The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×
PerspectivesFull Access

Short-Term Treatment as Long-Term Prevention: Can Early Intervention Produce Legacy Effects?

For much of the 20th century, research cast doubt on prospects for preventing or treating antisocial behavior. But family-based research (e.g., 1) suggested that behavioral interventions to build parenting skill might reduce child conduct problems, and multiple parent training programs were developed and shown to be effective (e.g., 24; see reference 5 for other examples). Later evidence showed that even seriously delinquent adolescents could be turned around through intensive multisystem interventions (e.g., 6), although the high cost of such interventions limits their availability, and they are often introduced after arrests have been made and considerable harm done to youths, families, and society. If earlier, less costly intervention could prevent later development of serious problems, this would be good news indeed.

In this issue, Scott, Briskman, and O’Connor (7) test whether lower-cost treatment in childhood might in fact have benefits that carry over to later years, essentially operating as long-term prevention of antisocial behavior and traits. They present follow-up data from two of their group’s earlier studies (8, 9) in which parents of young children (ages 3–7 in one study, 4–6 in the other) were randomly assigned to a behavioral parent training program (4) or a control group. The initial sample in one study—severely antisocial and referred for treatment—was reassessed at ages 10–17; the initial sample in the second study—less severe but “high risk”—was reassessed at ages 9–13.

Findings at follow-up were quite different for the two early intervention study samples. For the more severe, clinic-referred sample, there were significant differences between the treatment and control groups at long-term follow-up on measures of oppositional defiant behavior, antisocial traits, parental warmth in an expressed emotion measure, parent-reported child monitoring/supervision, and reading skill. The findings suggest that early intervention focusing on parenting skills may have effects that extend well into adolescence. In contrast, follow-up analyses of the second study sample (high-risk but not clinically referred) showed no long-term effects of any kind. The contrast between these two sets of findings, and the rich body of detail provided by the authors, raises intriguing questions.

1. How firm are the long-term effects? The long-term effect on reading ability in the clinically referred sample was derived from a standardized performance measure with subjective factors minimized. However, all the positive findings on antisocial behavior and traits were derived from reports by youths and parents who had participated in the earlier trial, who were aware of which intervention they had received, and who presumably were aware that this was a follow-up study by the same investigative team. By contrast, measures involving direct observation of parenting and ratings by naive informants (i.e., current teachers) showed no long-term effects. So, should we view the findings as solid evidence that early intervention can produce long-term effects on antisocial behavior and traits, or should we see the findings as subjective reports by former study participants perhaps motivated to report good news? The question cannot be answered definitively from the evidence currently available, but it does suggest the need for a further step.

2. Can early intervention affect the objective outcomes of greatest concern to society? A potentially valuable next step for Scott and colleagues will be to follow the study participants into the years when the most high-stakes adult outcomes can be assessed, and rather objectively—outcomes such as reduced criminal behavior, gainful employment, and success in interpersonal, romantic, and parenting roles. Long-term benefits on outcomes like these could directly address questions about whether the findings transcend subjective reports, making the societal payoff for early intervention clear to all.

3. What determines whether early intervention will have a long-term impact? If we assume, for the moment, the validity of the positive effects, a logical next question is what caused them. Juxtaposing the absence of long-term effects in the high-risk study sample with the multiple long-term effects found in the clinically referred sample suggests several hypotheses, all warranting further study. At first blush, the differing findings for the two samples might seem to suggest that early intervention is more likely to produce long-term effects in children with more severe antisocial traits than in those with less severe antisocial traits, a pattern consistent with at least some previous findings (5, 10, 11). However, supplemental analyses by Scott et al. suggest that initial severity does not explain their findings, nor do between-study differences in ethnicity, maternal education, or housing type (public or other). Discerning which factors did make a difference may be difficult, in part because the two study follow-ups differed in numerous ways. For example, in the severe and referred sample, follow-up extended to age 17, potentially permitting more adolescent variability in outcomes (and thus more opportunity to detect treatment versus control differences) than in the follow-up with the high-risk sample, which topped out at age 13. The authors suggest that differences between the long-term findings of the two studies may reflect 1) larger initial intervention effects for the clinic-referred sample, which were thus more likely to be sustained over time; 2) higher motivation by parents of children for whom treatment was being sought; and 3) regression to the mean on antisocial behavior by the high-risk community control group. These three differences, common in treatment-versus-prevention study comparisons, fit the hypothesis that treatment trials with referred children are more likely to produce lasting effects than prevention trials with high-risk children who are not clinically referred. Verifying this or other hypotheses in future studies could help policy makers determine which early intervention investments might be most likely to maximize long-term societal benefit.

4. What if there were no long-term effects? Scott et al. have appropriately focused on the question of long-term benefit, and that benefit may turn out to be quite real. However, we should not forget that short-term effects alone can mean a great deal to families and to society. Both parents and their children benefit when parents learn to devote quality time to their children, give instructions in ways that are clear and clearly understood, and reward their children with attention and praise for good behavior. Both children and parents benefit when children learn to attend to their parents, follow adult instructions, obey rules, and behave prosocially with siblings and peers. The more such parent and child behavior flourishes and spreads, the more the social systems involving the parent and child benefit. These effects may all be “short-term” benefits of effective parenting interventions like those used by Scott et al., but the benefit to society can be real and potentially quite broad, even if they do not last for many years. If the effects also leave a legacy that lasts into adolescence and beyond, that would be icing on the cake.

In their engaging and thoughtful article, Scott et al. raise important questions about the potential legacy effects of high-quality interventions for parents of young children. The findings answer some of these questions and highlight other questions for the days ahead. In both respects, the authors have made a valuable contribution.

From the Department of Psychology, Faculty of Arts and Sciences, Harvard University, Cambridge, Mass.
Address correspondence to Dr. Weisz ().

Dr. Weisz receives royalties for books related to youth psychopathology and psychotherapy. Dr. Freedman has reviewed this editorial and found no evidence of influence from these relationships.

References

1 Patterson GR, Ray RS, Shaw DA: Direct intervention in families of deviant children. Oregon Research Institute Research Bulletin 1968; 8:1–11Google Scholar

2 Zisser A, Eyberg SM: Parent-child interaction therapy and the treatment of disruptive behavior disorders, in Evidence-Based Psychotherapies for Children and Adolescents, 2nd ed. Edited by Weisz JRKazdin AE. New York, Guilford Press, 2010, pp 179–193Google Scholar

3 Kazdin AE: Parent management training: evidence, outcomes, and issues. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1997; 36:1349–1356Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

4 Webster-Stratton C, Reid J: The Incredible Years parents, teachers, and children’s training series: a multifaceted treatment approach for young children with conduct problems, in Evidence-Based Psychotherapies for Children and Adolescents, 2nd ed. Edited by Weisz JRKazdin AE. New York, Guilford Press, 2010, pp 194–210Google Scholar

5 Weisz JR, Ng MN, Rutt C, Lau N, Masland SA: Psychotherapy for children and adolescents, in Bergin and Garfield’s Handbook of Psychotherapy and Behavior Change, 6th ed. Edited by Lambert MJ. New York, John Wiley & Sons, 2013, pp 541–586Google Scholar

6 Henggeler SW, Schoenwald SK, Borduin CM, Rowland MD, Cunningham PB: Multisystemic Treatment of Antisocial Behavior in Children and Adolescents. New York, Guilford Press, 1998Google Scholar

7 Scott S, Briskman J, O’Connor TG: Early prevention of antisocial personality: long-term follow-up of two randomized controlled trials comparing indicated and selective approaches. Am J Psychiatry 2014; 171:649–657LinkGoogle Scholar

8 Scott S, Spender Q, Doolan M, Jacobs B, Aspland H: Multicentre controlled trial of parenting groups for childhood antisocial behaviour in clinical practice. BMJ 2001; 323:194–198Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

9 Scott S, Sylva K, Doolan M, Price J, Jacobs B, Crook C, Landau S: Randomised controlled trial of parent groups for child antisocial behaviour targeting multiple risk factors: the SPOKES project. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2010; 51:48–57Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

10 Conduct Problems Prevention Research Group: The effects of the Fast Track preventive intervention on the development of conduct disorder across childhood. Child Dev 2011; 82:331–345Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

11 Weisz JR: Psychotherapy for Children and Adolescents: Evidence-Based Treatments and Case Examples. Cambridge, UK, Cambridge University Press, 2004CrossrefGoogle Scholar