The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×
ArticlesFull Access

Common and Dissociable Mechanisms of Executive System Dysfunction Across Psychiatric Disorders in Youth

Abstract

Objective:

Disruption of executive function is present in many neuropsychiatric disorders. However, determining the specificity of executive dysfunction across these disorders is challenging given high comorbidity of conditions. Here the authors investigate executive system deficits in association with dimensions of psychiatric symptoms in youth using a working memory paradigm. The authors hypothesize that common and dissociable patterns of dysfunction would be present.

Method:

The authors studied 1,129 youths who completed a fractal n-back task during functional magnetic resonance imaging at 3-T as part of the Philadelphia Neurodevelopmental Cohort. Factor scores of clinical psychopathology were calculated using an item-wise confirmatory bifactor model, describing overall psychopathology as well as four orthogonal dimensions of symptoms: anxious-misery (mood and anxiety), behavioral disturbance (attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and conduct disorder), psychosis-spectrum symptoms, and fear (phobias). The effect of psychopathology dimensions on behavioral performance and executive system recruitment (2-back > 0-back) was examined using both multivariate (matrix regression) and mass-univariate (linear regression) analyses.

Results:

Overall psychopathology was associated with both abnormal multivariate patterns of activation and a failure to activate executive regions within the cingulo-opercular control network, including the frontal pole, cingulate cortex, and anterior insula. In addition, psychosis-spectrum symptoms were associated with hypoactivation of the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, whereas behavioral symptoms were associated with hypoactivation of the frontoparietal cortex and cerebellum. In contrast, anxious-misery symptoms were associated with widespread hyperactivation of the executive network.

Conclusions:

These findings provide novel evidence that common and dissociable deficits within the brain’s executive system are present in association with dimensions of psychopathology in youth.

Deficits of executive function are present in a wide range of psychiatric disorders, including attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) (1), conduct disorder (2), and psychotic disorders such as schizophrenia (3, 4). Executive deficits negatively affect everyday functioning (5) and contribute to diminished quality of life in many clinical populations (6, 7). Consequences of executive deficits may be particularly acute in childhood and adolescence, and they may include increased interpersonal conflict, decreased academic achievement, and risk-taking behavior (7, 8).

Many studies have investigated the neural basis of executive impairments in individual psychiatric disorders. Working memory is one of the most commonly studied components of executive function. For example, meta-analyses in patients with ADHD demonstrate hypoactivation within a network of regions, including the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, anterior cingulate cortex, thalamus, superior parietal lobule, and precuneus (9, 10). Patients with schizophrenia also fail to recruit portions of the executive network, such as the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (1113). Likewise, conduct disorder is associated with a failure to activate a similar network of regions (14). Taken together, these studies suggest there likely are overlapping effects across disorders, with hypoactivation of the executive system being a common underlying brain phenotype. However, evidence also exists for dissociable abnormalities among psychiatric disorders. For example, hyperactivation of executive regions has been reported in major depressive disorder and anxiety disorders (1517). Nonetheless, some heterogeneity in results exists, and such hyperactivation has been reported in schizophrenia as well (18).

A case-control design in a single disorder is the predominant approach in the studies reviewed above, and thus most studies are unable to directly evaluate whether common executive deficits exist across disorders. Furthermore, while the incidence of psychiatric comorbidity is quite high, studies typically do not explicitly evaluate its impact. In epidemiological studies, approximately 40% of individuals meeting criteria for one diagnosis met criteria for at least one additional disorder in a different class (19). Thus, studies that use subjects with “pure” single diagnoses may not be representative. More recently, the neural basis of executive deficits across disorders has begun to be investigated in an attempt to isolate regions of dysfunction specific to each diagnosis (20). However, studies comparing multiple clinical groups are typically hampered by small sample size, and few prior studies compared more than two disorders at a time. Furthermore, because psychiatric symptomatology exists on a continuum of normal to abnormal, dimensional analyses that cut across categorical clinical diagnoses may both enhance power and improve biological interpretability (21). Thus, studies in large samples evaluating the effect of multiple dimensions of psychopathology on executive functioning are necessary.

Accordingly, here we used a dimensional approach to examine executive dysfunction with a working memory functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) task in a sample of 1,129 youths imaged as part of the Philadelphia Neurodevelopmental Cohort (22, 23). Notably, the ascertainment strategy used in the Philadelphia Neurodevelopmental Cohort differs substantially from studies using clinical help-seeking samples, instead employing a community-based approach to examine symptoms in non-help-seeking youths. While such a design will likely have lower symptom severity than clinical samples, a community-based approach may nonetheless be valuable for investigating dimensions of psychopathology. We hypothesized that there would be both common and dissociable deficits in executive recruitment associated with different dimensions of psychopathology. Specifically, we predicted that general psychopathology would be associated with executive hypoactivation regardless of clinical diagnosis. Furthermore, we predicted that specific dimensions of psychopathology would be linked to dissociable regional patterns of impairment within the executive network.

Method

Participants

As has been described, the Philadelphia Neurodevelopmental Cohort is a collaboration between the Center for Applied Genomics at the Children’s Hospital of Philadelphia and the Brain Behavior Laboratory at the University of Pennsylvania (22, 23). In this report, we consider the entire cross-sectional sample of 1,601 subjects imaged as part of the Philadelphia Neurodevelopmental Cohort; of these, 1,462 completed the n-back task described below. Following subject exclusions—including medical comorbidity, task nonperformance, and image quality assurance (see the data supplement that accompanies the online edition of this article)—the final sample included in the analyses comprised 1,129 subjects (mean age=15.5 years, SD=3.4 years; 520 were male). This sample thus constitutes a superset of subjects previously included in reports that focused on effects of working memory performance (24) and associations with psychosis-spectrum symptoms (25).

Clinical Assessment

Psychopathology symptoms were evaluated using a structured screening interview (GOASSESS), which has been detailed elsewhere (26, 27). Computerized algorithms used endorsement of symptoms, their frequency and duration, and the presence of distress or impairment to determine whether DSM-IV criteria were met (see the online data supplement). The frequency and relevant demographic data for each screening diagnosis considered are detailed in Table 1. As in large-scale epidemiologic data sets (19), comorbidity was quite common, with more subjects meeting criteria for more than one category (N=529) than for a single category (N=249).

TABLE 1. Demographic Information by Screening Diagnostic Categorya

Age (Years)Maternal Education (Years)N-Back Performance (d′)
Screening Diagnostic CategoryNFemale (%)Caucasian (%)MeanSDMeanSDMeanSD
Typically developing35150.758.715.343.8814.812.572.960.65
ADHD17440.842.014.703.1614.122.562.690.64
Agoraphobia6677.328.816.132.6213.552.082.690.57
Anorexia1369.253.816.121.9813.622.262.780.81
Bulimia4100.0100.018.311.0415.502.523.410.38
Conduct disorder9543.218.916.202.7712.932.202.600.61
Major depression16663.945.817.472.3613.932.382.960.61
Generalized anxiety disorder2152.457.116.463.0214.763.033.040.89
Mania1361.515.416.842.9413.751.822.850.55
Obsessive-compulsive disorder3470.641.216.823.0713.852.582.980.84
Oppositional defiant disorder36851.135.315.592.9013.742.382.740.65
Panic875.025.015.682.6312.751.042.750.37
Phobias35166.142.215.243.3314.152.412.800.67
Psychosis-spectrum32151.432.716.142.9713.792.192.760.64
PTSD14665.835.616.802.9013.662.352.850.61
Separation anxiety5558.254.515.153.5014.252.282.920.48
Social phobia28158.735.915.883.0213.912.412.840.66

aIndividual subjects may be present in multiple categories due to comorbidity.

TABLE 1. Demographic Information by Screening Diagnostic Categorya

Enlarge table

Psychopathology Factor Analysis

In order to parse this comorbidity into orthogonal dimensions of psychopathology, we performed factor analyses of item-level data from GOASSESS. To produce stable factor scores, analyses included all subjects for whom clinical data were available (N=9,498), rather than only the subset who completed neuroimaging (26, 27). As initial exploratory factor analyses indicated correlated traits of psychopathology, we used a bifactor confirmatory model, which can produce orthogonal scores from correlated traits (28).

Methods for estimating the bifactor model will be presented in detail elsewhere (see also the online data supplement). Briefly, the confirmatory bifactor model (Figure 1A) was estimated using a Bayesian estimator in Mplus, version 7.1. As predicted by theory and supported by initial exploratory models, the four factors primarily represent anxious-misery (mood and anxiety) symptoms, psychosis-spectrum symptoms, behavioral symptoms (conduct and ADHD), and fear symptoms (phobias). Additionally, the bifactor model estimated a general psychopathology factor, representing the overall burden of psychopathology while controlling for the presence of specific symptom dimensions. Importantly, all five factors (including general psychopathology) from the bifactor model are orthogonal and can be considered jointly in analysis of imaging data. Factor scores within each of the categorical screening categories were as expected, given item-level loading (Figure 1B).

FIGURE 1.

FIGURE 1. Bifactor Model of Common and Divergent Dimensions of Psychopathology Across Categorical Screening Diagnosesa

a The input data to the bifactor model were 112 items from the GOASSESS screening diagnostic interview, resulting in orthogonal dimensions of psychopathology (panel A). Mean factor scores of each dimension for each screening diagnosis (with at least N=20) are displayed in panel B. MDD=major depressive disorder; GAD=generalized anxiety disorder; OCD=obsessive-compulsive disorder; ODD=oppositional defiant disorder; PS=psychosis-spectrum symptoms; TD=typically developing.

Task Paradigm, Image Acquisition, and Image Processing

Task paradigm, image acquisition, and preprocessing methods were as previously reported (24). Briefly, a fractal version of the n-back task (29) was used to probe executive system function across three levels of working memory load (Figure 2A). The primary behavioral measure was d′, a signal detection metric that limits the influence of response bias. Task performance (d′) across all levels of working memory load was related to categorical diagnosis from GOASSESS and dimensional factor scores using linear models while controlling for age and sex. The Bonferroni correction was used to account for the testing of five dimensions of psychopathology.

FIGURE 2.

FIGURE 2. Working Memory Task Paradigm, Behavioral Performance, and Contrast of Interesta

a A fractal version of the n-back task was used to probe working memory function where working memory load was varied parametrically across three levels (panel A). Higher working memory load was associated with fewer correct responses to targets (panel B) and more false positive responses to foils (panel C). Correct responses and false positives were summarized across all levels of working memory load using the signal detection metric d′ to provide an integrated measure of working memory performance. Task performance was reduced in association with both overall psychopathology and behavioral symptoms; anxious-misery was conversely associated with a trend toward improved task performance (panel D). The contrast of interest (2-back > 0-back) robustly activated the entire executive system and deactivated nonexecutive regions (panel E).

A single scanner (Siemens 3-T Tim Trio) was used to acquire fMRI, T1, and B0 images for all subjects (see the data supplement). Time-series analysis of subject-level imaging data used the FMRIB Software Library version 5.0 (http://www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl) (FSL) (30) to model three condition blocks (0-back, 1-back, and 2-back); the primary contrast was 2-back > 0-back, which robustly recruits the executive network (24). Subject-level statistical maps were distortion corrected, coregistered to the T1 image using boundary-based registration, normalized to the Montreal Neurological Institute’s (MNI) 152 1 mm template using Advanced Normalization Tools (31), and then downsampled to 2 mm. All transformations were concatenated so only one interpolation was performed.

Multivariate Group-Level Analysis: Global Associations With Psychopathology

As a first step, we evaluated the degree to which dimensions of psychopathology from the factor analysis affected overall multivariate patterns of activation (see Figure S1 in the online data supplement). To do this, we used multivariate distance-based matrix regression, a statistical technique developed originally for large-scale ecologic data sets but which has recently been used in image analysis (32); this was implemented using the “vegan” package in R (33). Subject-level activation maps are compared on a pairwise basis (Euclidean distance) to yield a distance matrix. Matrix regression is then used to test whether each phenotypic variable explains the distances among each participant’s activation patterns. In contrast to other multivariate methods, this approach allowed us to examine the influence of multiple dimensions of psychopathology simultaneously while also controlling for covariates (age, sex, and in-scanner motion). Multiple tests were accounted for using the Bonferroni correction as above.

Mass-Univariate Group-Level Analysis: Regional Associations With Psychopathology

While the multivariate analysis detailed above provided an estimate of the degree to which dimensions of psychopathology affected the global pattern of activation, this analysis does not evaluate regional effects. Accordingly, we next conducted a standard mass-univariate analysis using a general linear model implemented in FSL (30). We evaluated the effect of each dimension of psychopathology within this linear model, with covariates as noted above. Additionally, we investigated whether dimensions of psychopathology that showed a significant effect were significantly different from one another using an F test across dimensions. Type I error control was provided by cluster correction using 10,000 Monte Carlo simulations (voxel height of z>3.09; cluster probability of p<0.001; minimum cluster size of k=67) (34). All analyses described below used unmasked, whole-brain, voxelwise data. Images were displayed using Caret.

Supplementary Analyses

To evaluate whether potentially confounding factors influenced observed results, we conducted a series of supplementary analyses. These included removing the minority (11.4%) of subjects who were taking psychoactive medication; counting task performance (d′) as a covariate; excluding subjects with poor performance (>7 nonresponses) on the 2-back condition; and using mean accuracy on an out-of-scanner computerized neuropsychological battery, race, assessment-scan interval, maternal education, and in-scanner performance (d′) together as covariates in one model.

Results

Multiple Dimensions of Psychopathology Affect Task Performance

As expected, increasing working memory load was associated with fewer correct responses to targets and increased false positive responses to foils (Figure 2B and Figure 2C). These measures were integrated using the signal detection measure d′. As expected, d′ varied considerably by screening categorical diagnosis (see Table 1). When summarized as psychopathology dimensions, several factors significantly affected working memory task performance (Figure 2D). Higher levels of both overall psychopathology (t=2.58, df=1124, p=0.001) and behavioral symptoms (t=3.82, df=1124, p=0.0001) were associated with lower working memory performance. In contrast, higher levels of anxious-misery symptoms were associated with a trend toward better working memory performance that did not survive correction for multiple comparisons (t=2.45, df=1124, p=0.015). There was not a significant relationship between fear or psychosis and working memory performance.

Overall Psychopathology Alters Global Patterns of Executive System Recruitment

As expected, the 2-back > 0-back contrast robustly recruited the entire executive network and resulted in deactivation of nonexecutive regions (Figure 2E). We next evaluated whether dimensional psychopathology was associated with changes in this multivariate pattern of activation and deactivation using multivariate distance-based matrix regression. This procedure revealed that overall psychopathology was associated with a significant disturbance in the global pattern of executive system recruitment (p=0.009). Other symptom dimensions did not have a significant relationship; nonsignificant trends were observed for behavioral (p=0.044) and anxious-misery (p=0.092) symptoms.

Dimensions of Psychopathology Differentially Affect Regional Executive Activation

Multivariate distance-based matrix regression evaluates the presence of global multivariate effects, but it is not sensitive to regional changes. Accordingly, we used mass-univariate generalized linear models to examine the relationship between dimensions of psychopathology and executive system activation (Figure 3; see also Table S1 in the online data supplement). As suggested by multivariate results, overall psychopathology had the most robust impact on executive activation. Higher levels of overall psychopathology were associated with diminished activation of the left and right frontal pole, anterior cingulate cortex, anterior insula, thalamus, and precuneus. Behavioral symptoms were associated with diminished activation of the frontoparietal cortex as well as the thalamus and cerebellum. Psychosis-spectrum symptoms were associated with diminished activation of the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Fear (phobia symptoms) was associated with diminished activation of a marginally significant cluster of the medial frontal cortex, but this effect did not survive supplementary analyses (see below) and was not evaluated further. Finally, in contrast to the hypoactivation described above, anxious-misery symptoms were associated with a marked hyperactivation of multiple executive regions including the anterior cingulate cortex, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, parietal cortex, and thalamus. Notably, a small area of overlapping significant effects across dimensions was seen in the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (MNI coordinates: x=−28, y=−6, z=58; k=10). Significant differential effects of each dimension were present in multiple executive regions (Figure 4; see also Table S2 in the data supplement). These effects were driven by the divergent impact of global psychopathology (hypoactivation) and anxious-misery (hyperactivation). We did not find any differences in default mode regions where task-induced deactivation was present.

FIGURE 3.

FIGURE 3. Relationship of Orthogonal Dimensions of Psychopathology With Executive Network Recruitmenta

a The association of each dimension of psychopathology with executive network activation (2-back > 0-back) was examined using linear regression. The most robust effects were seen for overall psychopathology, where greater symptom severity was associated with diminished activation of the frontal pole, anterior cingulate, anterior insula, thalamus, and precuneus. Behavioral symptoms were associated with frontoparietal, thalamic, and cerebellar hypoactivation; psychosis-spectrum symptoms were associated with diminished activation of the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. In contrast to the observed hypoactivation for all other dimensions of psychopathology, anxious-misery was associated with hyperactivation of regions within the executive network, including the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulate cortex. All analyses included age, sex, and in-scanner motion as covariates; images were thresholded at a z value of 3.09 and a corrected p value of 0.001. Scatter plots were drawn from significant clusters identified by red circles. Activation is plotted in arbitrary blood-oxygen-level-dependent units. For detailed results, see Table S1 in the online data supplement.

Figure 4.

Figure 4. Differential Effects of Psychopathology on Executive System Activationa

a A linear model comparing the differential impact of each dimension of psychopathology revealed significant differences in multiple executive regions, including the anterior cingulate cortex, frontal pole, and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Differential effects were driven by the divergent impact of anxious-misery (hyperactivation) and overall psychopathology (hypoactivation). Activation is plotted in arbitrary blood-oxygen-level-dependent units. The shaded area represents 95% CIs. For detailed results, see Table S2 in the online data supplement.

Supplementary Analyses

Overall, convergent results were obtained from supplementary analyses when participants taking psychoactive medications were excluded (Table S3 in the data supplement), when working memory performance was included as a covariate (Table S4 in the data supplement), and when multiple additional covariates were included (Table S5 in the data supplement). When participants with poor performance on the 2-back condition were excluded, psychosis-spectrum symptoms were associated with a marginally significant cluster of hyperactivation in the cingulate gyrus; otherwise, results were similar (Table S6 in the data supplement). As noted above, fear did not have a significant association with activation in any of the supplementary analyses.

Discussion

In this large study of psychopathology in youth, we examined associations between diverse psychopathology and activation of the brain’s executive system during a working memory task. To account for the fact that psychiatric disorders are highly comorbid, factor analysis of the psychopathology data using a bifactor model allowed us to examine both general psychopathology and orthogonal dimensions of psychopathology. Overall psychopathology was associated with a significant alteration of the global multivariate pattern of activation. Furthermore, dimensions of psychopathology significantly influenced regional patterns of activation in different ways. In contrast to the hypoactivation seen in association with overall psychopathology, anxious-misery symptoms were associated with hyperactivation of the same network. Taken together, these data emphasize that dimensions of psychiatric symptomatology are associated with both common and distinct deficits within the executive system.

Evidence for Common Executive Deficits Across Psychiatric Syndromes in Youth

The most robust finding in the present study is evidence of common executive deficits attributable to overall psychopathology present across categorical clinical diagnoses. This was observed on both a global and local scale: overall psychopathology was associated with an alteration of the global multivariate pattern of activation, driven by regional hypoactivation in a network of executive regions including the frontal pole, anterior cingulate, anterior insula, and precuneus. These results accord with a copious literature of case-control studies from multiple individual disorders and, moreover, highlight the centrality of executive dysfunction across major psychiatric syndromes.

We were able to estimate the influence of overall psychopathology across disorders through the use of a bifactor analysis of the item-level responses from the psychopathology screening interview. This approach obviates several major obstacles to estimating common deficits across traditional categorical diagnoses. First, when comorbidity is represented in standard models as shared variance, it is controlled for but cannot be estimated. Estimating overall psychopathology may be particularly important in youths, where the pattern of psychopathology may not fit standard diagnostic criteria in the context of ongoing development. Second, strongly nonrandom patterns of comorbidity exist between different disorders (19). For example, mood and anxiety disorders have a high rate of comorbidity, as do ADHD and conduct disorder. Third, the frequency of each diagnosis varies considerably; thus, the statistical power to estimate the effect of each individual diagnosis is not equal across effects. Fourth, categorical diagnoses are by definition dichotomous, and important dimensional effects cannot be examined.

Crucially, each of these problems can be overcome through modeling of latent dimensions of psychopathology. Here we used confirmatory factor analysis to estimate a bifactor model, which provides orthogonal scores for every subject in each psychopathology domain, as well as a score for general psychopathology. These orthogonal scores can thus be included in a single model that can estimate dissociable dimensions of psychopathology with equal power, as well as the impact of overall psychopathology through the general score. The regions implicated by overall psychopathology include the frontal pole, anterior cingulate cortex, and anterior insula. Notably, all these regions are part of the cingulo-opercular control network (also called the salience or ventral attention network), which is consistently identified as among the most reproducible large-scale functional brain networks both at rest and during task performance (35). This network is critical for cognitive control and is particularly implicated in maintenance of task sets and error monitoring (36). These regions are among the most commonly impaired in case-control studies of schizophrenia, ADHD, and conduct disorder, suggesting that dysfunction of this network is not associated with one specific domain of psychopathology (4, 10, 37). Further support of this theory is provided by results of a large-scale meta-analysis by Goodkind et al. (38), who found that gray matter loss in these cingulo-opercular regions was present across psychiatric disorders and was associated with impaired executive function.

Orthogonal Dimensions of Psychopathology Are Associated With Dissociable Deficits

In addition to robust effects of overall psychopathology, we also identified both regionally and directionally dissociable effects of specific dimensions of psychopathology. Behavioral symptoms were associated with diminished activation of a network of regions, including the frontoparietal cortex, thalamus, and cerebellum, whereas psychosis-spectrum symptoms were associated with hypoactivation of the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. The behavioral dimension loaded most prominently onto items assessing externalizing disorders such as ADHD, conduct disorder, and oppositional defiant disorder. The results are consistent with case-control studies in each of these disorders, which have demonstrated hypofunction of executive regions including, most commonly, frontoparietal regions (10, 20). Similarly, dysfunction of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex is considered one of the cardinal impairments of psychosis (39). However, it should be noted that the effects of the psychosis spectrum observed here are more circumscribed than those typically reported; this may be due to the use of a population rather than a clinical sampling strategy, a focus on subthreshold psychosis symptoms, and accounting for overall psychopathology through the bifactor model. However, consistent with the present results, our previous report on psychosis-spectrum symptoms in this sample observed that hypoactivation of executive regions was linked to cognitive performance but not to severity of positive psychotic symptoms, suggesting a substantial effect of overall impairment rather than psychosis per se (25).

In contrast to the diminished recruitment seen in other symptom domains, anxious-misery symptoms were associated with widespread hyperactivation of the executive network. Indeed, after accounting for overall psychopathology, anxious-misery symptoms were associated with a trend toward better working memory performance. However, executive hyperactivation is unlikely to be simply an epiphenomenon associated with working memory performance, as hyperactivation was still seen in sensitivity analyses that controlled for both in-scanner and out-of-scanner cognitive performance. This result is consistent with several studies of executive function in major depression and anxiety disorders that have found overactivation of executive regions (1517). In contrast to the deficient ability to recruit the executive network seen in association with other symptom dimensions, these data suggest that mood and anxiety symptoms are associated with an inefficient executive system, where higher levels of executive network activation occur. It should be noted that participants in our sample who screened positive for mood and anxiety disorders frequently also had high levels of overall psychopathology. As these dimensions have directionally opposite associations with executive recruitment, the relative presence of comorbidity in a given sample could conceivably result in divergent results.

Limitations

Certain limitations of the present work should be noted. First, the young community-based sample studied here had diminished symptom severity compared with that found in typically older help-seeking samples drawn from clinical practices. Drawing from a different distribution of symptoms may result in reduced generalizability to clinical populations. However, the approach used here allowed us to accrue a larger, mainly unmedicated sample at a single site and scanner, and this approach is well suited to investigating broad dimensions of psychopathology. A second important limitation is that this cross-sectional report does not include longitudinal data. Our prior work in this data set has demonstrated that between-subject cross-sectional age effects are relatively subtle (24); within-subject longitudinal data will be particularly informative regarding whether specific symptom domains are associated with the development of executive deficits over time. Third, certain important classes of psychopathology, such as substance use, that have been shown to affect executive function were not considered in the present analysis (40). Fourth, despite prior research showing abnormalities of the default mode network in multiple psychiatric disorders, we did not find significant effects in the default mode network; this may be due to the task paradigm employed. Finally, as previously noted (26, 27), the highly structured screening format of GOASSESS may have resulted in high sensitivity but relatively diminished specificity, potentially leading to overestimation of the frequency of several disorders. Alternatively, high rates of endorsed symptoms may accurately reflect this particular sample. Although factor-analytic results of GOASSESS appear consistent with prior findings and lend support to the validity of the measure, ongoing longitudinal follow-up with semistructured assessments applying DSM criteria will allow further evaluation of the clinical relevance and stability of the present findings.

Conclusions

These data provide novel evidence regarding common and dissociable executive system deficits across multiple dimensions of psychopathology in young people. The results emphasize that executive dysfunction is present in association with overall psychopathology across traditional categorical psychiatric diagnoses, underscoring this system’s central relevance for circuit-based conceptualizations of neuropsychiatric disorders such as the National Institute of Mental Health’s Research Domain Criteria (21). These results may suggest that interventions seeking to enhance executive function may not fit well within the existing categorical diagnostic framework and may be beneficial to individuals across diverse clinical syndromes where executive deficits are present. Future research employing longitudinal designs may motivate targeted early interventions that seek to mitigate executive dysfunction in youths before negative outcomes accrue.

From the Departments of Psychiatry, of Biostatistics and Epidemiology, of Radiology, and of Pediatrics, Perelman School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia; the NIMH Mood and Anxiety Disorders Program, Bethesda, Md.; the Center for Applied Genomics, Children’s Hospital of Philadelphia; and the Philadelphia Veterans Administration Medical Center, Philadelphia.
Address correspondence to Dr. Satterthwaite ().

Presented at the 2015 annual meeting of the Society of Biological Psychiatry, Toronto, May 14–16, 2015.

Supported by NIMH RC2 grants MH089983, MH089924, and P50MH096891. Support for developing statistical analyses was provided by a seed grant from the Center for Biomedical Computing and Image Analysis at the University of Pennsylvania to Dr. Satterthwaite, Dr. Shinohara, and Mr. Vandekar. Additional support was provided by grants K23MH098130 and R01MH107703 to Dr. Satterthwaite, R01MH101111 to Dr. Wolf, K01MH102609 to Dr. Roalf, K08MH079364 to Dr. Calkins, R01NS085211 to Dr. Shinohara, T32MH065218 to Mr. Vandekar, and by the Dowshen Program for Neuroscience.

Dr. Shinohara has received consulting fees from Hoffmann-La Roche. Dr. Ruben C. Gur has received royalties from the Brain Resource Centre. Dr. Raquel E. Gur has served on an advisory board for Otsuka. All other authors report no financial relationships with commercial interests.

The authors thank the acquisition and recruitment team: Karthik Prabhakaran, Jeff Valdez, Raphael Gerraty, Marisa Riley, Jack Keefe, Elliott Yodh, Nicholas DeLeo, and Rosetta Chiavacci. The authors also thank Frank Mentch for data management.

References

1 Barkley RA: Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychol Bull 1997; 121:65–94Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

2 Sergeant JA, Geurts H, Oosterlaan J: How specific is a deficit of executive functioning for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder? Behav Brain Res 2002; 130:3–28Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

3 Fusar-Poli P, Deste G, Smieskova R, et al.: Cognitive functioning in prodromal psychosis: a meta-analysis. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2012; 69:562–571Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

4 Forbes NF, Carrick LA, McIntosh AM, et al.: Working memory in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Psychol Med 2009; 39:889–905Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

5 Green MF: What are the functional consequences of neurocognitive deficits in schizophrenia? Am J Psychiatry 1996; 153:321–330LinkGoogle Scholar

6 Klassen AF, Miller A, Fine S: Health-related quality of life in children and adolescents who have a diagnosis of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Pediatrics 2004; 114:e541–e547Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

7 Tolman AW, Kurtz MM: Neurocognitive predictors of objective and subjective quality of life in individuals with schizophrenia: a meta-analytic investigation. Schizophr Bull 2012; 38:304–315Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

8 Wehmeier PM, Schacht A, Barkley RA: Social and emotional impairment in children and adolescents with ADHD and the impact on quality of life. J Adolesc Health 2010; 46:209–217Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

9 Hart H, Radua J, Nakao T, et al.: Meta-analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging studies of inhibition and attention in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: exploring task-specific, stimulant medication, and age effects. JAMA Psychiatry 2013; 70:185–198Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

10 Cortese S, Kelly C, Chabernaud C, et al.: Toward systems neuroscience of ADHD: a meta-analysis of 55 fMRI studies. Am J Psychiatry 2012; 169:1038–1055LinkGoogle Scholar

11 Weinberger DR, Berman KF, Zec RF: Physiologic dysfunction of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia. I. Regional cerebral blood flow evidence. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1986; 43:114–124Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

12 Barch DM, Carter CS, Braver TS, et al.: Selective deficits in prefrontal cortex function in medication-naive patients with schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2001; 58:280–288Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

13 Minzenberg MJ, Laird AR, Thelen S, et al.: Meta-analysis of 41 functional neuroimaging studies of executive function in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2009; 66:811–822Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

14 Rubia K, Smith AB, Halari R, et al.: Disorder-specific dissociation of orbitofrontal dysfunction in boys with pure conduct disorder during reward and ventrolateral prefrontal dysfunction in boys with pure ADHD during sustained attention. Am J Psychiatry 2009; 166:83–94LinkGoogle Scholar

15 Fales CL, Barch DM, Burgess GC, et al.: Anxiety and cognitive efficiency: differential modulation of transient and sustained neural activity during a working memory task. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 2008; 8:239–253Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

16 Matsuo K, Glahn DC, Peluso MAM, et al.: Prefrontal hyperactivation during working memory task in untreated individuals with major depressive disorder. Mol Psychiatry 2007; 12:158–166Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

17 Schöning S, Zwitserlood P, Engelien A, et al.: Working-memory fMRI reveals cingulate hyperactivation in euthymic major depression. Hum Brain Mapp 2009; 30:2746–2756Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

18 Callicott JH, Bertolino A, Mattay VS, et al.: Physiological dysfunction of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia revisited. Cereb Cortex 2000; 10:1078–1092Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

19 Merikangas KR, He J-P, Burstein M, et al.: Lifetime prevalence of mental disorders in U.S. adolescents: results from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication–Adolescent Supplement (NCS-A). J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2010; 49:980–989Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

20 Rubia K: “Cool” inferior frontostriatal dysfunction in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder versus “hot” ventromedial orbitofrontal-limbic dysfunction in conduct disorder: a review. Biol Psychiatry 2011; 69:e69–e87Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

21 Insel T, Cuthbert B, Garvey M, et al.: Research domain criteria (RDoC): toward a new classification framework for research on mental disorders. Am J Psychiatry 2010; 167:748–751LinkGoogle Scholar

22 Satterthwaite TD, Elliott MA, Ruparel K, et al.: Neuroimaging of the Philadelphia neurodevelopmental cohort. Neuroimage 2014; 86:544–553Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

23 Satterthwaite TD, Connolly JJ, Ruparel K, et al.: The Philadelphia Neurodevelopmental Cohort: a publicly available resource for the study of normal and abnormal brain development in youth. Neuroimage 2016; 124(Pt B):1115–1119CrossrefGoogle Scholar

24 Satterthwaite TD, Wolf DH, Erus G, et al.: Functional maturation of the executive system during adolescence. J Neurosci 2013; 33:16249–16261Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

25 Wolf DH, Satterthwaite TD, Calkins ME, et al.: Functional neuroimaging abnormalities in youth with psychosis spectrum symptoms. JAMA Psychiatry 2015; 72:456–465Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

26 Calkins ME, Moore TM, Merikangas KR, et al.: The psychosis spectrum in a young U.S. community sample: findings from the Philadelphia Neurodevelopmental Cohort. World Psychiatry 2014; 13:296–305Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

27 Calkins ME, Merikangas KR, Moore TM, et al.: The Philadelphia Neurodevelopmental Cohort: constructing a deep phenotyping collaborative. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2015; 56:1356–1369Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

28 Reise SP: Invited paper: the rediscovery of bifactor measurement models. Multivariate Behav Res 2012; 47:667–696Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

29 Ragland JD, Turetsky BI, Gur RC, et al.: Working memory for complex figures: an fMRI comparison of letter and fractal n-back tasks. Neuropsychology 2002; 16:370–379Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

30 Jenkinson M, Beckmann CF, Behrens TE, et al.: FSL. Neuroimage 2012; 62:782–790Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

31 Avants BB, Epstein CL, Grossman M, et al.: Symmetric diffeomorphic image registration with cross-correlation: evaluating automated labeling of elderly and neurodegenerative brain. Med Image Anal 2008; 12:26–41Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

32 Shehzad Z, Kelly C, Reiss PT, et al.: A multivariate distance-based analytic framework for connectome-wide association studies. Neuroimage 2014; 93:74–94Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

33 Oksanen J, Guillaume Blanchet F, Kindt R, et al: Package ‘vegan’: community ecology package, version 2.3-2. 2015; https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/vegan/vegan.pdfGoogle Scholar

34 Cox RW: AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. Comput Biomed Res 1996; 29:162–173Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

35 Power JD, Cohen AL, Nelson SM, et al.: Functional network organization of the human brain. Neuron 2011; 72:665–678Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

36 Dosenbach NU, Fair DA, Miezin FM, et al.: Distinct brain networks for adaptive and stable task control in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2007; 104:11073–11078Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

37 Salvadore G, Cornwell BR, Sambataro F, et al.: Anterior cingulate desynchronization and functional connectivity with the amygdala during a working memory task predict rapid antidepressant response to ketamine. Neuropsychopharmacology 2010; 35:1415–1422Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

38 Goodkind M, Eickhoff SB, Oathes DJ, et al.: Identification of a common neurobiological substrate for mental illness. JAMA Psychiatry 2015; 72:305–315Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

39 Baker JT, Holmes AJ, Masters GA, et al.: Disruption of cortical association networks in schizophrenia and psychotic bipolar disorder. JAMA Psychiatry 2014; 71:109–118Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

40 Whelan R, Watts R, Orr CA, et al.: Neuropsychosocial profiles of current and future adolescent alcohol misusers. Nature 2014; 512:185–189Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar