The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×
New ResearchFull Access

Impact of Neurocognition on Social and Role Functioning in Individuals at Clinical High Risk for Psychosis

Abstract

Objective:

Cognitive deficits have been well documented in schizophrenia and have been shown to impair quality of life and to compromise everyday functioning. Recent studies of adolescents and young adults at high risk for developing psychosis show that neurocognitive impairments are detectable before the onset of psychotic symptoms. However, it remains unclear how cognitive impairments affect functioning before the onset of psychosis. The authors assessed cognitive impairment in adolescents at clinical high risk for psychosis and examined its impact on social and role functioning.

Method:

A sample of 127 treatment-seeking patients at clinical high risk for psychosis and a group of 80 healthy comparison subjects were identified and recruited for research in the Recognition and Prevention Program. At baseline, participants were assessed with a comprehensive neurocognitive battery as well as measures of social and role functioning.

Results:

Relative to healthy comparison subjects, clinical high-risk patients showed significant impairments in the domains of processing speed, verbal memory, executive function, working memory, visuospatial processing, motor speed, sustained attention, and language. Clinical high-risk patients also displayed impaired social and role functioning at baseline. Among patients with attenuated positive symptoms, processing speed was related to social and role functioning at baseline.

Conclusions:

These findings demonstrate that cognitive and functional impairments are detectable in patients at clinical high risk for psychosis before the onset of psychotic illness and that processing speed appears to be an important cognitive predictor of poor functioning.

Schizophrenia is associated with multiple indicators of disease liability, including functional and structural brain abnormalities, sensory processing deficits, and neuromotor abnormalities. In addition, particularly pronounced impairments in cognition have been well documented and are thought to be a core feature of the illness (15). Assessed by various neuropsychological tests, cognitive impairments in schizophrenia are a large and pervasive facet of the illness (68), enduring through the lifespan (9). Not only do these impairments reduce a patient's ability to reason, plan, retain information, and problem-solve, but they also have been shown to impair quality of life and to compromise everyday functioning (10, 11) and are considered to be directly associated with the long-term disability that represents one of the major public health costs associated with schizophrenia. A primary question that remains to be answered, however, is: At what point in the illness do cognitive impairments begin to influence ability to function? While evidence clearly indicates that cognitive deficits in patients with chronic illness are directly related to functional outcome, independently of positive symptoms (10, 11), the interaction between these two domains before the onset of illness is unknown. The issue is whether cognition affects functioning before the onset of psychosis or whether this association is an outcome of illness. Such developmental information is becoming increasingly important for designing effective early treatments to improve functional outcome.

Studies of adolescents and young adults at high risk for developing psychosis have demonstrated that neurocognitive impairments are detectable before the onset of psychotic symptoms. For example, in a preliminary Recognition and Prevention Program study, Lencz et al. (12) reported that individuals at clinical high risk for developing psychosis performed one standard deviation below the healthy comparison group mean on tests measuring verbal memory, executive function, language, and processing speed. Additionally, several recent studies investigating neurocognitive function in patients at high risk for psychosis reported impairments in verbal memory/learning, executive function, and processing speed (1315). While these studies involved a wide range of assessment methods, and although reports of specific neurocognitive impairments have not been wholly consistent, collectively the findings support the idea that cognitive abnormalities are detectable well before the onset of psychosis.

Preliminary evidence also supports the view that in addition to cognitive impairments, functional impairments are also rooted early in development. Pre-illness social problems and school difficulties have been consistently reported in studies of those at genetic high risk and in cohort studies. In addition, two prospective studies of patients at clinical high risk for psychosis (16, 17) indicated that, relative to healthy comparison subjects, patients at high risk have significant impairments in maintaining social/interpersonal relationships and managing academic and work tasks, comparable to individuals with first-episode and multi-episode schizophrenia (16). These findings are consistent with the increasing emphasis on functional decline as a critically important outcome that parallels conversion to psychosis and with the growing notion that psychosis and long-term functional disability are equally important targets for prevention.

As noted, the relationship between neurocognitive and functional impairments before the onset of psychosis is unknown. To date, only one study has examined this relationship in adolescents at high risk for psychosis, and it reported a positive relationship between neurocognition and functional impairments (18). The findings are difficult to generalize, however, given the small sample size and lack of a healthy comparison group. In this study, we focused especially on the relationship between neurocognition and early functional behavior before the emergence of psychosis. We examined a large sample of treatment-seeking adolescents and young adults at clinical high risk for psychosis. A healthy adolescent group was used as a comparison group to determine the degree to which patients at high risk deviate from normal. A broad neuropsychological battery representing multiple cognitive domains was administered to all participants. In addition, social and role (academic or occupational) functioning was measured with the Global Functioning: Social and Role scales (17), developed specifically for use with adolescents and young adults at clinical high risk for developing psychosis. We addressed the following questions: What is the extent of cognitive and functional impairment before the onset of psychosis? How does impaired cognition affect and contribute to poor functioning in adolescents at clinical high risk for psychosis? How do positive symptoms combine with neurocognition in explaining functioning?

Method

Study Participants

We enrolled 127 patients who met criteria for “clinical high risk, positive,” derived from the Scale of Prodromal Syndromes (19, 20). Inclusion criteria were based on the presence of one or more attenuated positive symptoms rated as moderate, moderately severe, or severe (scores of 3, 4, or 5 on a scale of 0–6) on the Scale of Prodromal Syndromes without reaching the level of psychosis. Positive symptoms included unusual thought content, suspiciousness, grandiosity, perceptual abnormalities, and disorganized communication. A score of 6 (severe and psychotic) on any item was an exclusion criterion. Participants in the clinical high-risk group are broadly comparable to those considered to have prodromal symptoms in most other studies. Participants were referred to the Recognition and Prevention Program by affiliated outpatient and inpatient psychiatry departments, local mental health providers, and school psychologists or counselors or were self-referred.

We enrolled 80 healthy volunteers as comparison subjects. Healthy comparison subjects were recruited through announcements in local newspapers and within the medical center during the same period as the patients.

All participants had to be English-speaking and between the ages of 12 and 22 years. Exclusion criteria for all participants included any of the following: a schizophrenia spectrum diagnosis, such as schizophrenia, schizoaffective disorder, schizophreniform disorder, or delusional disorder, as assessed by the semi-structured interview of the Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children, Epidemiologic Version (21); a medical or neurological disorder that could affect brain functioning; and estimated IQ below 70. Healthy comparison subjects were excluded if they had a first-degree relative with a diagnosed axis I psychotic disorder.

Written informed consent was obtained from patients 18 years of age or older and from parents of patients under 18, with written assent from the patient. The research protocol was approved by the Institutional Review Board at North Shore-Long Island Jewish Health System. The data reported here were collected as part of the larger Recognition and Prevention Program, an ongoing longitudinal investigation initiated in 1998 and funded by the National Institute of Mental Health in 2000. In this article, we report baseline data on all participants who completed the phase 1 neuropsychological test battery (January 1998–February 2008) and were rated on the two global functioning rating scales.

Baseline Neurocognitive Assessment

A comprehensive battery of tests, taking approximately 3.5 hours, was administered to all participants at study entry. Assessors were at the master's level or above and were trained in the administration and scoring of all tests. Estimated full-scale IQ scores were derived from the vocabulary and block design subscales of the WISC-III (22) for patients under age 16 and from the same subscales of the WAIS-R (23) for those age 16 and older. All participants received the reading subtest of the Wide-Range Achievement Test 3 (WRAT-3; 24), which would provide an estimate of premorbid intellectual levels in the patient group. In addition to the intelligence scales, the battery included neuropsychological tests that assessed eight cognitive domains: processing speed, verbal memory, executive function, working memory, visuospatial processing, motor speed, sustained attention, and language. Domain construction was based on several factors: rational criteria derived from the literature on clinical neuroscience and neuropsychology; previous work by our group and others that demonstrated the content validity of the domains (25); and findings of separable factors in the schizophrenia cognitive architecture, including processing speed (26, 27). As shown in Table 1, internal reliability (Cronbach's alpha) for these domains was good, thus minimizing the possibility of identifying spurious differences across domains (28, 29).

TABLE 1. Neurocognitive Domains, Individual Tests, and Dependent Measures Used in a Study of the Effect of Neurocognition on Social and Role Functioning in Individuals at Clinical High Risk for Psychosis

Domain, Tests, and Cronbach's AlphaDependent Measure
Verbal memory (α=0.83)
California Verbal Learning Test
    Total for trials 1–5Words recalled in trials 1–5
    Long delay free recallRecognition errors
Wechsler Memory Scale, Revised
    Logical memory subtest, immediate and delayedStory elements recalled
Working memory (α=0.77)
WISC-III or WAIS-R
    Digit span task, forward and backwardDigit sequences recalled
    Letter-number span taskNumber of correct trials
Executive function (α=0.76)
Ruff Figural Fluency TestNumber of correctly reproduced figures
Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, Version 2
    Perseverative errorsPercentage of perseverative errors
    Categories completedNumber of correctly completed categories
    Conceptual level responsesNumber of consecutive correct responses in at least three runs
    Controlled oral word association testWords produced in 1 minute
Sustained attention (α=0.84)
Continuous Performance Test, Identical Pairs Version, 2-, 3-, and 4-digitd′ (for all stimulus sets)
Processing speed (α=0.80)
Trail Making Test, Parts A and BTime to complete trails
WISC-III/WAIS-R, digit symbol-coding subtestSymbols accurately coded in 2 minutes
Motor speed (α=0.75)
Finger tapping test, dominant and nondominant hand scoresTaps in 10 seconds, over five blocks
Grooved Pegboard Test, dominant and nondominant hand scoresTime to place pegs
Visuospatial processing (α=0.60)
Judgment of Line Orientation TestLines accurately matched
WAIS-R/WISC-III, block design testCorrectly reconstruct blocks to match patterns on cards
Language (α=0.80)
Wide-Range Achievement Test 3, reading subtestTotal score for words read correctly
Boston Naming TestNumber of correctly named items
WAIS-R/WISC-III, vocabulary subtestNumber of words orally defined

TABLE 1. Neurocognitive Domains, Individual Tests, and Dependent Measures Used in a Study of the Effect of Neurocognition on Social and Role Functioning in Individuals at Clinical High Risk for Psychosis

Enlarge table

Baseline Clinical Assessment

Social and role functioning was assessed using the Global Functioning: Social and Role scales (17). These scales, which provide clinician-rated single overall scores, are similar in scope and design to the Global Assessment of Functioning Scale and the Social and Occupational Functioning Assessment Scale (30). However, the new Global Functioning scales differ substantially from both of these in that they represent parallel (one targeting social, the other role) well-anchored scales that take age and phase of illness into account, enabling social and role functioning to be studied as independent domains not confounded by clinical symptoms. The Social scale assesses quantity and quality of peer relationships, level of peer conflict, age-appropriate intimate relationships, and involvement with family members. The Role scale rates level of performance in primary role: school, work, or homemaker. For both scales, scores range from 1 to 10, with 1 indicating extreme dysfunction and 10 indicating superior functioning.

Statistical Analysis

All analyses were conducted using SPSS, version 16.0 (SPSS Inc., Chicago). Before domain scores were computed, raw test scores were transformed into standard z scores using the age-stratified means and standard deviations of the healthy comparison subjects to control for age-related change in cognitive performance. When applicable, tests were reverse-scored, so that lower scores always reflected worse performance. Domain scores were then computed by averaging each participant's z scores on tests assessing the same neurocognitive domain (Table 1); z scores for each domain were then restandardized using the mean and standard deviation of the domain scores of the healthy comparison group. A composite of global neurocognitive performance was calculated by creating a mean of the eight domain scores. The numbers of patients contributing to the analyses of the various tests varied slightly because of missing data.

To examine whether patients at clinical high risk for developing psychosis have different neurocognitive test profiles as compared to healthy comparison subjects, the eight neurocognitive domain scores were used as dependent variables in a multivariate analysis of covariance (MANCOVA) with group as a between-subject factor and neurocognitive domain scores as within-subject factors. MANCOVA was used to assess the effects of gender and race since the two groups differed on these variables. Deviations from flatness in the clinical high-risk cognitive profile, if suggested by a significant main effect of overall cognitive performance and a significant interaction between cognitive performance and group, were assessed by contrasting the mean for each individual domain with the mean of all other domains, using paired t tests. This procedure allowed us to identify possible specific impairments relative to each of the other domains, rather than a generalized impairment affecting all cognitive domains in the clinical high-risk group, psychometric characteristics of the tests notwithstanding.

Next, we examined neurocognitive performance independent of performance in other cognitive domains by using analysis of covariance (ANCOVA). Individual ANCOVAs were used to examine differences in neurocognitive performance by domain, with group as a between-subject factor, neurocognitive domain scores as dependent variables, and gender and race as covariates. The strength of the variables examined by ANCOVA was evaluated using Cohen's f statistic (31).

Two sets of multiple linear regressions with forward (stepwise) inclusion were constructed to predict the independent contributions of neurocognitive performance to functional impairment in patients at clinical high risk for psychosis. The first set of regression models examined the association between global neurocognition and functioning, with the global neurocomposite score as an independent variable and the Global Functioning: Social and Role scores as dependent variables. After accounting for global neurocognition, the second set of regression models examined the relationship between domain-specific neurocognitive performance and functional impairment, with neurocognitive domain scores as independent variables and social and role scores as dependent variables, controlling for gender and race. Follow-up stepwise linear regressions were constructed to determine the relative contributions of neurocognitive performance and positive symptom severity in explaining the variance in social and role functioning.

Logistic regression analysis was used to test whether neurocognition could be used to accurately determine functional status at study entry. Social and role scores were transformed into dichotomous variables in which poor functioning was labeled 1 and good functioning was labeled 0 (scores ≤6 were rated as poor functioning and scores >6 as good functioning) (32). One set of binomial logistic regression models with forward (stepwise, likelihood ratio method) inclusion was performed with the neurocognitive domain scores of both groups as independent variables and baseline functional status (social and role) as dependent variables, controlling for gender and race.

A final logistic regression model was constructed to examine whether neurocognition and functional status could accurately determine participant group membership at study entry. A binomial logistic regression was performed with neurocognitive domain scores and functional status of both participant groups as independent variables and group as dependent variable, controlling for gender and race.

The logistic regression models contained only significant (p<0.05) domain-specific neurocognitive predictors of social and role functioning. Model calibration was assessed with the Hosmer-Lemeshow (33) goodness of fit test (p≥0.10). Pseudo R2 was estimated using the Nagelkerke statistic (an approximate measure of the proportion of explained variation in the logistic model) (34). The discriminative ability of the regression models was evaluated by the area under the receiver operator characteristic curve (35) (for more details, see the data supplement that accompanies the online edition of this article).

Results

Table 2 summarizes the baseline demographic and clinical characteristics of the study participants. The healthy comparison and high-risk groups did not differ significantly on age at testing, WRAT-3 reading score, years of education, parental socioeconomic status, handedness, or ethnicity; however, the groups differed significantly on estimated current IQ, race, and gender ratio. The healthy comparison group had a higher mean estimated current IQ and a lower proportion of white males compared with the clinical high-risk group.

TABLE 2. Baseline Demographic and Clinical Characteristics of Participants in a Study of the Effect of Neurocognition on Social and Role Functioning in Individuals at Clinical High Risk for Psychosis

CharacteristicHealthy Comparison Group (N=80)Clinical High-Risk Group (N=127)Analysis
MeanSDMeanSDtdfp
Age (years)16.002.7716.092.060.241340.81
Wide-Range Achievement Test 3, reading subtest, standard score108.4610.36105.4610.291.752020.08
Estimated current IQ108.6213.84102.1415.892.93197<0.01
Education (years)10.182.779.902.080.771320.44
Global Functioning: Social scale8.630.975.941.5315.41203<0.001
Global Functioning: Role scale8.681.005.462.1814.13192<0.001
Structured Interview for Prodromal Syndromes
    Positive symptom score0.410.928.823.8123.24157<0.001
    Negative symptom score1.141.5611.825.4319.29147<0.001
N%N%χ2dfp
Low parental socioeconomic statusa911.886.51.7610.19
Gender9.721<0.01
    Female4455.04233.1
    Male3645.08566.9
Right-handed7188.811187.40.7240.72
Race8.423<0.05
    White4758.89877.2
    African American1215.01310.2
    Asian1316.2107.9
    Other810.064.7
Hispanic ethnicity67.51713.48.7440.07

a Hollingshead index (36), where 1–3=high and 4–5=low.

TABLE 2. Baseline Demographic and Clinical Characteristics of Participants in a Study of the Effect of Neurocognition on Social and Role Functioning in Individuals at Clinical High Risk for Psychosis

Enlarge table

The clinical high-risk group demonstrated significant impairments in social (Cohen's d=2.01) and role (Cohen's d=1.78) functioning relative to the healthy comparison group (Table 2). Social functioning of the clinical high-risk patients ranged from good to an inability to function socially, and role functioning ranged from good to extreme dysfunction. Although significantly correlated with each other, the social and role measures appear to be independent constructs sharing some method variance (healthy comparison group: r=0.37, p<0.001; clinical high-risk group: r=0.29, p<0.001).

At the time of testing, a majority of the clinical high-risk patients (55.9%) were not receiving any medication. The remaining patients (44.1%) were receiving atypical antipsychotics, antidepressants, mood stabilizers, stimulants, and/or anxiolytics. There were no significant effects of medication type or status (medication naive or treated) for clinical high-risk patients on any of the demographic characteristics or neurocognitive domains reported here.

Cognitive Profiles

Figure 1 illustrates the mean performance across the eight neurocognitive domains for the clinical high-risk group relative to the healthy comparison group. The results of the MANCOVA indicated a significant main effect for neurocognitive performance (Wilks's lambda=0.88; F=3.73, df=7, 186, p<0.01). Gender was a significant covariate (Wilks's lambda=0.86; F=4.19, df=7, 186, p<0.001), as female patients exhibited poorer global neurocognitive performance than male patients. MANCOVA also revealed that the neurocognitive profile of the clinical high-risk group deviated significantly from flatness, as indicated by the significant interaction between group and neurocognitive performance (Wilks's lambda=0.88; F=3.47, df=7, 186, p<0.01). The clinical high-risk group showed a global neurocognitive impairment (mean=–0.63, SD=0.99) as compared to the healthy comparison group (mean=0.00, SD=0.61). Post hoc paired t tests in the clinical high-risk group indicated that processing speed (t=4.31, df=126, p<0.001) and verbal memory (t=4.91, df=126, p<0.001) were significantly more impaired relative to the mean of all the other neurocognitive domains.

FIGURE 1.

FIGURE 1. Mean Performance on Eight Neurocognitive Domains, Relative to Healthy Comparison Subjects, Among Patients at Clinical High Risk for Developing Psychosisa

a The healthy comparison group mean was set to 0 and the standard deviation to 1. Error bars indicate standard errors of the mean.

*p<0.05. **p<0.01. ***p<0.001.

Table 3 summarizes the individual univariate ANCOVAs for each cognitive domain. Individual ANCOVAs revealed that, relative to the healthy comparison group, the clinical high-risk group was significantly impaired in all eight cognitive domains. There was a significant effect of gender on attention (F=6.00, df=1, 193, p<0.05), visuospatial processing (F=22.04, df=1, 203, p<0.001), and language (F=4.89, df=1, 202, p<0.05), as female patients exhibited poorer neurocognitive performance than male patients in these domains.

TABLE 3. Neurocognitive Performance in Patients at Clinical High Risk for Developing Psychosis (N=127)

Analysis
DomainMeanSDFdfCohen's fap
Processing speed–1.232.0566.771, 2030.37<0.001
Verbal memory–1.021.3230.671, 2030.43<0.001
Executive function–0.701.919.291, 2030.22<0.010
Working memory–0.581.1718.331, 2030.27<0.001
Visuospatial processing–0.461.3115.001, 2030.20<0.001
Motor speed–0.321.365.681, 2020.13<0.050
Sustained attention–0.321.046.951, 1930.16<0.010
Language–0.351.216.161, 2020.16<0.050

a Indicates a measure of effect size that provides an estimate of the proportion of variance explained for the sample and can be interpreted using the following categories (30): small=0.10, medium=0.25, large=0.40.

TABLE 3. Neurocognitive Performance in Patients at Clinical High Risk for Developing Psychosis (N=127)

Enlarge table

Relationships Between Cognition and Social and Role Functioning

As shown in Table 4, global neurocognition was a significant predictor of social and role functioning at baseline. The global neurocomposite score accounted for 8% and 5% of the variance for social and role functioning, respectively. Among the domain-specific neurocognitive scores, processing speed was a significant predictor of social and role functioning at baseline. Processing speed predicted 10% and 7% of the variance for social and role functioning, respectively. This relationship was independent of positive symptoms, as follow-up regression analyses indicated no significant relationships between total attenuated positive symptoms and either social or role functioning.

TABLE 4. Linear Regression Results for the Effects of Neurocognitive Performance on Social and Role Functioning at Baseline in Individuals at Clinical High Risk for Psychosis

Social Functioning
Role Functioning
VariableBSEβpΔR2ΔFBSEβpΔR2ΔF
Global neurocognitive performance
    Racea0.880.30.24<0.010.079.09
    Global neurocomposite0.420.130.27<0.0010.0810.780.490.190.22<0.050.056.31
Domain-specific neurocognitive performance
    Racea0.880.310.24<0.010.078.58
    Processing speed0.230.060.31<0.0010.1011.490.280.100.26<0.010.078.73

aCoded as 0 (nonwhite) or 1 (white).

TABLE 4. Linear Regression Results for the Effects of Neurocognitive Performance on Social and Role Functioning at Baseline in Individuals at Clinical High Risk for Psychosis

Enlarge table

Given that processing speed was significantly associated with social and role functioning, additional analyses were performed to determine whether processing speed scores could accurately determine functional status when the latter was dichotomized as good or poor functioning. Of the 127 clinical high-risk patients, 83 (65.4%) were in the poor social functioning group and 85 (66.9%) were in the poor role functioning group. Of the 80 healthy comparison subjects, 74 (94.9%) were in the good social functioning group and 73 (93.6%) were in the good role functioning group. The final logistic regression model indicated that participants with higher processing speed scores had a significantly lower likelihood of poor social functioning (odds ratio=0.60, 95% confidence interval [CI]=0.48–0.76; Wald's χ2=18.57, df=1, p<0.001) and poor role functioning (odds ratio=0.62, 95% CI=0.50–0.78; df=1, p<0.001) after controlling for gender and race. The final models accounted for 19% and 18% of the pseudovariance (Nagelkerke's R2) for social and role functioning, respectively. Both models were well calibrated according to the Hosmer-Lemeshow test.

Logistic regression was performed to determine whether neurocognition and functioning could discriminate between clinical high-risk patients and healthy comparison subjects. Processing speed (odds ratio=0.56, 95% CI=0.36–0.87; Wald's χ2=6.72, df=1, p<0.01), social functional status (odds ratio=34.51, 95% CI=8.98–132.70; Wald's χ2=26.56, df=1, p<0.001), and role functional status (odds ratio=20.88, 95% CI=6.58–66.27; Wald's χ2=26.60, df=1, p<0.001) were significant predictors of group membership. The final model was well calibrated and accounted for 71% of the pseudovariance. Participants with impaired processing speed and poor functioning were more likely in the clinical high-risk group.

Discussion

To assess the relationship between cognitive and functional impairments before the onset of psychosis, we examined the baseline neurocognitive performance and functioning of a large group of patients at clinical high risk for developing psychosis. The patients in our study displayed significant neurocognitive impairments, particularly in the domains of processing speed, verbal memory, executive function, and working memory. They also had significant functional impairments, confirming that social and role impairments are present before the onset of psychosis. Our results showed that these functional impairments were related to neurocognitive performance, independent of positive symptoms. Specifically, we found that processing speed was significantly related to social and role functioning, indicating that the relationship between neurocognition and functioning exists before the onset of psychosis and is not an outcome of chronic illness.

Our findings highlight the central role of impaired processing speed in the pathogenesis of schizophrenia and its morbidity. This result is consistent with a number of recent studies that have reported processing speed impairments to be a hallmark feature of the cognitive deficit in patients with schizophrenia (37) and to be associated with poor functioning (38). Our results in this study extend this notion to the early stages of the illness and suggest that speed of processing may be particularly important in the development of functional disability. It can be speculated from our findings that processing speed impairments are a rate-limiting factor for developing good social and role functioning during a stage in life when social and occupational skills are rapidly built. Slowing in processing, understanding, and reacting to incoming information is likely to be debilitating in multiple domains of real-world functioning (37).

In addition to highlighting the impact of cognitive impairments on functioning before the onset of psychosis, these findings provide further support for the core role of cognition in the process leading to psychosis and introduce functional deficits as part of the core disorder. Clinical high-risk patients showed marked neurocognitive impairments across multiple domains, with large impairments seen in processing speed and verbal memory. Impairments in these domains have been well documented in studies of schizophrenia (2, 39), including in first-episode patients (25, 40) as well as nonaffected first-degree relatives (7, 41). Our findings confirm that the cognitive impairment associated with schizophrenia is present and detectable before the onset of psychosis and are consistent with an emerging view that cognition is a critical dimension in individuals at high risk for psychosis.

To our knowledge, this is the first study to examine the relationship between neurocognitive performance and functioning in a large sample of patients at clinical high risk for psychosis and healthy comparison subjects. In a previous study of 45 patients at high risk for psychosis (18), verbal learning and memory were found to be moderately correlated with social functioning, and reasoning and problem solving were weakly correlated with global functioning. That study, unlike ours, did not find a relationship between functioning and processing speed. Our findings on this relationship may be attributable to our use of the Global Functioning: Social and Role scales to assess functioning; these scales have several advantages over established functional measures currently used throughout the field because they were designed to capture subtle difficulties before the onset of full-blown psychosis, cover the age range typical of the prodromal phase, and disentangle the social and role functioning domains from one another (17).

Traditionally, prevention of psychosis in clinical high-risk patients has largely focused on the reduction or delay of positive symptoms. A more comprehensive prevention approach should attempt to target the functional impairments that are also present before the onset of psychosis. Therefore, there is a growing need to understand the factors that contribute to poor functioning in individuals at high risk. While impaired cognition serves as an important risk factor, it is not the only explanation for poor functioning. It is possible that other factors, such as independently reduced functional capacity, social stigma, and lack of social support and services, further contribute to the development of functional impairment between adolescence and adulthood. Further research is needed to determine how these factors can be addressed to prevent long-term functional disability.

From the Department of Psychiatry Research, Zucker Hillside Hospital, North Shore-Long Island Jewish Health System; and the Department of Psychiatry, Albert Einstein College of Medicine, Bronx, N.Y.
Address correspondence and reprint requests to Dr. Carrión,
Zucker Hillside Hospital, Psychiatry Research, 75-59 263rd St., Glen Oaks, NY 11004
; (e-mail).

Received Aug. 26, 2010; revision received Jan. 6, 2011; accepted Feb. 14, 2011.

Dr. Goldberg has received consulting fees from Merck and GlaxoSmithKline and receives royalties for use of the Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia in clinical trials. Dr. Correll has been a consultant or adviser to or received grant support or honoraria from Actelion, AstraZeneca, Boehringer-Ingelheim, Bristol-Myers Squibb, Cephalon, Eli Lilly, Feinstein Institute for Medical Research, IntraCellular Therapies, GlaxoSmithKline, Hoffmann-La Roche, Lundbeck, Medicure, Merck, NARSAD, NIMH, Ortho-McNeill/Janssen/Johnson & Johnson, Otsuka, Pfizer, Schering-Plough, Sepracor/Sunovion, Supernus, Takeda, and Vanda. Dr. Cornblatt was the original developer of the Continuous Performance Test, Identical Pairs Version, and has been an adviser to Merck and Bristol-Myers Squibb. The other authors report no financial relationships with commercial interests.

Supported by NIMH grants MH61523-02 and MH61523-8 (to Dr. Cornblatt) and the Zucker Hillside Hospital NIMH Advanced Center for Intervention and Services Research for the Study of Schizophrenia grant MH 074543-01 (to John M. Kane, M.D.).

The authors thank the study participants and the staff of the Recognition and Prevention Program for their time and effort from the very outset of these studies. In particular, the authors thank Pradeep Nagachandran, M.D., and Ruth Olsen, B.S., for their assistance in carrying out this study.

References

1. Bowie CR , Reichenberg A , Patterson TL , Heaton RK , Harvey PD: Determinants of real-world functional performance in schizophrenia subjects: correlations with cognition, functional capacity, and symptoms. Am J Psychiatry 2006; 163:418–425LinkGoogle Scholar

2. Elvevag B , Goldberg TE: Cognitive impairment in schizophrenia is the core of the disorder. Crit Rev Neurobiol 2000; 14:1–21Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

3. Gold JM: Cognitive deficits as treatment targets in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 2004; 72:21–28Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

4. Green MF , Kern RS , Heaton RK: Longitudinal studies of cognition and functional outcome in schizophrenia: implications for MATRICS. Schizophr Res 2004; 72:41–51Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

5. Weickert TW , Goldberg TE , Gold JM , Bigelow LB , Egan MF , Weinberger DR: Cognitive impairments in patients with schizophrenia displaying preserved and compromised intellect. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2000; 57:907–913Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

6. Allen AJ , Griss ME , Folley BS , Hawkins KA , Pearlson GD: Endophenotypes in schizophrenia: a selective review. Schizophr Res 2009; 109:24–37Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

7. Sitskoorn MM , Aleman A , Ebisch SJ , Appels MC , Kahn RS: Cognitive deficits in relatives of patients with schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Schizophr Res 2004; 71:285–295Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

8. Snitz BE , Macdonald AW , Carter CS: Cognitive deficits in unaffected first-degree relatives of schizophrenia patients: a meta-analytic review of putative endophenotypes. Schizophr Bull 2006; 32:179–194Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

9. Hyde TM , Nawroz S , Goldberg TE , Bigelow LB , Strong D , Ostrem JL , Weinberger DR , Kleinman JE: Is there cognitive decline in schizophrenia? a cross-sectional study. Br J Psychiatry 1994; 164:494–500Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

10. Green MF: What are the functional consequences of neurocognitive deficits in schizophrenia? Am J Psychiatry 1996; 153:321–330LinkGoogle Scholar

11. Green MF , Kern RS , Braff DL , Mintz J: Neurocognitive deficits and functional outcome in schizophrenia: are we measuring the “right stuff”? Schizophr Bull 2000; 26:119–136Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

12. Lencz T , Smith CW , McLaughlin D , Auther A , Nakayama E , Hovey L , Cornblatt BA: Generalized and specific neurocognitive deficits in prodromal schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 2006; 59:863–871Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

13. Frommann I , Pukrop R , Brinkmeyer J , Bechdolf A , Ruhrmann S , Berning J , Decker P , Riedel M , Moller HJ , Wolwer W , Gaebel W , Klosterkotter J , Maier W , Wagner M: Neuropsychological profiles in different at-risk states of psychosis: executive control impairment in the early-and additional memory dysfunction in the late-prodromal state. Schizophr Bull(Epub ahead of print, Jan 6, 2010 )MedlineGoogle Scholar

14. Jahshan C , Heaton RK , Golshan S , Cadenhead KS: Course of neurocognitive deficits in the prodrome and first episode of schizophrenia. Neuropsychology 2010; 24:109–120Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

15. Pflueger MO , Gschwandtner U , Stieglitz RD , Riecher-Rossler A: Neuropsychological deficits in individuals with an at risk mental state for psychosis: working memory as a potential trait marker. Schizophr Res 2007; 97:14–24Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

16. Addington J , Penn D , Woods SW , Addington D , Perkins DO: Social functioning in individuals at clinical high-risk for psychosis. Schizophr Res 2008; 99:119–124Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

17. Cornblatt BA , Auther AM , Niendam T , Smith CW , Zinberg J , Bearden CE , Cannon TD: Preliminary findings for two new measures of social and role functioning in the prodromal phase of schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 2007; 33:688–702Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

18. Niendam TA , Bearden CE , Johnson JK , McKinley M , Loewy R , O'Brien M , Nuechterlein KH , Green MF , Cannon TD: Neurocognitive performance and functional disability in the psychosis prodrome. Schizophr Res 2006; 84:100–111Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

19. Miller TJ , McGlashan TH , Rosen JL , Cadenhead K , Cannon T , Ventura J , McFarlane W , Perkins DO , Pearlson GD , Woods SW: Prodromal assessment with the Structured Interview for Prodromal Syndromes and the Scale of Prodromal Symptoms: predictive validity, interrater reliability, and training to reliability. Schizophr Bull 2003; 29:703–715Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

20. Miller TJ , McGlashan TH , Woods SW , Stein K , Driesen N , Corcoran CM , Hoffman R , Davidson L: Symptom assessment in schizophrenic prodromal states. Psychiatr Q 1999; 70:273–287Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

21. Orvaschel H , Puig-Antich J: Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children-Epidemiologic Version. Fort Lauderdale, Fla, Center for Psychological Studies, Nova Southeastern University, 1994Google Scholar

22. Wechsler D: Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children. San Antonio, Tex, Psychological Corp, 1991Google Scholar

23. Wechsler D: Manual for the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, Revised (WAIS-R). San Antonio, Tex, Psychological Corp, 1981Google Scholar

24. Wilkinson GS: Wide-Range Achievement Test 3: Administration Manual. Wilmington, Del, Wide Range, 1993Google Scholar

25. Bilder RM , Goldman RS , Robinson D , Reiter G , Bell L , Bates JA , Pappadopulos E , Willson DF , Alvir JMJ , Woerner MG , Geisler S , Kane JM , Lieberman JA: Neuropsychology of first-episode schizophrenia: initial characterization and clinical correlates. Am J Psychiatry 2000; 157:549–559LinkGoogle Scholar

26. Green MF , Nuechterlein KH , Gold JM , Barch DM , Cohen J , Essock S , Fenton WS , Frese F , Goldberg TE , Heaton RK , Keefe RS , Kern RS , Kraemer H , Stover E , Weinberger DR , Zalcman S , Marder SR: Approaching a consensus cognitive battery for clinical trials in schizophrenia: the NIMH-MATRICS conference to select cognitive domains and test criteria. Biol Psychiatry 2004; 56:301–307Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

27. Green MF , Nuechterlein KH: The MATRICS initiative: developing a consensus cognitive battery for clinical trials. Schizophr Res 2004; 72:1–3Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

28. Chapman LJ , Chapman JP: Problems in the measurement of cognitive deficit. Psychol Bull 1973; 79:380–385Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

29. Chapman LJ , Chapman JP: Commentary on two articles concerning generalized and specific cognitive deficits. J Abnorm Psychol 2001; 110:31–39Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

30. Goldman HH , Skodol AE , Lave TR: Revising axis V for DSM-IV: a review of measures of social functioning. Am J Psychiatry 1992; 149:1148–1156LinkGoogle Scholar

31. Cohen J: Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. Hillsdale, NJ, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1988Google Scholar

32. Cannon TD , Cadenhead K , Cornblatt B , Woods SW , Addington J , Walker E , Seidman LJ , Perkins D , Tsuang M , McGlashan T , Heinssen R: Prediction of psychosis in youth at high clinical risk: a multisite longitudinal study in North America. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2008; 65:28–37Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

33. Lemeshow S , Hosmer DW: A review of goodness of fit statistics for use in the development of logistic regression models. Am J Epidemiol 1982; 115:92–106Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

34. Nagelkerke NJD: A note on a general definition of the coefficient of determination. Biometrika 1991; 78:691–692CrossrefGoogle Scholar

35. Hanley JA , McNeil BJ: The meaning and use of the area under a receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curve. Radiology 1982; 143:29–36Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

36. Hollingshead AB , Redlich FC: Social Class and Mental Illness: A Community Study. New York, John Wiley & Sons, 1958CrossrefGoogle Scholar

37. Dickinson D , Ramsey ME , Gold JM: Overlooking the obvious: a meta-analytic comparison of digit symbol coding tasks and other cognitive measures in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2007; 64:532–542Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

38. Bowie CR , Leung WW , Reichenberg A , McClure MM , Patterson TL , Heaton RK , Harvey PD: Predicting schizophrenia patients' real-world behavior with specific neuropsychological and functional capacity measures. Biol Psychiatry 2008; 63:505–511Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

39. Aleman A , Hijman R , de Haan EHF , Kahn RS: Memory impairment in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Am J Psychiatry 1999; 156:1358–1366AbstractGoogle Scholar

40. Rodriguez-Sanchez JM , Crespo-Facorro B , Gonzalez-Blanch C , Perez-Iglesias R , Vazquez-Barquero JL: Cognitive dysfunction in first-episode psychosis: the processing speed hypothesis. Br J Psychiatry Suppl 2007; 51:s107–s110Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

41. Seidman LJ , Giuliano AJ , Smith CW , Stone WS , Glatt SJ , Meyer E , Faraone SV , Tsuang MT , Cornblatt B: Neuropsychological functioning in adolescents and young adults at genetic risk for schizophrenia and affective psychoses: results from the Harvard and Hillside Adolescent High Risk Studies. Schizophr Bull 2006; 32:507–524Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar