The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×

To the Editor: There has been great interest in deriving a multidimensional model of obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) symptoms. Factor analyses have delineated the heterogeneous symptoms (as measured by the Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale–Symptom Checklist) into three to six dimensions associated with separable patterns of comorbidity, treatment response, and neural correlates (1) .

In the December 2008 issue of the Journal , Michael H. Bloch, M.D., et al. (2) presented a meta-analysis of 21 factor-analytic studies of OCD and reported the following four factors: 1) symmetry (symmetry obsessions and repeating, ordering, and counting compulsions); 2) forbidden thoughts (aggressive, sexual, religious, and somatic obsessions and checking compulsions); 3) cleaning (cleaning and contamination); and 4) hoarding (hoarding obsessions and compulsions). The authors noted that the meta-analysis was limited by the use of category-level data rather than item-level (symptom) data from the Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale–Symptom Checklist. Category-level factor analyses are problematic, since they assume the validity of the symptom groupings, restrict the number of items available for analysis, and limit the symptom dimensions that can emerge. An item-level confirmatory factor analysis (3) failed to empirically support the Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale–Symptom Checklist categories, given the heterogeneity within certain checklist categories. In particular, the aggressive obsessions category includes items that target fear of aggressive impulses as well as over-responsibility for harm. Dr. Bloch et al. recommended item-level studies to clarify the relationship between the aggressive obsessions items and the sexual, religious, somatic, and checking categories.

Two recently published item-level studies, one with 485 subjects (4) and one with 398 subjects (5) , addressed this issue. Applying novel statistical methods, five similar factors were derived in both of these studies (e.g., symmetry/ordering; hoarding; doubt/checking; contamination/cleaning; and taboo thoughts [aggressive, sexual, religious obsessions] [4] ). Also in both studies, fear of aggressive impulse items loaded with the other “taboo” obsessions, while the over-responsibility for harm items loaded with checking compulsions. This separation of the a priori aggressive obsessions category was previously reported in an exploratory factor analysis (6) . Although they share variance, the taboo thoughts and doubt/checking factors represent differentiable phenomena that are more homogeneous than the corresponding combined aggressive/sexual/religious/somatic/checking factor reported in the meta-analysis conducted by Dr. Bloch et al. Differentiating these components may further help decrease the phenotypic heterogeneity of OCD in the hope of making gains in conceptualization, pathophysiology, and treatment of this disorder.

New York, N.Y.
Providence, R.I.
Bethesda, Md.
Baltimore, Md.
Providence, R.I.

Drs. Pinto, Greenberg, Nestadt, and Rasmussen have received grant support from the National Institute of Mental Health. Dr. Murphy reports no competing interests.

This letter (doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.09020287) was accepted for publication in March 2009.

References

1. Mataix-Cols D, Rosario-Campos MC, Leckman JF: A multidimensional model of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Am J Psychiatry 2005; 162:228–238Google Scholar

2. Bloch MH, Landeros-Weisenberger A, Rosario MC, Pittenger C, Leckman JF: Meta-analysis of the symptom structure of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Am J Psychiatry 2008; 165:1532–1542Google Scholar

3. Summerfeldt LJ, Richter MA, Antony MM, Swinson RP: Symptom structure in obsessive-compulsive disorder: a confirmatory factor-analytic study. Behav Res Ther 1999; 37:297–311Google Scholar

4. Pinto A, Greenberg BD, Grados MA, Bienvenu OJ 3rd, Samuels JF, Murphy DL, Hasler G, Stout RL, Rauch SL, Shugart YY, Pauls DL, Knowles JA, Fyer AJ, McCracken JT, Piacentini J, Wang Y, Willour VL, Cullen B, Liang KY, Hoehn-Saric R, Riddle MA, Rasmussen SA, Nestadt G: Further development of YBOCS dimensions in the OCD Collaborative Genetics study: symptoms vs. categories. Psychiatry Res 2008; 160:83–93Google Scholar

5. Schooler C, Revell AJ, Timpano KR, Wheaton M, Murphy DL: Predicting genetic loading from symptom patterns in obsessive- compulsive disorder: a latent variable analysis. Depress Anxiety 2008; 25:680–688Google Scholar

6. Pinto A, Eisen JL, Mancebo MC, Greenberg BD, Stout RL, Rasmussen SA: Taboo thoughts and doubt/checking: a refinement of the factor structure for obsessive compulsive disorder symptoms. Psychiatry Res 2007; 151:255–258Google Scholar