The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×

Abstract

Objective:

An increased understanding of repetitive dysfunctional patterns and their relationship to an individual’s life history is regarded as a key mechanism of change in insight-oriented therapies. At the same time, empirical research on the insight-outcome relationship is rare, and its generalizability is restricted by the use of a wide range of definitions and methods among studies. The authors conducted a meta-analysis to systematically examine the association between patient insight and psychotherapy outcome across a range of treatment modalities.

Method:

Insight was defined as patients’ understanding of associations between past and present experiences, typical relationship patterns, and the relation between interpersonal challenges, emotional experience, and psychological symptoms. From 13,849 initially identified abstracts, the authors extracted 23 independent effect sizes. A random-effects meta-analysis was performed to assess the magnitude of the insight-outcome relationship. Risk of publication bias was assessed with funnel plot inspections, Egger’s regression test, and Duval and Tweedie’s trim-and-fill procedure as sensitivity analyses.

Results:

A significant, moderate correlation (r=0.31) was observed between insight and treatment outcome. Sensitivity analyses demonstrated the robustness of the results.

Conclusions:

The findings support the importance of insight for psychotherapy outcome. Insight may be a relevant mechanism of change across different treatment modalities.

Despite compelling evidence for the efficacy of psychotherapy for mental disorders, there is an ongoing debate about underlying mechanisms of change (1, 2). Hence, process-outcome research is a key area of contemporary psychotherapy research (36), and understanding the mechanisms that underlie psychotherapy effects is vital to increase the effectiveness of psychosocial treatments (3, 7).

One of the oldest proposed curative mechanisms of psychotherapy is insight. Freud and Breuer (8) assumed that neurotic symptoms would disappear if patients became aware of split-off and repressed memories. Later developments in ego psychology stressed that insight must be understood as a process that involves both cognitive and affective components (9). Contemporary psychodynamic theorists point toward the importance of relational aspects of seeking, constructing, and conversing about insight (1014). Insight is closely related to psychodynamic interventions, such as clarification, confrontation, and interpretation (15). According to psychodynamic theory, higher levels of self-understanding result in fewer negative automatic reactions to stress and challenges, more positive emotional experiences, and greater freedom to choose adaptive interpersonal and health-related behaviors (1620).

A fundamental challenge to the study of insight is that the term has no clear definition (21). In psychiatry, it often denotes insight into illness as a prerequisite for treatment motivation (22). Experiential models emphasize attaining a new perspective through experiencing (23). In cognitive-behavioral therapy (CBT), insight relates to becoming aware of automatic negative thoughts (24). Within these definitions, insight refers to a process, a state, or both. As a trait characteristic, it is often referred to as insightfulness (25). Our definition of insight is based on the psychodynamic conceptualizations outlined above. Throughout this article, the terms insight and self-understanding are used interchangeably to refer to patients’ understanding of associations between past and present experiences, typical relationship patterns, and the relation between interpersonal challenges, emotional experience, and psychological symptoms (2, 26). Although a focus on insight as a curative factor is most prevalent within psychodynamic treatments, other types of psychotherapy, such as CBT, enhance patients’ self-understanding, for example, by targeting automatic thought processes. To gain a comprehensive image of the association between insight and outcome, we use the term psychotherapy to refer to those primarily verbal psychosocial treatments in which self-understanding is encouraged.

An important, although nonsystematic, first overview of the literature by Crits-Christoph et al. (2) suggests that self-understanding is related to psychotherapy outcome and that this relationship may be specific to psychodynamic treatments. A systematic review of studies on the insight-outcome association would allow a comprehensive evaluation of the current evidence base, an estimation of the population effect size, and identification of topics for future research. We therefore conducted the present systematic review with two overall aims. First, we comprehensively reviewed the literature to collect empirical evidence on the insight-outcome association and assess the validity of the findings. Second, using meta-analysis, we quantitatively combined the results of previous studies to estimate the mean population effect size for the insight-outcome association.

Method

This meta-analysis was conducted using the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) guidelines. The review protocol was registered in advance at the international prospective register of systematic reviews (PROSPERO), registration number CRD42016043104.

Eligibility Criteria

Population.

Studies had to be based on adult patients (age 18 or older) who were seeking treatment for a psychological condition. One of the studies otherwise identified as eligible employed a subsample (15%) of adolescents (27). Since the proportion of adolescents was small, we deemed the threats to validity negligible and included the study. Excluding this study from the analyses did not change the pattern of results. We had no requirements regarding diagnoses, but we excluded studies that were based on volunteer samples without any psychological burden.

Interventions.

Participants had to receive psychological treatments such as psychotherapy or counseling. Treatments had to be delivered by trained therapists, such as psychiatrists, clinical psychologists, or counselors. We included all bona fide psychotherapies (28) that were conducted in a dyadic or group therapy setting. We did not include interventions such as dream interpretation, hypnosis, bibliotherapy, and music or art therapy unless they were integrated into a broader psychological treatment.

Predictor and outcome variables.

As a predictor variable, insight had to be measured by a quantitative instrument that fit with our definition. Assessment had to take place during treatment or at termination. We did not include studies employing only a pretreatment measure of trait insightfulness. As outcome variables, we included any quantitative outcome measure such as symptom improvement, quality of life, or psychological functioning. We accepted session and overall treatment outcomes but required that outcome be measured simultaneously with or, preferably, subsequent to the assessment of insight.

Study designs and publications.

We included empirical studies that reported quantitative group statistics. Studies could be published as journal articles, books, or dissertations.

Search Strategy

Publications were identified by a search of PubMed, PsycINFO, PsycARTICLES, and PSYNDEX using the search terms (insight OR insightfulness OR self-understanding) AND (psychotherapy OR counseling). Results were restricted to publications in English or German. The last search was conducted on April 6, 2018. In addition, reference lists of relevant publications were examined by hand search, and experts were consulted to identify missing studies.

Study Selection

A flow diagram of the study selection process is presented in Figure 1. Electronic database and hand search yielded a total of 13,849 records. After duplicate removal, the titles and abstracts of 12,780 publications were screened. Next, full texts were inspected. Unpublished dissertations were requested from authors. A total of 261 full-text publications were assessed for eligibility. Publications with unclear inclusion status were discussed between two authors (S.J., U.D.), and ambiguity was resolved by consensus. If two publications were based on the same data, we chose the study with the insight measure that fit our definition of insight best. When insight measures were identical, we selected the study that reported the most comprehensive statistics. We selected Andreoli et al. (29) over Andreoli et al. (30) because the latter only reported associations between insight and outcome for subsamples. Hoffart et al. (31) was chosen over Hoffart and Sexton (32) because only the former reported a zero-order correlation between insight and outcome. We selected Kolden (33) over Kolden (34) because the former reported the purer association between insight and outcome. We chose Luborsky et al. (35) over Grenyer and Luborsky (36) because the insight operationalization in the former fit our definition more closely. In cases of ambiguity about the primary data source, authors were contacted for clarification. This procedure resulted in 22 unique studies (21 peer-reviewed journal articles and one dissertation) representing a k of 23 effect sizes from independent samples that were included in the meta-analysis.

FIGURE 1.

FIGURE 1. Flow Diagram of the Study Selection Process in a Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis of Insight and Outcome of Psychotherapy

Data Extraction Procedure

We developed a data extraction template according to the preregistered study protocol, pretested it on five studies, and subsequently refined it. One author (S.J.) extracted the following data: study characteristics, sample characteristics, information on predictor and outcome measures, and effect sizes for the association between insight and outcome. If zero-order correlations could not be extracted or computed from the reported data, authors were contacted for additional information. One randomized controlled trial reported data for two independent treatment subgroups (dynamic therapy and CBT) (37). In this case, each subgroup was treated as a separate study (38). When correlations of several insight scores with outcome (39, 40), of insight with several outcomes (41), or all subscales of an outcome measure (42) were reported, we computed the mean correlation coefficient for that study. For studies reporting only partial information on the insight-outcome association (e.g., only for significant subscales), we imputed missing nonsignificant correlations with a correlation coefficient of zero before calculating a mean score (42, 43).

Risk of Bias Assessment

Two authors (S.J., J.H.) independently assessed the risk of bias in each primary study. For this assessment, we adapted the Systematic Assessment of Quality in Observational Research Scale (44) and included additional items to assess common artifacts in meta-analyses of correlations (45). The modified 16-item risk of bias scale covers four categories: sample, quality of measurements, quality of statistical analyses, and reporting of data (see the online supplement). Each category includes three to five items, which are evaluated with ratings of 0 (no risk of bias), 1 (definite or unclear risk of bias), or not applicable. A mean score of applicable items yields a ratio between 0 and 1. Higher scores represent more risk of bias. Interrater reliability for the risk of bias assessment was good (intraclass correlation coefficient [ICC] of 0.80 using the formula ICC[3,1]; see reference 46), and scores were averaged across raters.

Data Analysis

Effect sizes were measured as correlation coefficients. All effects were coded such that positive correlations indicate more insight to be associated with better outcome. All Pearson correlations were transformed with Fisher’s r-to-z transformation prior to any computations and back-transformed for interpretation purposes.

Analyses were performed using the R package metafor, version 1.9-9 (47). Alpha was set at 0.05 for all analyses. First, we estimated the average true population effect size of the insight-outcome association in a random-effects model. This model was chosen a priori to allow for unconditional inferences about the effect size distribution in the population (48). Residual heterogeneity was estimated with the restricted maximum likelihood estimator (49). Study weight was calculated as the inverse of the sum of the study’s standard deviation and the estimated heterogeneity. The amount of heterogeneity was assessed using the Q-test for heterogeneity, the estimated amount of variability in the true effect sizes (tau), and the proportion of observed variability that can be attributed to true heterogeneity (I2) (38). Values for I2 of 25%, 50%, and 75% indicate a low, moderate, and high degree of heterogeneity, respectively (50). We performed prospective power analyses to detect different population effect sizes for conditions of low, moderate, and high heterogeneity in SAS Studio, version 3.6 (51), according to the method described by Hedges and Pigott (52). The analyses indicated adequate power to detect small (power=0.70–0.84), medium (power≈1) and large (power≈1) effects for all heterogeneity conditions.

Finally, we assessed the risk of publication bias by visually examining the funnel plot and conducting Egger’s regression test for funnel plot asymmetry (38). We applied Duval and Tweedie’s trim-and-fill procedure (53) as a sensitivity analysis to estimate the unbiased average population effect.

Results

Characteristics of Included Studies

The 22 studies (with a k of 23 independent effect sizes) subsumed a total of 1,112 individuals. The mean age of participants was 35.27 years (SD=6.01, range=23.70–46.40), and 63.84% of the total sample was female. Participants attended a median of 20 sessions (mean=73.27, SD=239.63, range=1–1118). There was considerable variety in psychopathology. Eighteen of the studies did not have major restrictions regarding diagnostic categories and used convenience samples of patients seeking treatment. In these studies, patients were mostly diagnosed as having axis I disorders according to DSM-IV-TR criteria (or criteria from earlier versions of DSM), among them mood, anxiety, somatoform, and eating disorders. There was considerable comorbidity with axis II disorders. The remaining four studies comprised samples of patients with mood, anxiety, eating, or cluster C personality disorders. Eighteen studies provided individual outpatient treatment. Three studies were based on inpatients who received a combination of individual and group psychotherapies. One study employed an outpatient group therapy setting. Theoretical orientations varied among treatments (Table 1). Twelve treatments were identified as psychodynamic and 11 as other treatment types, such as cognitive-behavioral, unspecified counseling, emotion-focused, and mixed forms of humanistic, client-centered, and interpersonal psychotherapy. The 22 studies used 18 different insight measures and 27 different outcome scales. Six studies examined patients’ self-reports of insight, and the remaining 16 studies used clinician-rated insight scales. As outcome measures, 12 studies employed measures of psychopathological symptoms, three studies assessed interpersonal functioning, and one study evaluated quality of life. For 13 studies, other categories, such as occupational functioning and target complaints, were used as outcome measures. Studies employed various designs to associate insight and outcome. Five effect sizes were based on cross-sectional correlations between insight and session outcome, three were correlations of insight and subsequent outcome, five were correlations of insight with change in outcome, and nine represented associations between change in insight and change in outcome.

TABLE 1. Study and Measurement Characteristics in a Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis of Insight and Outcome of Psychotherapya

Measure of Insight
StudyNType of TreatmentInstrument or MeasureTypebOutcome MeasuresType(s) of Outcome(s)Type of Associationc
Aafjes-van Doorn et al. (54)31DynamicATOSCRCORE-OM, BSI, IIP-32Symptoms, interpersonal functioning, other2
Ambühl (55)18Client-centered/CBTHRSCRSelf-developed session outcomeOther1
Andreoli et al. (29)31Supportive psychotherapyPSAICRHSRSOther3
Castonguay et al. (56)30CBTEXPCRBDI, HAM-D, GASSymptoms, other3
Cogan and Porcerelli (57)25DynamicSWAPCRGAFOther1
Connolly et al. (58)29DynamicSUIPSRBAISymptoms4
Connolly Gibbons et al. (27)124Interpersonal/dynamic, CBT, and “other”SUIP-RSRHAM-A, HAM-D, BAI, BDI, QOLISymptoms, quality of life4
Cromer and Hilsenroth (41)71DynamicCDPSCRBSI, PEI, SOS, GAF, GARF, SOFASSymptoms, interpersonal functioning, other3
Diemer et al. (59)25CounselingEvent Insight ScaleCRGSI (SCL-90)Symptoms1
Gelso et al. (60)33CounselingRQCRCOMOther3
Grande et al. (39)39DynamicHSCSCRDynamic interviewOther4
Hoffart et al. (31)35CBTPPIQ: self-understandingSRPPIQ: postsessional distressSymptoms1
Høglend (61)43DynamicDynamic interviewCRDynamic interviewOther4
Ivanova (62)16Emotion focusedCTSCSREDI-3Symptoms2
Johansson et al. (63)100DynamicSelf-developed scaleCRPFSInterpersonal functioning4
Kallestad et al. (37)24DynamicATOSCRGSI (SCL-90)Symptoms4
Kallestad et al. (37)25CBTATOSCRGSI (SCL-90)Symptoms4
Kivlighan et al. (43)12DynamicIEQ, IRSCRTCSOther2
Kolden (33)106DynamicTSR: therapeutic realizationsSRTSR: session outcomeOther1
Levy et al. (64)76DynamicSWAPCRGSI (SCL-90)Symptoms3
Luborsky et al. (35)43DynamicCCRTCRSelf-developed composite scoreOther4
Mohr et al. (65)90CounselingRQCRCOMOther3
Nyklícek et al. (42)86CBTBIPMSRSCL-90Symptoms4

aATOS=Achievement of Therapeutic Objectives Scale; BAI=Beck Anxiety Inventory; BDI=Beck Depression Inventory; BIPM=Balanced Index of Psychological Mindedness; BSI=Brief Symptom Inventory; CBT=cognitive-behavioral therapy; CCRT=core conflictual relationship themes; CDPS=Capacity for Dynamic Processes Scale; COM=Counseling Outcome Measure; CORE-OM=Clinical Outcomes in Routine Evaluation–Outcome Measure; CTSC=Client Task Specific Change Measure; EDI-3=Eating Disorder Inventory–3; EXP=Experiencing Scale; GAF=Global Assessment of Functioning; GARF=Global Assessment of Relational Functioning; GAS=Global Assessment Scale; GSI=Global Severity Index; HAM-A=Hamilton Anxiety Rating Scale; HAM-D=Hamilton Depression Rating Scale; HRS=Heuristik-Rating-Skalen; HSCS=Heidelberg Structural Change Scale; HSRS=Health-Sickness Rating Scale; IEQ=Important Events Questionnaire; IIP-32=Inventory of Interpersonal Problems–32 item; IRS=Insight Rating Scale; PEI=Patient Estimate of Improvement; PFS=Psychodynamic Functioning Scales; PPIQ=pre- and postsession impact questionnaire; PSAI=Psychotherapeutic Attainment Index; QOLI=Quality of Life Inventory; RQ=Relationship Questionnaire, insight scale; SCL-90=Symptom Checklist–90; SOFAS=Social and Occupational Functioning Assessment Scale; SOS=Schwartz Outcome Scale; SUIP(-R)=Self-Understanding of Interpersonal Patterns(–Revised); SWAP=Shedler-Westen Assessment Procedure, insight scale; TCS=Target Complaint Scale; TSR=Therapy Session Report.

bCR=clinician rated; SR=self-report.

cThis column reports which indicators of insight and outcome were correlated. 1=insight and same-session outcome; 2=insight and subsequent outcome; 3=insight and change in outcome; 4=change in insight and change in outcome.

TABLE 1. Study and Measurement Characteristics in a Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis of Insight and Outcome of Psychotherapya

Enlarge table

Overall Meta-Analysis and Heterogeneity

The random-effects model for the association between insight and outcome estimated an average true population effect size (r) of 0.31 (95% CI=0.22–0.40, p<0.05). Effect sizes and 95% confidence intervals of the studies (27, 29, 31, 33, 35, 37, 39, 4143, 5465) are presented in a forest plot in Figure 2. Eleven of the 23 reported individual effect sizes were nonsignificant within the primary study. There was significant heterogeneity in the effect sizes (Q=58.71, p<0.05). The estimated standard deviation of true effects (tau) was 0.19 (95% CI=0.10–0.28), and the ratio of true heterogeneity to total variation in the observed effects (I2) was 60.30% (95% CI=29.08−78.02). The amount of heterogeneity between studies suggested the examination of potential moderating variables. Moderator analyses were attempted for type of treatment (dynamic versus other treatments), patient diagnosis (percentage of personality disorders within study sample), and source of information on insight (observer rating versus self-report). However, the presumed statistical power for the moderator analyses was low (<0.20) (38, 66). Within treatments classified as dynamic, the magnitude of effects (r) ranged from 0.01 to 0.59. Effect sizes (r) for the studies on CBT ranged from −0.08 to 0.34, and those for the unspecified counseling studies varied from 0.24 to 0.68. The associations in the remaining studies on emotion-focused, interpersonal, client-centered, supportive, and mixed types of therapy ranged from 0.19 to 0.53.

FIGURE 2.

FIGURE 2. Forest Plot of Effect Sizes Measured as Correlations Between Insight and Outcome in a Meta-Analysis of Insight and Outcome of Psychotherapya

a In the graph, the squares represent the effect sizes, their area the relative weight assigned to each effect size, and the horizontal lines the 95% confidence intervals. The center of the diamond at the bottom depicts the mean effect size and its width the 95% confidence interval (precision of the mean estimate). The dotted line indicates the bounds of the prediction interval (distribution of true effect sizes).

Study Quality and Risk of Bias

The assessment of study quality with the risk of bias scale demonstrated moderate risks across individual studies and risk of bias categories (see Table 2). Risk of bias scores were highest for sample risks, meaning that most studies lacked sufficient sample size or representativeness or did not adequately clarify the source of the given sample. There was a high risk of measurement bias in about a third of the studies, where insight or outcome measures were not operationalized precisely or did not have adequate psychometric properties. In about half the studies, there was a notable risk of biased calculations because of attrition rates ≥5%, insight and outcome ratings provided by the same person, unclear times of measurement, same-session assessment of insight and outcome, or no direct availability of a zero-order correlation. The least concerns were present for risk of reporting bias, as most studies gave explanations for missing data, presented data clearly and accurately, and showed no indicators of selective reporting. Only three studies reported therapist effects, and they controlled for them using a multilevel modeling approach (54, 64, 65). None of the studies had a preregistered study protocol, and none explicitly stated taking preventive actions against possible researcher allegiance effects, such as blind ratings of insight or blind data analysis.

TABLE 2. Risks of Biases in Individual Studies in a Meta-Analysis of Insight and Outcome of Psychotherapya

StudyRisk of Sample BiasRisk of Measurement BiasRisk of Biased Statistical AnalysesRisk of Reporting BiasTotal Risk of Bias
Aafjes-van Doorn et al. (54)0.670.250.000.000.20
Ambühl (55)0.670.630.200.000.39
Andreoli et al. (29)0.330.380.330.000.28
Castonguay et al. (56)0.670.000.330.330.31
Cogan and Porcerelli (57)0.500.250.600.000.39
Connolly et al. (58)0.330.000.350.170.23
Connolly Gibbons et al. (27)0.170.000.420.000.19
Cromer and Hilsenroth (41)0.000.000.420.000.17
Diemer et al. (59)0.500.130.420.000.28
Gelso et al. (60)0.670.630.500.670.59
Grande et al. (39)0.500.380.250.170.31
Hoffart et al. (31)0.500.630.420.000.41
Høglend (61)0.670.630.420.000.44
Ivanova (62)0.500.000.500.330.34
Johansson et al. (63)0.170.000.500.000.23
Kallestad et al. (37)0.330.250.080.000.16
Kivlighan et al. (43)0.500.250.250.670.38
Kolden (33)0.000.130.420.500.28
Levy et al. (64)0.000.380.000.000.10
Luborsky et al. (35)0.500.380.400.500.43
Mohr et al. (65)0.170.000.470.000.21
Nyklícek et al. (42)0.000.250.500.330.31

aRisks of biases were assessed with the adapted Systematic Assessment of Quality in Observational Research Scale (see the online supplement for the complete scale). Columns list mean scores of risk of bias categories averaged across two independent raters. Scores are ratios of prevalent risks standardized by applicable risk items and yield numbers between 0 and 1; higher scores represent greater risk of bias.

TABLE 2. Risks of Biases in Individual Studies in a Meta-Analysis of Insight and Outcome of Psychotherapya

Enlarge table

Risk of Publication Bias and Sensitivity Analysis

Finally, we examined whether publication bias was a reason for concern. Visual inspection of the funnel plot for the random-effects model suggested minor asymmetry, and Egger’s regression test for funnel plot asymmetry was nonsignificant (z=−0.77, p>0.05). We applied Duval and Tweedie’s trim-and-fill procedure to assess the potential impact of a bias. The method imputed two studies to achieve funnel plot symmetry, leading to an estimated unbiased estimated effect size (r) of 0.35 (95% CI=0.25–0.45, p<0.05). The original and adjusted effect size estimates therefore differed only in the second decimal. Figure 3 presents the funnel plot including the data filled in by the trim-and-fill method.

FIGURE 3.

FIGURE 3. Funnel Plot of Transformed Correlations Between Insight and Outcome Depicted Against Their Standard Errors, in a Meta-Analysis of Insight and Outcome of Psychotherapya

a The green dots indicate the observed effect sizes, and the white dots represent the effect sizes estimated as missing by the trim-and-fill method.

Discussion

In this study, we reviewed the association between insight and psychotherapy outcome. A systematic literature search yielded 23 eligible independent effects. The random-effects meta-analysis estimated a moderate association between insight and psychotherapy outcome. Its magnitude is comparable with effect sizes of established treatment factors, such as the therapeutic alliance (67, 68), positive regard (69), and empathy (70). In other words, our findings indicate that insight may be a similarly relevant factor in psychotherapy. This is in line with clinical models about how self-understanding influences outcome: When gaining insight, patients may become aware of connections between present psychological problems and past experiences (25). They may also realize how they themselves contribute to the recurrence of these experiences (17). In terms of defense mechanisms, an increased awareness of patients’ pathological compromise formations, which contribute to the development and maintenance of the disorder, is a prerequisite to developing more adaptive ways of dealing with threatening or unpleasant experiences. This may lead to a reduction of distorted perceptions of oneself and others and to a better integration of unpleasant experiences into the conscious part of the self (21, 22). Therefore, self-understanding may lead to symptom reduction in two ways. First, understanding in itself increases patients’ sense of control and mastery. Although sometimes painful at first, knowing about one’s own contributions to the development and maintenance of symptoms is an inevitable first step in confronting difficulties, acting on, and eventually mastering them. Second, self-understanding enables patients to find new solutions and more adaptive ways of behaving (11, 16, 18).

Limitations and Further Research

Although the results of our meta-analysis are encouraging for the relevance of insight as a curative factor in psychotherapy, limitations must be noted. First and most important is that the analyses are based exclusively on correlational data and thus do not allow causal inferences. Not only could insight lead to improved well-being, but it is also possible that symptom reduction enables patients to reflect about themselves more openly and gain new insights (reverse causation). Furthermore, the association between insight and psychotherapy outcome could be caused by a third variable, such as enhanced quality of object relations. The studies discussed in this review vary in the study designs used and the conclusions that can be drawn. Cross-sectional correlations between insight and session outcome have the highest probability of confounding factors or reverse causation. For associations between insight and subsequent outcome or changes in outcome, confounding factors may still be present, but the risk of reverse causation is diminished. About half the studies in this review correlated change in insight with change in outcome and thus tested the insight-outcome association in a more rigorous way. To establish insight as a mediator, that is, a mechanism that brings about change in psychotherapy, several conditions must be met. First, the treatment must be associated with change in outcome. Second, the treatment must be associated with change in insight. Third, change in insight must predict change in outcome. Fourth, when insight is included in the statistical model, the effect of treatment on outcome must be diminished or eliminated. Fifth, it should be demonstrated that change in insight temporally precedes change in outcome (7173). In order to establish insight as a specific mechanism (as opposed to a common factor), the treatment rationale should target insight, for example, via interventions such as confrontation or interpretation for dynamic therapies. Ulberg and colleagues (71) demonstrated that increased insight during treatment mediated the effect of transference interpretations on improvements in interpersonal functioning. The data available for the present correlational meta-analysis only permitted tests of the association between insight (or change in insight) and outcome (or change in outcome). Further longitudinal research that facilitates all required steps for mediation is necessary to provide evidence for insight as a putative mediator. Nevertheless, demonstrating a significant association between self-understanding and psychotherapy outcome is an important first step to establish insight as mechanism of change (3).

Second, although the topic is relevant for a variety of therapeutic methods and inclusion criteria were defined broadly, relatively few studies were eligible for this meta-analysis. This not only reduced the precision of the population effect size estimate, but it also prevented moderator analyses because of low statistical power. Moderators are pretreatment variables that alter treatment response and thus would have answered the question of which patient characteristics or treatment conditions influence the magnitude of the insight-outcome association (7173). Further research is necessary to examine potential moderators, such as type of treatment, patient diagnosis, and the source of information on insight. Testing the type of treatment as a moderator will address whether insight should be considered a common factor or whether it is specific to psychodynamic therapy. Patient diagnosis is of interest, because different mechanisms of change may operate for patients with and without personality disorders. For example, patients with personality disorders seem to benefit more from transference interpretations that are assumed to enhance their interpersonal functioning (74, 75). Whether insight is of different importance for various groups of patients is an important topic for future research. Furthermore, the source of information on insight may be influential. Because lower insightfulness can limit patients’ ability to report on their degree of insight, studies with self-reports may yield different results compared with observer ratings.

Finally, the large number of different insight measures may be viewed as a threat to validity. Although only studies using scales that fit our preregistered definition of insight were included, the variety of measures raises the concern that different aspects of the construct were assessed. The different measures may have caused part of the heterogeneity between study effect sizes. Nonetheless, the amount of true heterogeneity in our study was within the usual range of meta-analyses on change mechanisms (such as the alliance [67, 68]) and still led to a considerably precise population effect size estimate that allows conclusions on the insight-outcome association.

Conclusions

To our knowledge, this study is the first systematic review and meta-analysis of the association between insight and outcome of psychotherapy. Across studies, more insight is moderately associated with better psychotherapy outcome. Although further research is needed to understand determinants of the association, this contributes to our understanding of how and why the investigated psychotherapies work.

From the Department of General Internal Medicine and Psychosomatics and the Institute of Medical Psychology, Heidelberg University, Heidelberg, Germany.
Address correspondence to Dr. Dinger () and Ms. Jennissen ().

The authors report no financial relationships with commercial interests.

References

1 Barber JP: Toward a working through of some core conflicts in psychotherapy research. Psychother Res 2009; 19:1–12Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

2 Crits-Christoph P, Connolly Gibbons MB, Mukherjee D: Psychotherapy process-outcome research, in Handbook of Psychotherapy and Behavior Change. Edited by Lambert MJ. New York, Wiley, 2013, pp 298–340Google Scholar

3 Kazdin AE: Mediators and mechanisms of change in psychotherapy research. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 2007; 3:1–27Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

4 Lambert MJ: The efficacy and effectiveness of psychotherapy, in Handbook of Psychotherapy and Behavior Change. Edited by Lambert MJ. New York, Wiley, 2013, pp 169–218Google Scholar

5 Orlinsky DE: The “generic model of psychotherapy” after 25 years: evolution of a research-based metatheory. J Psychother Integration 2009; 19:319–339CrossrefGoogle Scholar

6 Norcross JC, Lambert MJ: Psychotherapy relationships that work, II. Psychotherapy (Chic) 2011; 48:4–8Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

7 Insel T, Cuthbert B, Garvey M, et al.: Research Domain Criteria (RDoC): toward a new classification framework for research on mental disorders. Am J Psychiatry 2010; 167:748–751LinkGoogle Scholar

8 Freud S, Breuer J: Studien über Hysterie. Leipzig and Vienna, Franz Deuticke, 1895Google Scholar

9 Strachey J: The nature of the therapeutic action of psycho-analysis. Int J Psychoanal 1934; 15:127–159Google Scholar

10 Michels R: Oedipus and insight. Psychoanal Q 1986; 55:599–617Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

11 Gabbard GO: Psychodynamic Psychiatry in Clinical Practice. Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Publishing, 2014Google Scholar

12 Sandler J, Dreher AU, Dare C, et al.: The Patient and the Analyst: The Basis of the Psychoanalytic Process. London, Karnac Books, 1992Google Scholar

13 Huprich SK: Psychodynamic Therapy: Conceptual and Empirical Foundations. New York, Routledge, 2009Google Scholar

14 Safran J: Brief relational psychoanalytic treatment. Psychoanal Dialogues 2002; 12:171–195CrossrefGoogle Scholar

15 Caligor E, Kernberg OF, Clarkin JF: Handbook of Dynamic Psychotherapy for Higher Level Personality Pathology. Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Publishing, 2007Google Scholar

16 Luborsky L: Principles of Psychoanalytic Psychotherapy: A Manual for Supportive-Expressive Treatment. New York, Basic Books, 1984Google Scholar

17 Strupp HH, Binder JL: Psychotherapy in a New Key: A Guide to Time-Limited Psychotherapy. New York, Basic Books, 1984Google Scholar

18 Rosenblatt A: Insight, working through, and practice: the role of procedural knowledge. J Am Psychoanal Assoc 2004; 52:189–207Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

19 Leichsenring F, Schauenburg H: Empirically supported methods of short-term psychodynamic therapy in depression: towards an evidence-based unified protocol. J Affect Disord 2014; 169:128–143Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

20 Lemma A, Target M, Fonagy P: Dynamic interpersonal therapy (DIT): developing a new psychodynamic intervention for the treatment of depression. Psychoanal Inq 2013; 33:552–566CrossrefGoogle Scholar

21 Lacewing M: Psychodynamic psychotherapy, insight, and therapeutic action. Clin Psychol Sci Pract 2014; 21:154–171CrossrefGoogle Scholar

22 Messer SB, McWilliams N: Insight in psychodynamic therapy: theory and assessment, in Insight in Psychotherapy. Edited by Castonguay LG, Hill CE. Washington, DC, American Psychological Association, 2007, pp 9–29CrossrefGoogle Scholar

23 Pascual-Leone A, Greenberg LS: Insight and awareness in experiential therapy, in Insight in Psychotherapy. Edited by Castonguay LG, Hill CE. Washington, DC, American Psychological Association, 2007, pp 31–56CrossrefGoogle Scholar

24 Grosse Holtforth M, Castonguay LG, Boswell JF, et al.: Insight in cognitive-behavioral therapy, in Insight in Psychotherapy. Edited by Castonguay LG, Hill CE. Washington, DC, American Psychological Association, 2007, pp 57–99CrossrefGoogle Scholar

25 Hill CE, Castonguay LG, Angus L, et al.: Insight in psychotherapy: definitions, processes, consequences, and research directions, in Insight in Psychotherapy. Edited by Castonguay LG, Hill CE. Washington, DC, American Psychological Association, 2007, pp 441–454CrossrefGoogle Scholar

26 Connolly Gibbons MB, Crits-Christoph P, Barber JP, et al.: Insight in psychotherapy: a review of empirical literature, in Insight in Psychotherapy. Edited by Castonguay LG, Hill CE. Washington, DC, American Psychological Association, 2007, pp 143–165CrossrefGoogle Scholar

27 Connolly Gibbons MB, Crits-Christoph P, Barber JP, et al.: Unique and common mechanisms of change across cognitive and dynamic psychotherapies. J Consult Clin Psychol 2009; 77:801–813Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

28 Wampold BE, Mondin GW, Moody M, et al.: A meta-analysis of outcome studies comparing bona fide psychotherapies: empiricially, “all must have prizes”. Psychol Bull 1997; 122:203–215CrossrefGoogle Scholar

29 Andreoli AV, Muehlebach A, Gognalons M, et al.: Crisis intervention response and long-term outcome: a pilot study. Compr Psychiatry 1992; 33:388–396Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

30 Andreoli A, Frances A, Gex-Fabry M, et al.: Crisis intervention in depressed patients with and without DSM-III-R personality disorders. J Nerv Ment Dis 1993; 181:732–737Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

31 Hoffart A, Versland S, Sexton H: Self-understanding, empathy, guided discovery, and schema belief in schema-focused cognitive therapy of personality problems: a process-outcome study. Cognit Ther Res 2002; 26:199–219CrossrefGoogle Scholar

32 Hoffart A, Sexton H: The role of optimism in the process of schema-focused cognitive therapy of personality problems. Behav Res Ther 2002; 40:611–623Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

33 Kolden GG: The generic model of psychotherapy: an empirical investigation of patterns of process and outcome relationships. Psychother Res 1991; 1:62–73CrossrefGoogle Scholar

34 Kolden GG: Effective microprocesses in early sessions of dynamic psychotherapy. J Psychother Pract Res 1996; 5:122–131MedlineGoogle Scholar

35 Luborsky L, Crits-Christoph P, Mintz J, et al.: The central relationship pattern: self-understanding of core relationship themes, in Who Will Benefit From Psychotherapy? Predicting Therapeutic Outcomes. Edited by Luborsky L, Crits-Christoph P, Mintz J, et al.. New York, Basic Books, 1988, pp 243–249Google Scholar

36 Grenyer BFS, Luborsky L: Dynamic change in psychotherapy: mastery of interpersonal conflicts. J Consult Clin Psychol 1996; 64:411–416Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

37 Kallestad H, Valen J, McCullough L, et al.: The relationship between insight gained during therapy and long-term outcome in short-term dynamic psychotherapy and cognitive therapy for cluster C personality disorders. Psychother Res 2010; 20:526–534Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

38 Borenstein M, Hedges LV, Higgins JPT, et al.: Introduction to Meta-Analysis. Chichester, UK, Wiley, 2009CrossrefGoogle Scholar

39 Grande T, Rudolf G, Oberbracht C, et al.: Progressive changes in patients’ lives after psychotherapy: which treatment effects support them? Psychother Res 2003; 13:43–58Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

40 Luborsky L, Crits-Christoph P: The assessment of transference by the CCRT method, in Psychoanalytic Process Research Strategies. Edited by Dahl H, Kächele H, Thomä H. Berlin, Springer, 1988, pp 99–108CrossrefGoogle Scholar

41 Cromer TD, Hilsenroth MJ: Patient personality and outcome in short-term psychodynamic psychotherapy. J Nerv Ment Dis 2010; 198:59–66Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

42 Nyklícek I, Majoor D, Schalken PAAM: Psychological mindedness and symptom reduction after psychotherapy in a heterogeneous psychiatric sample. Compr Psychiatry 2010; 51:492–496Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

43 Kivlighan DMJ, Multon KD, Patton MJ: Insight and symptom reduction in time-limited psychoanalytic counseling. J Couns Psychol 2000; 47:50–58CrossrefGoogle Scholar

44 Ross LE, Grigoriadis S, Mamisashvili L, et al.: Quality assessment of observational studies in psychiatry: an example from perinatal psychiatric research. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res 2011; 20:224–234Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

45 Hunter JE, Schmidt FL: Methods of Meta-Analysis: Correcting Error and Bias in Research Findings. Thousand Oaks, Calif, Sage, 2004CrossrefGoogle Scholar

46 Shrout PE, Fleiss JL: Intraclass correlations: uses in assessing rater reliability. Psychol Bull 1979; 86:420–428Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

47 Viechtbauer W: Conducting meta-analyses in R with the metafor package. J Stat Softw 2010; 36:1–48CrossrefGoogle Scholar

48 Hedges LV, Vevea JL: Fixed- and random-effects models in meta-analysis. Psychol Methods 1998; 3:486–504CrossrefGoogle Scholar

49 Viechtbauer W: Bias and efficiency of meta-analytic variance estimators in the random-effects model. J Educ Behav Stat 2005; 30:261–293CrossrefGoogle Scholar

50 Higgins JP, Thompson SG, Deeks JJ, et al.: Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. BMJ 2003; 327:557–560Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

51 Cafri G, Kromrey JD, Brannick MT: A SAS macro for statistical power calculations in meta-analysis. Behav Res Methods 2009; 41:35–46Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

52 Hedges LV, Pigott TD: The power of statistical tests in meta-analysis. Psychol Methods 2001; 6:203–217Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

53 Duval S, Tweedie R: Trim and fill: a simple funnel-plot-based method of testing and adjusting for publication bias in meta-analysis. Biometrics 2000; 56:455–463Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

54 Aafjes-van Doorn K, Lilliengren P, Cooper A, et al.: Patients’ affective processes within initial experiential dynamic therapy sessions. Psychotherapy (Chic) 2017; 54:175–183Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

55 Ambühl H: [What is therapeutic in psychotherapy? An empirical evaluation of assumptions of the “generic model of psychotherapy”]. Z Klin Psychol Psychopathol Psychother 1993; 41:285–303 (German)MedlineGoogle Scholar

56 Castonguay LG, Goldfried MR, Wiser S, et al.: Predicting the effect of cognitive therapy for depression: a study of unique and common factors. J Consult Clin Psychol 1996; 64:497–504Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

57 Cogan R, Porcerelli JH: Validation of the Shedler-Westen assessment procedure insight scale. J Nerv Ment Dis 2013; 201:706–708Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

58 Connolly MB, Crits-Christoph P, Shelton RC, et al.: The reliability and validity of a measure of self-understanding of interpersonal patterns. J Couns Psychol 1999; 46:472–482CrossrefGoogle Scholar

59 Diemer RA, Lobell LK, Vivino BL, et al.: Comparison of dream interpretation, event, interpretation, and unstructured sessions in brief therapy. J Couns Psychol 1996; 43:99–112CrossrefGoogle Scholar

60 Gelso CJ, Wine B, Kiylighan DM, et al.: Transference, insight, and the course of time-limited therapy. J Couns Psychol 1997; 44:209–217CrossrefGoogle Scholar

61 Høglend P, Engelstad V, Sørbye O, et al.: The role of insight in exploratory psychodynamic psychotherapy. Br J Med Psychol 1994; 67:305–317Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

62 Ivanova I: The “how” of change in emotion-focused group therapy for eating disorders (doctoral dissertation). Toronto, University of Toronto, Ontario Institute for Studies in Education, Graduate Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development, 2013Google Scholar

63 Johansson P, Høglend P, Ulberg R, et al.: The mediating role of insight for long-term improvements in psychodynamic therapy. J Consult Clin Psychol 2010; 78:438–448Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

64 Levy SR, Hilsenroth MJ, Owen JJ: Relationship between interpretation, alliance, and outcome in psychodynamic psychotherapy: control of therapist effects and assessment of moderator variable impact. J Nerv Ment Dis 2015; 203:418–424Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

65 Mohr JJ, Fuertes JN, Stracuzzi TI: Transference and insight in psychotherapy with gay and bisexual male clients: the role of sexual orientation identity integration. Psychotherapy (Chic) 2015; 52:119–126Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

66 Hempel S, Miles JN, Booth MJ, et al.: Risk of bias: a simulation study of power to detect study-level moderator effects in meta-analysis. Syst Rev 2013; 2:107Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

67 Flückiger C, Del Re AC, Wampold BE, et al.: How central is the alliance in psychotherapy? A multilevel longitudinal meta-analysis. J Couns Psychol 2012; 59:10–17Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

68 Horvath AO, Del Re AC, Flückiger C, et al.: Alliance in individual psychotherapy. Psychotherapy (Chic) 2011; 48:9–16Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

69 Farber BA, Doolin EM: Positive regard. Psychotherapy (Chic) 2011; 48:58–64Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

70 Elliott R, Bohart AC, Watson JC, et al.: Empathy. Psychotherapy (Chic) 2011; 48:43–49Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

71 Ulberg R, Amlo S, Dahl HSJ, et al.: Does insight mediate treatment and enhance outcome? Psychoanal Inq 2017; 37:140–152CrossrefGoogle Scholar

72 Baron RM, Kenny DA: The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. J Pers Soc Psychol 1986; 51:1173–1182Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

73 Hayes AF, Rockwood NJ: Regression-based statistical mediation and moderation analysis in clinical research: observations, recommendations, and implementation. Behav Res Ther 2017; 98:39–57Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

74 Høglend P, Dahl HS, Hersoug AG, et al.: Long-term effects of transference interpretation in dynamic psychotherapy of personality disorders. Eur Psychiatry 2011; 26:419–424Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

75 Levy KN, Clarkin JF, Yeomans FE, et al.: The mechanisms of change in the treatment of borderline personality disorder with transference focused psychotherapy. J Clin Psychol 2006; 62:481–501Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar