fit chi-square values for these two regressions were as follows: χ2=3.39, df=3, p=0.34, and χ2=8.15, df=8, p=0.42, respectively, indicating a good fit.) However, the interaction term was not significantly associated with the other outcome variables.

DISCUSSION

Our first hypothesis was confirmed: medical illness burden was independently associated with current major depression, depressive symptoms, and psychiatric function. Our hypothesis that neuroticism moderated the association of physical illness with depression received partial support, because the Cumulative Illness Rating Scale-neuroticism interaction term was independently associated with major depression, but not with the Hamilton depression scale or the Geriatric Depression Scale.

Neuroticism's independent association with the outcome variables supports the need to further examine personality traits in the pathogenesis of depressive disorders in later life (2). However, the measurement of neuroticism is potentially confounded by depression itself (12). The state-trait aspects of this confound may be addressed to some extent by longitudinal studies or informant reports, but the conceptual overlap between trait neuroticism and depressive symptoms is more vexing. Specific avenues that may prove fruitful include examination of whether neuroticism's role as a moderator between medical illness and depression is influenced by factors such as medical disability, pain, or self-health perception and examination of neuroticism's role as a moderator in specific medically or psychiatrically defined patient subgroups.

TABLE 1

Received Sept. 3, 1997; revision received Dec. 17, 1997; accepted Feb. 9, 1998. From the Departments of Psychiatry and Biostatistics, University of Rochester School of Medicine and Dentistry. Address reprint requests to Dr. Lyness, Department of Psychiatry, University of Rochester Medical Center, 300 Crittenden Blvd., Rochester, NY 14642; (e-mail). Supported by NIMH grants MH-01113 (Dr. Lyness) and MH-01135 (Dr. Duberstein). The authors thank Tami Kelly Noel, M.S., Cynthia Doane, M.S.P.H., Aaron Gleason, Gerard Kiernan, Holly Stiner, and Ziggy Yoediono for technical assistance; the patients and staff of the offices of Drs. Judith Allen, Russell Maggio, and Bruce Peyser and the Family Medicine Center at Highland Hospital for their help with this project; and the support staff of the Program in Geriatrics and Neuropsychiatry.

References

1 NIH Consensus Development Panel on Depression in Late Life: Diagnosis and treatment of depression in late life. JAMA 1992; 268:1018–1024Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

2 Duberstein PR, Seidlitz L, Lyness JM, Conwell Y: Dimensional measures and the five factor model: clinical implications and research directions, in Personality Disorders in Older Adults: Emerging Issues in Diagnosis and Treatment. Edited by Rosowsky E, Abrams RC, Zweig RA. Hillsdale, NJ, Lawrence Erl~baum Associates (in press)Google Scholar

3 Kunik ME, Mulsant BH, Rifai AH, Sweet RA, Pasternak R, Zu~benko GS: Diagnostic rate of comorbid personality disorder in elderly psychiatric inpatients. Am J Psychiatry 1994; 151:603–605LinkGoogle Scholar

4 Abrams RC, Rosendahl E, Card C, Alexopoulos GS: Personality disorder correlates of late and early onset depression. J Am Geriatr Soc 1994; 42:727–731Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

5 Burvill PW, Hall WD, Stampfer HG, Emmerson P: The prognosis of depression in old age. Br J Psychiatry 1991; 158:64–71Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

6 Lyness JM, Noel TK, Cox C, King DA, Conwell Y, Caine ED: Screening for depression in primary care elderly: a comparison of the CES-D and the GDS. Arch Intern Med 1997; 157:449–454Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

7 Spitzer RL, Williams JBW, Gibbon M: Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R (SCID). New York, New York State Psychiatric Institute, Biometrics Research, 1986Google Scholar

8 Williams JB: A structured interview guide for the Hamilton Depression Rating Scale. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1988; 45:742–747Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

9 Costa PT, McCrae RR: The NEO Personality Inventory: Revised Professional Manual. Odessa, Fla, Psychological Assessment Resources, 1992Google Scholar

10 Linn BS, Linn MW, Gurel L: Cumulative Illness Rating Scale. J Am Geriatr Soc 1968; 16:622–626Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

11 Yesavage JA, Brink TL, Rose TL, Lum O, Huang V, Adey M, Leirer VO: Development and validation of a geriatric depression screening scale: a preliminary report. J Psychiatr Res 1982–1983; 17:37–49Google Scholar

12 Boyce P, Hadzi-Pavlovic D, Parker G, Brodaty H, Hickie I, Mitchell P, Wilhelm K: Depressive type and state effects on personality measures. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1990; 81:197–200Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar