The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×

Abstract

OBJECTIVE: Sibling recurrence risk in autism has been estimated to be approximately 10%. This study investigated subsyndromal autistic impairments among siblings of probands with pervasive developmental disorders. METHOD: The authors used the Social Responsiveness Scale to obtain quantitative assessments of autistic social impairment in three groups of proband-sibling pairs: 1) autistic children from multiple-incidence families and their closest in age nonautistic brothers (N=49 pairs); 2) children with any pervasive developmental disorder, including autism, and their closest-in-age brothers (N=100 pairs), and 3) children with psychopathology unrelated to autism and their closest-in-age brothers (N=45 pairs). RESULTS: Sibling Social Responsiveness Scale scores were continuously distributed and substantially elevated for both the autistic and pervasive developmental disorder groups. Highest scores (i.e., greatest impairment) were seen among siblings of autistic probands from multiple-incidence families, followed by siblings of probands with any pervasive developmental disorder, then siblings of probands with psychopathology unrelated to autism. CONCLUSIONS: Taken together with previous findings, these results support the notion that genetic susceptibility factors responsible for common, subsyndromal social impairments may be related to the causes of categorically defined pervasive developmental disorders.

Pervasive developmental disorders—the most common of which include autistic disorder, Asperger’s disorder, and pervasive developmental disorder not otherwise specified—are highly heritable and may lie along a severity continuum determined by a common set of genetic susceptibility factors (1). Autistic traits with severity that falls below the clinical threshold for a pervasive developmental disorder diagnosis have been found to aggregate in the family members of autistic individuals (1, 2).

A limitation of previously published family studies has been the lack of a validated method for quantitative assessment of heritable subsyndromal autistic traits. Recent advances, however, have made such quantitative characterization possible (35). In this study, we measured familial aggregation of such traits among the siblings of children with and without pervasive developmental disorders. We limited the siblings to brothers in order to avoid underestimation of familial aggregation, since lower phenotypic expression of genetic susceptibility in female subjects has been observed in studies of autistic traits in the general population (3).

Method

Subjects were recruited from two sources: the Autism Genetic Resource Exchange and the Washington University Child and Adolescent Psychiatry Service. The Autism Genetic Resource Exchange preferentially selects for multiple-incidence autism families, and for this study 49 families were recruited by mail from a pool of 75 families eligible on the basis of 1) at least one known nonautistic boy in the family and 2) prior completion of the entire Autism Genetic Resource Exchange research protocol. Washington University serves a clinical population representing the entire range of pervasive developmental disorder diagnoses (as well as all other child psychiatric conditions), and for this study, 145 families were consecutively enrolled on the basis of recruitment mailings and follow-up phone contacts. All Washington University probands were diagnosed independently by two expert clinicians. All autistic disorder diagnoses were confirmed with the Autism Diagnostic Interview—Revised. Because Washington University did not preferentially select for multiple-incidence families, autistic probands from the two sources were maintained in separate groups for data analysis.

One proband from each family was assessed along with the closest-in-age brother (since the Autism Genetic Resource Exchange preferentially selected for multiple-incidence families, the closest-in-age nonautistic brother was selected to allow more conservative estimation of familial aggregation). Thus, three sibling pair groups were studied: 1) autistic probands from multiple-incidence families and their closest-in-age nonautistic brothers (N=49 pairs); 2) probands with any pervasive developmental disorder (autistic disorder [N=27], Asperger’s disorder [N=8], or pervasive developmental disorder not otherwise specified [N=65]) and their closest-in-age brothers (N=100 pairs); 3) probands with psychopathology unrelated to autism (disruptive behavior disorders, primarily ADHD [N=28], affective disorders [N=6], or anxiety disorders [N=11]) and their closest-in-age brothers (N=45 pairs). The subjects were predominantly Caucasian and ranged in age from 4 to 18 years.

After obtaining written informed consent, the Social Responsiveness Scale scores for the probands and their brothers were obtained from both parent report (94% completion rate) and teacher report (83% completion rate). The Social Responsiveness Scale capitalizes on observations of children in natural social contexts and distinguishes children with a pervasive developmental disorder from those with other psychiatric conditions (3, 4). It generates a singular severity score for autistic social impairment (higher score equates with greater severity), use of which has been validated by extensive factor and cluster analyses among clinical and nonclinical populations (4). Social Responsiveness Scale scores are highly heritable in males (3, 5) and generally unrelated to age (from 7 to 18 years) and IQ. Composite nonverbal IQ scores on the Raven Colored Progressive Matrices obtained from the Autism Genetic Resource Exchange autistic subjects were not significantly correlated with Social Responsiveness Scale scores (Pearson’s r=–0.16).

The mean Social Responsiveness Scale score from all available informants was calculated for each subject (parent report/teacher report correlation: r=0.76). These were compared across the sibling groups using single-factor analysis of variance (ANOVA), with and without square-root transformation (which resulted in absolute values for skew of less than 0.5 in both the proband and sibling distributions). Two-factor ANOVA (proband diagnostic group, proband versus sibling status) was also employed for an overarching analysis of all study groups.

Results

Means and distributions of quantitative trait scores for autistic social impairment for all subject groups are depicted in Figure 1. Social Responsiveness Scale scores were continuously distributed in each group. Siblings of autistic probands from multiple-incidence families had the highest (most impaired) Social Responsiveness Scale scores of any sibling group, followed by siblings of probands with any pervasive developmental disorder, then by siblings of the probands with psychopathology unrelated to autism (single-factor ANOVA using square-root-transformed scores: F=16.2, df=2, 188, p<0.000001). This result was essentially unchanged when raw Social Responsiveness Scale scores were used or when two-factor ANOVA was employed with proband groups included (Figure 1). Intraclass proband-sibling Social Responsiveness Scale correlation for the entire sample was 0.35; separate scatterplots of proband versus sibling Social Responsiveness Scale score for each diagnostic group were successively positioned along a uniform regression line.

Discussion

Siblings of children with pervasive developmental disorders, who have a 10-fold increase in risk for affected pervasive developmental disorder status, are also more likely than their counterparts in unaffected families to exhibit subthreshold levels of autistic social impairment. This increased liability was observed among siblings of autistic probands from multiple-incidence autism families and to a lesser (but still highly significant) extent among siblings of probands with any pervasive developmental disorder. Subthreshold autistic impairments were quantitatively measured in this study by using an instrument (the Social Responsiveness Scale) for which scores have been shown to be highly heritable in male subjects (3, 5), and sibling intraclass correlation for Social Responsiveness Scale scores in this clinical study group (ICC=0.35) was consistent with that observed in twins in the general population (3). Therefore, these findings add to the body of evidence suggesting that the specific genetic susceptibility factors responsible for pervasive developmental disorders may also be responsible for more common social impairments, the severity of which falls below the threshold for a categorical pervasive developmental disorder diagnosis.

This study was limited to some degree by size and by variation in ascertainment strategy across the two primary sources of subjects. Although the findings support an additive genetic model for a substantial proportion of autism/pervasive developmental disorder cases, the family data collected were not extensive enough to determine whether genes of major effect are operative in specific subsets of families (as occurs, for example, in rare autistic syndromes associated with tuberous sclerosis). Future studies involving larger numbers of subjects, parents (5), and extended pedigrees are warranted to confirm and extend these findings. Additional support for shared genetic liability across the disorders in the autism spectrum comes from a recent Danish national register-based study (N=943,664 children) that showed elevated rates of autistic disorder among both the siblings of autistic probands and the siblings of children with other pervasive developmental disorders, including Asperger’s disorder (6).

We conclude that across categorically defined pervasive developmental disorder diagnoses, genetic susceptibility is manifested by familial aggregation of continuously distributed (subsyndromal) autistic traits, and that genetic studies may be enhanced by quantitative assessment of those traits across the entire range of severity with which they occur in affected families.

Received Oct. 18, 2004; revision received Jan. 4, 2005; accepted March 18, 2005. From the Departments of Psychiatry, Pediatrics, and Genetics, Washington University School of Medicine; and the Autism Genetic Resource Exchange, Los Angeles. Address correspondence and reprint requests to Dr. Constantino, Associate Professor of Psychiatry and Pediatrics, Washington University School of Medicine, 660 S. Euclid Ave., Campus Box 8134, St. Louis, MO 63110; (e-mail).Supported by a grant from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (HD 042541-01) to Dr. Constantino. The authors thank Daniel H. Geschwind, M.D., for his assistance with the project and his review of an earlier version of this manuscript. The authors also thank the families from the Autism Genetic Resource Exchange (AGRE) for their participation and the AGRE Consortium for providing resources. AGRE is a program of Cure Autism Now.

Figure 1.

Figure 1. Distribution of Social Responsiveness Scale Scores by Proband/Sibling Group

aNumber of siblings per family (mean=2.79, SD=0.91) did not differ significantly between groups. The closest-in-age brother was the sibling selected for all pairs except for the siblings from multiple-incidence autism families. For this group, the closest-in-age nonautistic brother was chosen to allow more conservative estimation of familial aggregation. In pairwise comparisons of sibling groups only, siblings of pervasive developmental disorder probands significantly differed from both siblings of autistic probands from multiple-incidence autism families (t=–2.34, df=147, p=0.02) and siblings of probands with psychopathology unrelated to autism (t=4.40, df=143, p<0.0001).

bCompared across all groups by type III, two-factor analysis of variance (proband diagnostic status, proband versus sibling status) after square-root transformation of scores (F=83.2, df=5, 388; R2=0.52 [p<0.00001 for full model and for each factor]).

References

1. Pickles A, Starr E, Kazak S, Bolton P, Papanikolaou K, Bailey A, Goodman R, Rutter M: Variable expression of the autism broader phenotype: findings from extended pedigrees. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2000; 41:491–502Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

2. Murphy M, Bolton PF, Pickles A, Fombonne E, Piven J, Rutter M: Personality traits of the relatives of autistic probands. Psychol Med 2000; 30:1411–1424Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

3. Constantino JN, Todd RD: Autistic traits in the general population: a twin study. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2003; 60:524–530Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

4. Constantino JN, Gruber CP, Davis S, Hayes S, Passanante N, Przybeck T: The factor structure of autistic traits. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2004; 45:719–726Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

5. Constantino JN, Todd RD: Intergenerational transmission of subthreshold autistic traits in the general population. Biol Psychiatry 2005; 57:655–660Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

6. Lauritsen MB, Pedersen CB, Mortensen PB: Effects of familial risk factors and place of birth on the risk of autism: a nationwide register-based study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2005; 46:963–971Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar