The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×

Abstract

Objective: This study evaluated the association of neurocognition and symptoms with measures of social and occupational functioning in the Clinical Antipsychotic Trials of Intervention Effectiveness (CATIE). Method: CATIE was an 18-month study of individuals with schizophrenia. Symptoms of 1,386 patients were measured with the positive syndrome scale of the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) and a PANSS negative symptom scale that eliminated items that most overlap with measures of community functioning or neurocognition. The Heinrichs-Carpenter Quality of Life Scale, which a rater completes on the basis of the patient’s self-report, and recent employment were used to assess community functioning. Hierarchical regression analyses and mixed models tested the association of neurocognition and symptoms with social/occupational functioning as well as changes in these measures during treatment. Results: Both symptoms and neurocognition were associated with quality of life in bivariate correlation analyses. Symptoms contributed more to the incremental explained variance in quality of life than did neurocognitive functioning, but both kinds of measures were significantly related to quality of life. In an analysis including only the positive syndrome scale, the increased explained variance in quality of life was about equal to that associated with neurocognition. Neurocognition and both symptom measures were independently associated with quality of life in the cross-sectional mixed-model analysis. Changes in neurocognition and both symptom measures during treatment were also significantly associated with change in the quality of life. Conclusions: Both psychotic symptoms and neurocognitive deficits appear to contribute independently to decreased quality of life in schizophrenia.

Social and occupational impairment has long been recognized as a core feature of schizophrenia affecting social interaction, vocational and instrumental functioning skills, self-care, and recreation (1) . Poor performance on neurocognitive tasks has been widely observed to be associated with poor performance on measures of community functioning, psychosocial skill acquisition, and social problem solving (2) , and several domains of neuropsychological performance have been consistently associated with overall clinical outcomes, albeit less consistently in first-episode patients (35) .

Both negative and positive symptoms of schizophrenia have also been found to be associated with functional outcomes. Few studies, however, have sought to compare the magnitude of the associations of neurocognition and symptoms with functioning, and the results that have been published have been somewhat conflicting (610) . Some cross-sectional studies of chronic schizophrenia have suggested that psychopathology might be more strongly correlated with community functioning than cognition (8 , 11) , while others have concluded that cognition is the strongest correlate (2) .

A recent cross-sectional study (12) using data from a large randomized Department of Veterans Affairs (VA) trial of antipsychotic medication for schizophrenia examined the association of measures of positive and negative symptoms, as well as of neurocognition, with community functioning as measured by both the Heinrichs-Carpenter Quality of Life Scale (13) and days of employment. In that study of nonelderly, predominantly community-dwelling adults, the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) (14) measure of negative symptoms was modified to exclude items that directly related to cognitive capacity or functioning. Results suggested that 1) symptoms and neurocognition both have independent associations with functioning and 2) symptoms may be even more strongly associated with functioning than neurocognition. Further examination of the relationship of neurocognition and symptoms to functioning is needed and could potentially guide the development of pharmacological and rehabilitative treatments and further our understanding of the complexity of this illness.

In this study, we examined the relative strength of association of measures of community functioning, as assessed by the Quality of Life Scale and self-reported days of employment, with measures of both neurocognition and symptoms. The data were from the Clinical Antipsychotic Trials of Intervention Effectiveness (CATIE) schizophrenia study, a large randomized, controlled trial funded by the National Institute of Mental Health and designed to compare outcomes of antipsychotic medications (15) . Although CATIE aimed to compare the effectiveness of different medications over an 18-month follow-up period, investigators found few differences between treatments in their effect on symptoms (16) , neurocognition (17) , or community functioning but did note significant relationships of both baseline positive symptoms and neurocognition with improvement in community functioning (18) .

We used the rich data available from this large study to evaluate the relationship of symptoms and neurocognition to functioning, in order to determine whether the cross-sectional analyses of the VA trial (12) could be replicated. We hypothesized, accordingly, that social and occupational functioning would be significantly associated with measures of both neurocognition and symptoms and that, as in the VA study, measures of neurocognition would contribute somewhat less explanatory power than symptoms to models of community functioning when added either before or after the effect of symptoms was accounted for.

Method

Study Design

The CATIE study was designed to compare the effectiveness and cost-effectiveness of currently available atypical and conventional antipsychotic medications through a randomized clinical trial involving a large number of patients treated for schizophrenia at multiple sites, including both academic and more representative community providers. Participants gave written informed consent to participate in protocols approved by local institutional review boards. Details of the study design and entry criteria have been presented elsewhere (19) . The diagnosis of schizophrenia was confirmed by the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID) (20) . The current study relies on data gathered at baseline and at the 6-month and 18-month follow-ups, the only three time points at which data on symptoms, neurocognitive functioning, and social/vocational functioning were collected at the same time.

Measures

The primary dependent measure of interest was the Heinrichs-Carpenter Quality of Life Scale (13) , a rater-administered scale that assesses overall quality of life and functioning on 21 items rated from 0 to 6 (with higher scores reflecting better quality of life) and yields measures on four subscales that address 1) social activity, 2) instrumental functioning, 3) use of objects and participation in activities, and 4) intrapsychic functioning (i.e., sense of purpose, motivation, curiosity, and ability to experience pleasure). This scale showed high sensitivity to both change and treatment effect and moderate-to-high correlations with other measures of quality of life, and it was found to have substantial sensitivity to subtle change and treatment effects (21) . A second dependent measure was the number of days of employment in the previous 30 days, as reported by the patient.

Symptoms of schizophrenia were assessed with the PANSS (14) , which yields a total average symptom score, based on 30 items rated from 1 to 7 (with higher scores indicating more severe symptoms), as well as subscales reflecting positive and negative symptoms. In order to avoid statistical redundancy between the PANSS negative syndrome scale and our measures of functional status, we modified the negative syndrome scale by omitting three items that were, at face value, related to either cognition or overall functioning and that, in fact, correlated highly with the total score on the Quality of Life Scale: “difficulty in abstracting,” “emotional withdrawal,” and “passive/apathetic social withdrawal.”

Neurocognitive functioning was measured by separate test scores, described in a previous publication (22) , that were converted to z-scores and combined to construct scales for five domains: 1) processing speed, calculated as the average score for the Grooved Pegboard test, the WAIS-R digit symbol test, and the average of the Controlled Oral Word Association Test and category instances; 2) verbal memory, assessed with the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test (average of three trials), 3) vigilance, as shown by the summary score for the Continuous Performance Test d′ scores; 4) reasoning, reflected by the average scores on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test and WISC-R mazes; and 5) working memory, as represented by the average on a computerized test of visuospatial working memory (sign reversed) and letter-number sequencing. A neurocognitive composite score was calculated by averaging the standardized scores (z-scores) of the summary scores for these five domains.

Analyses

Pearson’s product-moment correlations were used to evaluate the bivariate associations between neurocognition, symptoms, and functional status and to identify sociodemographic control variables for inclusion in multivariate models. The strength of the correlations was compared by using the Fisher z-transform test. A series of hierarchical regression models (on the quality of life total score, on each quality of life subscale, and on the number of days employed) were then used to examine the association of functional status with 1) demographic characteristics and visit number (time), 2) schizophrenia symptoms (PANSS positive syndrome scale and modified negative syndrome scale), and 3) neurocognition, entered in that order. The outcome of primary interest was the incremental proportion of variance explained by each model in the hierarchical series. To avoid potential confound due to possible change in performance over time, visit number was included as a class variable in the first step of the hierarchical regression model. The significance of differences in R 2 values across models was also tested with the Fisher z-transform test (23) . Although data were pooled across the three time points (baseline, 6 months, and 18 months), the variable representing postrandomization month controlled for time effects. Since previous publications have demonstrated that there were few significant differences between treatments in CATIE on the Quality of Life Scale (18) or neurocognitive functioning measures (17) and few significant differences on symptom ratings (24) , treatment assignment was not included in the model. Data were included from all of the phases of CATIE (19) .

Three additional variations on these models were run. First, in light of shared variance between neurocognition and symptoms, an alternate series of standard regression models were run that entered neurocognition in the step before symptoms. Second, in order to eliminate concerns about inflation of the relationship between schizophrenia symptoms and functioning due to a conceptual and/or statistical overlap between negative symptoms and the measure of functioning employed, we evaluated the association between schizophrenia symptoms and functioning using only the PANSS positive syndrome scale. Third, we examined the independent association of neurocognition measures and positive and negative symptoms with the Quality of Life Scale using all available data. Mixed-effects models were used to adjust for the correlation of repeated observations from the same subjects.

To test the robustness of the findings, we repeated the mixed-model analyses, 1) separating the data for each drug, using observations from phase 1 of the trial, 2) substituting measures of each of the five domains of the neurocognitive battery for the composite measure, in separate analyses, 3) limiting the analysis to baseline data only, 4) adjusting for site effects, and 5) controlling for potentially confounding factors, i.e., insight (the Insight and Treatment Attitudes Questionnaire [25] ), depression (Calgary Depression Scale for Schizophrenia [26] ), extrapyramidal symptoms (Simpson-Angus Rating Scale [27] ), tardive dyskinesia (Abnormal Involuntary Movement Scale [28] ), and akathisia (Barnes Rating Scale for Drug-Induced Akathisia [29] ).

Finally, to examine the longitudinal association between the change in both symptoms and neurocognition and the change in quality of life (the association between change and change), we constructed an additional set of measures in which the baseline score on each measure for each subject was subtracted from each of the follow-up scores. An additional set of mixed models was used to test the associations between these measures of change. Baseline observations were excluded from these analyses, but the covariates included sociodemographic characteristics, time (6 months or 18 months), and baseline values for all three change measures. Subanalyses addressed 1) each of the phase 1 medications in separate analyses, 2) each of the five domains of the neurocognitive battery, and 3) further adjustment for the effects of potentially confounding factors, such as insight (25) , depression (26) , and site.

Results

Subject Characteristics

This analysis included 1,386 patients of the 1,432 individuals with a DSM-IV diagnosis of schizophrenia, validated by the SCID, who were treated at 57 U.S. sites. A total of 792 patients completed the 6-month follow-up, and 77 completed the 18-month follow-up. The patients were middle-aged, and most were Caucasian and chronically ill ( Table 1 ). The group was similar in sociodemographic and clinical characteristics to participants in other major trials of atypical antipsychotics (30) .

Bivariate Analyses

Bivariate analyses of correlations of the PANSS subscale scores and the neurocognitive score with the measures of community functioning showed significant relationships between functioning and measures of positive symptoms, negative symptoms, and neurocognition, with weak to moderate strength of association ( Table 2 ). The direction of the association between symptoms and functioning was negative (more severe symptoms were associated with poorer functioning), while the direction of the association between cognition and functioning was positive (better cognition was associated with better functioning). The Quality of Life Scale total score was more strongly correlated with negative symptoms than with either the neurocognitive score (Fisher z-transform, p=0.05) or the score for positive symptoms (p=0.03).

For the Quality of Life Scale subscales for interpersonal relations and instrumental role functioning and for the number of days of employment in the previous 30 days, the strengths of the correlations with positive symptoms and neurocognition were not significantly different in the Fisher z-transform test. The two other quality of life subscales, intrapsychic foundations and common objects and activities, were more strongly associated with neurocognition than with positive symptoms (p=0.05 and p=0.03, respectively).

Multivariate Analyses

For economy of presentation, only the final step in each sequential model of the standard hierarchical multiple regression analyses is presented ( Table 3 ). Sociodemographic characteristics explained 4% of the variation in the total score on the Quality of Life Scale when entered on the first step, with a range of 2% to 4% on the quality of life subscales and days of employment ( Table 3 , top panel; Figure 1 ).

Figure 1. Contributions of Sociodemographic Variables, Neurocognition, and Symptoms to Functional Measures for 1,386 Patients With Schizophrenia a

a Percents are based on R 2 values determined by means of hierarchical multiple regression analyses. Sociodemographic variables include age, race/ethnicity, and gender. The measure of neurocognitive functioning was the average of standardized scores on subscales for five domains, as described in text. Symptoms were measured with the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale; the scale for negative symptoms was modified to omit three items directly reflecting neurocognition and community functioning: “difficulty in abstract thinking,” “passive/apathetic withdrawal,” and “emotional withdrawal.”

The addition of symptoms to sociodemographic characteristics increased the explained variance in the quality of life total score by 15% ( Figure 2 ), with changes in the R 2 value ranging from 2% to 17% ( Table 3 , second panel). The PANSS modified negative syndrome scale was most strongly and negatively associated with the number of days worked in the previous 30 days, while the positive syndrome scale was most strongly and negatively associated with the intrapsychic foundations subscale of the Quality of Life Scale.

Figure 2. Effect of Order in Multiple Regression Analyses of Contributions of Sociodemographic Variables, Neurocognition, and Symptoms to Functional Measures for 1,386 Patients With Schizophrenia a

a Percents are based on R 2 values. Sociodemographic variables include age, race/ethnicity, and gender. The measure of neurocognitive functioning was the average of standardized scores on subscales for five domains, as described in text. Symptoms were measured with the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale; the scale for negative symptoms was modified to omit three items directly reflecting neurocognition and community functioning: “difficulty in abstract thinking,” “passive/apathetic withdrawal,” and “emotional withdrawal.”

When neurocognition was added to both sociodemographic characteristics and symptoms, it explained an additional 3% of the variance in the Quality of Life Scale total score ( Figure 2 ), with the largest incremental explained variance for the number of days of employment in the previous 30 days and the least incremental explained variance in the quality of life subscale for interpersonal relations and social networks ( Table 3 , third panel). The additional variance explained by neurocognition over and above symptoms (3%–6%) was less than half the variance explained by the addition of symptoms to neurocognition—12% for the Quality of Life Scale total score, with a range from 5% to 20%.

When neurocognition was added to the model including sociodemographic variables but not symptoms, it explained an additional 6% of the variance in the Quality of Life Scale total score ( Figure 2 ), with a range from 1% to 10% ( Table 3 , fourth panel; Figure 1 ). Thus, neurocognition added less than half as much to the explained variance as symptoms both when these measures were added to the model including sociodemographic variables alone and when neurocognition was added to the model that already included the other measure (i.e., when neurocognition was added to symptoms and when symptoms were added to neurocognition) ( Figure 2 ).

A more conservative replication of these analyses included only the positive syndrome scale as a symptom measure in addition to sociodemographic characteristics (i.e., the modified negative syndrome scale was excluded). After the positive syndrome subscale was added to sociodemographic variables ( Table 3 , fifth panel), the model explained 10% of the variance in the Quality of Life Scale total score (ΔR 2 =5%, with an incremental range of 1%–6% on the other measures). The incremental effect of the positive syndrome subscale was not significantly different from the effect of adding neurocognition to sociodemographic variables on the total score, on the number of days of employment, or on two of the quality of life subscales ( Table 3 , sixth panel).

Mixed-Model Regression Analysis

In the mixed-model analysis of the association of symptoms and neurocognition with the total Quality of Life Scale score, significant relationships were noted between quality of life and positive symptoms (r=–0.03, df=852, p<0.001), the modified negative symptom subscale (r=–0.06, df=852, p<0.0001), and neurocognition (r=0.30, df=852, p<0.0001), thus demonstrating independent effects for both symptoms and neurocognition in association with the measures of functioning (online data supplement Table 1).

The three independent variables representing positive symptoms, negative symptoms, and neurocognition also remained significantly related to the quality of life total score when these analyses were repeated 1) separately for each drug by using observations from phase 1 of the trial (there were only 22 observations for clozapine, but the nonsignificant associations were in the same directions as for the other drugs), 2) by using each of the five domains of the neurocognitive battery in separate analyses, 3) by limiting the analysis to baseline observations, and 4) by including adjustment for site effects and the potentially confounding effects of insight, depression, and extrapyramidal side effects (data supplement Table 1).

In the mixed-model analysis of the association of change in symptoms and neurocognition and change in quality of life, significant relationships were also noted between the changes in quality of life and positive symptoms (beta=–0.02, df=54, p<0.008), negative symptoms (beta=–0.05, df=54, p<0.0001), and neurocognition (beta=0.32, df=54, p<0.0001) (data supplement Table 2). The strengths of associations (magnitudes of coefficients) between change in the two symptom measures and change in quality of life were somewhat smaller in the longitudinal analysis than in the cross-sectional analysis (data supplement Table 1), but the strength of association was about the same for neurocognition. Because the numbers of observations available for individual drugs in phase 1 were small (range=52–135; see data supplement Table 2), most of these associations were not statistically significant, although the direction of the associations was consistent with the overall longitudinal analysis for 13 of 15 coefficients. The associations of change in symptoms and specific neurocognitive domains were all significant and in the same direction as those in the main longitudinal analysis, with little variation across domains. The result of the analysis that adjusted for site and additional clinical covariates was similar in magnitude to the outcome of the overall change analysis (last row in data supplement Table 2), although the symptom effects were larger and closer in magnitude to those observed in the mixed-model analyses of cross-sectional data (data supplement Table 1).

Discussion

This study examined the relationship of both neurocognition and schizophrenia symptoms to social and vocational functioning at a macrosocial level (31) , using the Heinrichs-Carpenter Quality of Life Scale and reported days of employment in a large group of patients (N=1,386). We used a newly constructed modification of the PANSS negative syndrome scale that eliminated items most directly overlapping with measures of community functioning, so as to avoid artificial inflation of the association of negative symptoms and functioning. Both symptoms and cognition, including positive symptoms considered alone, had statistically significant associations with the Quality of Life Scale total score, scores on its subscales, and a measure of recent employment; symptoms had significantly stronger associations on some measures. We also examined whether symptom severity would predict social functioning, independently of neurocognition, in hierarchical multiple regression analysis. In several of these analyses, symptoms contributed more to the incremental proportion of explained variance in the level of social and vocational functioning than measures of neurocognitive functioning, when each measure was added to models that included basic sociodemographic characteristics. Positive and negative symptoms together contributed an additional 15% to the explained variance in quality of life, after sociodemographic characteristics were controlled for, while neurocognition added an additional 6%. When entered after sociodemographic variables and symptoms, measures of neurocognitive functioning explained less incremental variance (3%) than was explained when symptoms were added to sociodemographic variables and neurocognition (12%). When we conducted a more conservative analysis using only the positive syndrome scale in addition to sociodemographic variables, positive symptoms explained an additional 5% of the variance in the quality of life total score, similar to the 6% additional explained variance when neurocognition was added to sociodemographic variables.

Neurocognition and both symptom measures were independently and significantly associated with quality of life in a mixed-model regression analysis. The findings were robust in analyses conducted separately for each drug, for each of the five domains of the neurocognitive battery, with the analysis limited to baseline observations, and with adjustment for site effects and the potentially confounding effects of insight, depression, and extrapyramidal side effects. The patterns observed in cross-sectional analyses were also replicated in a set of longitudinal analyses of the relationship between change in symptom measures and neurocognition and change in the Quality of Life Scale total score. The magnitudes of the symptom effects seemed somewhat smaller than in the cross-sectional analyses, but in a final analysis that controlled for insight, depression, and site effects, the magnitudes of effect were similar, confirming the previous results. Although this study is unique in addressing longitudinal associations, the results generally replicated the findings of an earlier study that used a similar analytic approach to data from patients who participated in a VA Cooperative Study (12) , although the magnitudes of the increments in the R 2 values were somewhat more modest in this study. Green, however, has noted that even small but statistically significant effects may be of potential clinical importance (32) . The findings of this study thus confirmed our hypothesis that social or occupational functioning is significantly and independently associated with measures of both neurocognition and symptoms.

“Macrosocial” outcome measures such as the Quality of Life Scale evaluate relatively complex aspects of functioning, such as empathic ability, daily social interaction, and employment, and are contrasted with “microsocial” measures, such as tests of interpersonal and social problem solving, that address more isolated skills in a laboratory context rather than in the context of community living (31) . While microsocial measures assess subcomponents of general functioning, such as social information processing, and are more similar in form and focus to tests of neurocognition, macrosocial measures address real-world functions, such as living in independent housing, having frequent social interactions, and employment, and are thus more likely to be associated with general psychopathology. Both are important in the study of outcomes of schizophrenia.

While previous studies have examined the association of either neurocognition (2) or psychopathology (33) separately and demonstrated significant associations with various measures of functioning, studies that have not included both symptom and neurocognitive measures contemporaneously are limited because they do not address the substantial variance that is shared between symptoms and neurocognition. Such models assign all variance accounted for to the particular type of measure under study. The major advantage of the methods used here and previously (12) is that we simultaneously examined the independent association of both kinds of measures with social functioning as well as their incremental association in sequential analyses.

Several other longitudinal studies have also found symptom measures, especially when they are assessed after initiation of treatment, to be as good at predicting future functioning as are measures of neurocognition (3436) . Consistent with our analyses, these longitudinal comparisons of cognitive and symptom measures in predicting functional outcomes support the notion that both kinds of measures play distinctive roles in predicting future functioning.

Other cross-sectional studies have also found significant correlations between symptoms and social and vocational functioning (6 , 7 , 12 , 37 , 38) . In contrast to these studies, Velligan et al. presented evidence that the relationship of symptoms to daily functioning was largely attenuated in models including neurocognitive variables (9) . However, their study group was limited to chronically ill, but recently admitted, inpatients. In contrast, our study, like several others (6 , 7 , 12) , primarily involved observations from outpatients residing in community settings. Residual symptoms among outpatients appear more likely to reflect enduring clinical traits associated with long-term functional status, as compared to the highly disorganized and transient states that emerge during acute exacerbations that require hospitalization.

The findings of this study that symptoms (negative and positive) and neurocognitive functioning are both independently significant correlates and predictors of social and vocational functioning have potentially important implications for treatment development. In recent years efforts have been made to develop both behavioral therapies and pharmacotherapies that are targeted at improving both symptoms and cognitive functioning. With the increased study of cognitive deficits in the last 10 years, “cognitive rehabilitation” interventions have been developed to directly address these deficits. Several reviews (39) have concluded that cognitive rehabilitation approaches seem to be capable of improving cognitive performance, psychiatric symptoms, and everyday functioning, although it is not known how sustainable these gains can be. When coupled with psychosocial interventions such as supported employment, cognitive rehabilitation has been found to yield substantial synergistic improvements in employment outcomes (40 , 41) .

Another line of research has focused on cognitive-behavioral therapy (CBT) and has also shown significant benefits in reducing symptoms among some patients with schizophrenia (42 , 43) . While these studies differ on a number of dimensions—including duration of intervention, number of sessions, comparison treatment, and outcome measures—CBT seems to be particularly effective in ameliorating psychopathology, even among inpatients with acute psychotic episodes, with effect sizes as large as 0.65 and continued gains over time (44) .

Recent studies have suggested that the cognitive improvement observed after treatment with atypical antipsychotics is consistent in magnitude with practice effects (17) , and some have questioned earlier findings suggesting neurocognitive advantages of the second-generation antipsychotics (32) . Some of the emphasis on the development of pharmacotherapies for neurocognition seems to be based on the assumption that symptomatic treatments are generally adequate.

A limitation of this study that requires comment is that at many CATIE sites the rater who completed the PANSS also completed the Quality of Life Scale and at some of these sites a different rater administered the neurocognitive tests, thus introducing a potential rater’s bias that could have inflated the specific correlation between these two variables.

The results of the present reanalysis of CATIE data, nevertheless, suggest that efforts should continue on all fronts, behavioral and pharmacological, to address both symptoms and neurocognitive deficits. Both kinds of impairments, as we have seen, have independent adverse effects on community functioning, and perhaps the best development paradigm would move forward on both behavioral and pharmacological treatments that target both symptoms and neurocognition. It seems advisable to systematically test combinations of therapies in the hope that synergies and positive interactions may lead to new and more effective treatment strategies in addition to individual interventions.

Received Nov. 3, 2007; revisions received Dec. 24, 2007, and Jan. 6, 2008; accepted Jan. 27, 2008 (doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.07111713). From the New England Mental Illness, Research, Education and Clinical Center, Department of Veterans Affairs, West Haven, Conn.; the Department of Psychiatry, Yale Medical School, New Haven, Conn.; the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Duke University, Durham, N.C.; the Department of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill, N.C.; and the Department of Psychiatry, Columbia College of Physicians and Surgeons, New York. Address correspondence and reprint requests to Dr. Mohamed, VA Connecticut Health Care System, 950 Campbell Ave., West Haven, CT 06516; [email protected] (e-mail).

Dr. Mohamed has received research funding from Forest Pharmaceuticals and consulting fees or advisory payments from Forest Laboratories, Eli Lilly, and Janssen. Dr. Rosenheck has received research funding from AstraZeneca, Eli Lilly, Janssen, and Wyeth and consulting fees from Bristol-Myers Squibb, GlaxoSmithKline, Janssen, and Organon. Dr. Lieberman has received research funding from AstraZeneca, Bristol-Myers Squibb, GlaxoSmithKline, Janssen, and Pfizer; he has received consulting and educational fees from AstraZeneca, Bristol-Myers Squibb, Eli Lilly, Forest, GlaxoSmithKline, Janssen, Novartis, Pfizer, and Solvay. Dr. Stroup has received speaking or consulting fees from Eli Lilly, Janssen, and Lundbeck. Dr. Swartz has received research funding and consulting fees from Eli Lilly and has received speaking fees from Bristol-Myers Squibb, Eli Lilly, and Pfizer. Dr. Keefe has received research funding from Eli Lilly, Johnson & Johnson, and Pfizer; unrestricted educational funding from AstraZeneca and Organon; consulting fees or advisory board payments from Abbott, Acadia, BiolineRx, Bristol-Myers Squibb, Cephalon, Cortex, Cyberonics, Dainippon Sumitomo, Eli Lilly, Johnson & Johnson, Lundbeck, Memory, Merck, Orexigen, Organon, Otsuka, Pfizer, Sanofi/Aventis, and Xenoport; speaking fees from Eli Lilly; and royalties for the Brief Assessment of Cognition and for the MATRICS Battery.

This article was based on results from the Clinical Antipsychotic Trials of Intervention Effectiveness (CATIE) project, supported by NIMH (N01 MH-90001). The aim of this project was to examine the comparative effectiveness of antipsychotic drugs in conditions for which their use is clinically indicated, including schizophrenia and Alzheimer’s disease. Medication was provided by AstraZeneca, Bristol-Myers Squibb, Eli Lilly, Forest, Janssen, Otsuka, Pfizer, and Zenith Goldline.

References

1. Bellack AS, Sayers M, Mueser KT, Bennett M: Evaluation of social problem solving in schizophrenia. J Abnorm Psychol 1994; 103:371–378Google Scholar

2. Green MF: What are the functional consequences of neurocognitive deficits in schizophrenia? Am J Psychiatry 1996; 153:321–330Google Scholar

3. Bilder RM, Goldman RS, Robinson D, Reiter G, Bell L, Bates JA, Pappadopulos E, Willson DF, Alvir JM, Woerner MG, Geisler S, Kane JM, Lieberman JA: Neuropsychology of first-episode schizophrenia: initial characterization and clinical correlates. Am J Psychiatry 2000; 157:549–559Google Scholar

4. Stirling J, White C, Lewis S, Hopkins R, Tantam D, Huddy A, Montague L: Neurocognitive function and outcome in first-episode schizophrenia: a 10-year follow-up of an epidemiological cohort. Schizophr Res 2003; 65:75–86Google Scholar

5. Verdoux H, Liraud F, Assens F, Abalan F, van Os J: Social and clinical consequences of cognitive deficits in early psychosis: a two-year follow-up study of first-admitted patients. Schizophr Res 2002; 56:149–159Google Scholar

6. Dickerson F, Boronow JJ, Ringel N, Parente F: Neurocognitive deficits and social functioning in outpatients with schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 1996; 21:75–83Google Scholar

7. Dickinson D, Coursey RD: Independence and overlap among neurocognitive correlates of community functioning in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 2002; 56:161–170Google Scholar

8. Kurtz MM, Moberg PJ, Ragland JD, Gur RC, Gur RE: Symptoms versus neurocognitive test performance as predictors of psychosocial status in schizophrenia: a 1- and 4-year prospective study. Schizophr Bull 2005; 31:167–174Google Scholar

9. Velligan DI, Mahurin RK, Diamond PL, Hazleton BC, Eckert SL, Miller AL: The functional significance of symptomatology and cognitive function in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 1997; 25:21–31Google Scholar

10. Wegener S, Redoblado-Hodge MA, Lucas S, Fitzgerald D, Harris A, Brennan J: Relative contributions of psychiatric symptoms and neuropsychological functioning to quality of life in first-episode psychosis. Aust NZ J Psychiatry 2005; 39:487–492Google Scholar

11. Heslegrave RJ, Awad AG, Voruganti LN: The influence of neurocognitive deficits and symptoms on quality of life in schizophrenia. J Psychiatry Neurosci 1997; 22:235–243Google Scholar

12. Perlick DA, Rosenheck RA, Kaczynski R, Bingham S, Collins J: Association of symptomatology and cognitive deficits to functional capacity in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 2008; 99:192–199; Epub 2007 Sept 12Google Scholar

13. Heinrichs DW, Hanlon TE, Carpenter WT Jr: The Quality of Life Scale: an instrument for rating the schizophrenic deficit syndrome. Schizophr Bull 1984; 10:388–398Google Scholar

14. Kay SR, Fiszbein A, Opler LA: The Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 1987; 13:261–276Google Scholar

15. Lieberman JA, Stroup TS, McEvoy JP, Swartz MS, Rosenheck RA, Perkins DO, Keefe RS, Davis SM, Davis CE, Lebowitz BD, Severe J, Hsiao JK: Effectiveness of antipsychotic drugs in patients with chronic schizophrenia. N Engl J Med 2005; 353:1209–1223Google Scholar

16. Rosenheck R, Leslie D, Keefe R, McEvoy J, Swartz M, Perkins D, Stroup S, Hsiao JK, Lieberman J, CATIE Study Investigators Group: Barriers to employment for people with schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 2006; 163:411–417Google Scholar

17. Keefe RS, Bilder RM, Davis SM, Harvey PD, Palmer BW, Gold JM, Meltzer HY, Green MF, Capuano G, Stroup TS, McEvoy JP, Swartz MS, Rosenheck RA, Perkins DO, Davis CE, Hsiao JK, Lieberman JA: Neurocognitive effects of antipsychotic medications in patients with chronic schizophrenia in the CATIE trial. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2007; 64:633–647Google Scholar

18. Swartz MS, Perkins DO, Stroup TS, Davis SM, Capuano G, Rosenheck RA, Reimherr F, McGee MF, Keefe RS, McEvoy JP, Hsiao JK, Lieberman JA, CATIE Investigators: Effects of antipsychotic medications on psychosocial functioning in patients with chronic schizophrenia: findings from the NIMH CATIE study. Am J Psychiatry 2007; 164:428–436Google Scholar

19. Stroup TS, McEvoy JP, Swartz MS, Byerly MJ, Glick ID, Canive JM, McGee MF, Simpson GM, Stevens MC, Lieberman JA: The National Institute of Mental Health Clinical Antipsychotic Trials of Intervention Effectiveness (CATIE) project: schizophrenia trial design and protocol development. Schizophr Bull 2003; 29:15–31Google Scholar

20. First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JB: Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID-I) (User’s Guide and Interview), Research Version. New York, New York State Psychiatric Institute, Biometrics Research, 1995Google Scholar

21. Cramer JA, Rosenheck R, Xu W, Thomas J, Henderson W, Charney DS: Quality of life in schizophrenia: a comparison of instruments: Department of Veterans Affairs Cooperative Study Group on Clozapine in Refractory Schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 2000; 26:659–666Google Scholar

22. Keefe RS, Mohs RC, Bilder RM, Harvey PD, Green MF, Meltzer HY, Gold JM, Sano M: Neurocognitive assessment in the Clinical Antipsychotic Trials of Intervention Effectiveness (CATIE) project schizophrenia trial: development, methodology, and rationale. Schizophr Bull 2003; 29:45–55Google Scholar

23. Papoulis A: Probability and Statistics. Englewood Cliffs, NJ, Prentice Hall, 1990Google Scholar

24. Rosenheck RA, Swartz M, McEvoy J, Stroup TS, Davis S, Keefe R, Hsiao J, Lieberman J: Changing perspectives on second generation antipsychotics: reviewing the cost-effectiveness component of the CATIE trial. Expert Review of Pharmacoeconomics and Outcomes Research 2007; 7:103–111Google Scholar

25. McEvoy JP, Aland J Jr, Wilson WH, Guy W, Hawkins L: Measuring chronic schizophrenic patients’ attitudes toward their illness and treatment. Hosp Community Psychiatry 1981; 32:856–858Google Scholar

26. Addington D, Addington J, Schissel B: A depression rating scale for schizophrenics. Schizophr Res 1990; 3:247–251Google Scholar

27. Simpson GM, Angus JW: A rating scale for extrapyramidal side effects. Acta Psychiatr Scand Suppl 1970; 212:11–19Google Scholar

28. Guy W (ed): ECDEU Assessment Manual for Psychopharmacology: Publication ADM 76-338. Washington, DC, US Department of Health, Education, and Welfare, 1976, pp 534–537Google Scholar

29. Barnes TRE: A rating scale for drug-induced akathisia. Br J Psychiatry 1989; 154:672–676Google Scholar

30. Rosenheck RA, Leslie DL, Sindelar J, Miller EA, Lin H, Stroup TS, McEvoy J, Davis SM, Keefe RS, Swartz M, Perkins DO, Hsiao JK, Lieberman J, CATIE Study Investigators: Cost-effectiveness of second-generation antipsychotics and perphenazine in a randomized trial of treatment for chronic schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 2006; 163:2080–2089Google Scholar

31. Spaulding WD, Storms L, Goodrich V, Sullivan M: Applications of experimental psychopathology in psychiatric rehabilitation. Schizophr Bull 1986; 12:560–577Google Scholar

32. Green MF: Cognition, drug treatment, and functional outcome in schizophrenia: a tale of two transitions. Am J Psychiatry 2007; 164:992–994Google Scholar

33. Eack SM, Newhill CE: Psychiatric symptoms and quality of life in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Schizophr Bull 2007; 33:1225–1237Google Scholar

34. Johnstone EC, Macmillan JF, Frith CD, Benn DK, Crow TJ: Further investigation of the predictors of outcome following first schizophrenic episodes. Br J Psychiatry 1990; 157:182–189Google Scholar

35. Goldman RS, Axelrod BN, Tandon R, Ribeiro SC, Craig K, Berent S: Neuropsychological prediction of treatment efficacy and one-year outcome in schizophrenia. Psychopathology 1993; 26:122–126Google Scholar

36. Norman RM, Malla AK, Cortese L, Cheng S, Diaz K, McIntosh E, McLean TS, Rickwood A, Voruganti LP: Symptoms and cognition as predictors of community functioning: a prospective analysis. Am J Psychiatry 1999; 156:400–405Google Scholar

37. Breier A, Schreiber JL, Dyer J, Pickar D: National Institute of Mental Health longitudinal study of chronic schizophrenia: prognosis and predictors of outcome. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1991; 48:239–246; correction, 1991; 48:642Google Scholar

38. Tsuang D, Coryell W: An 8-year follow-up of patients with DSM-III-R psychotic depression, schizoaffective disorder, and schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 1993; 150:1182–1188Google Scholar

39. Twamley EW, Jeste DV, Bellack AS: A review of cognitive training in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 2003; 29:359–382Google Scholar

40. McGurk SR, Mueser KT, Feldman K, Wolfe R, Pascaris A: Cognitive training for supported employment: 2–3 year outcomes of a randomized controlled trial. Am J Psychiatry 2007; 164:437–441Google Scholar

41. Wexler BE, Bell MD: Cognitive remediation and vocational rehabilitation for schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 2005; 31:931–941Google Scholar

42. Rector NA, Beck AT: Cognitive behavioral therapy for schizophrenia: an empirical review. J Nerv Ment Dis 2001; 189:278–287Google Scholar

43. Turkington D, Kingdon D, Chadwick P: Cognitive-behavioural therapy for schizophrenia: filling the therapeutic vacuum. Br J Psychiatry 2003; 183:98–99Google Scholar

44. Gould RA, Mueser KT, Bolton E, Mays V, Goff D: Cognitive therapy for psychosis in schizophrenia: an effect size analysis. Schizophr Res 2001; 48:335–342Google Scholar