The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×
ArticleFull Access

Hippocampal Volume in Adult Burn Patients With and Without Posttraumatic Stress Disorder

Abstract

OBJECTIVE: Increasing evidence suggests that posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) is associated with small hippocampal size. The authors compared trauma-exposed subjects with PTSD and trauma-exposed subjects without PTSD to clarify whether small hippocampal size is related to PTSD or to mere trauma exposure. METHOD: Three-dimensional structural magnetic resonance imaging was used to assess hippocampal volumes in 30 men who had recently been exposed to a severe burn trauma and 15 matched healthy comparison subjects. RESULTS: Relative to the comparison subjects, the trauma-exposed subjects with PTSD (N=15) as well as the trauma-exposed subjects without PTSD (N=15) had significantly smaller volumes of the right hippocampus (subjects with PTSD: –12%; subjects without PTSD: –13%). Larger total areas of burned body surface were significantly related to smaller left hippocampal volumes. Use of analgesic/sedative treatment with the N-methyl-d-aspartic acid (NMDA) antagonist ketamine was significantly related to larger right hippocampal volumes and to stronger PTSD symptoms. CONCLUSIONS: PTSD is not a necessary condition for small hippocampal size in trauma-exposed individuals. Rather, the results provide evidence that smaller hippocampal size in trauma-exposed individuals is a result of traumatic stress. The posttraumatic application of NMDA antagonists may protect against hippocampal damage induced by traumatic stressors but increases the patient’s risk of developing PTSD symptoms.

Previous studies have suggested that posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) is associated with small hippocampal size. Adults with PTSD related to combat exposure or childhood physical or sexual abuse have been repeatedly shown to have smaller hippocampal size, relative to healthy comparison subjects (17). However, two studies investigating children or adults with maltreatment-related PTSD failed to find smaller hippocampal size (8, 9).

Research in nonhuman primates has suggested that stress and prolonged glucocorticoid exposure may damage the hippocampus (10, 11), thus raising the possibility that traumatic stress or the chronic stress of having PTSD may induce hippocampal degeneration in humans. However, the mechanisms of possible stress-induced hippocampal degenerative changes in humans with PTSD are a matter of dispute (12, 13). Another possibility is that preexisting small hippocampal size increases the risk for traumatic exposure or for developing PTSD (14, 15).

The present study compared trauma-exposed subjects with PTSD and trauma-exposed subjects without PTSD to clarify whether small hippocampal size is related to PTSD or to trauma exposure (regardless of whether PTSD is present). Two studies have found that trauma-exposed subjects with PTSD had smaller hippocampal volumes than trauma-exposed subjects without PTSD (3, 15), whereas another study (16) did not. However, the latter study, which prospectively explored whether smaller hippocampal size occurs in recent trauma survivors with PTSD, did not include a healthy comparison group.

Studies investigating individuals with combat- or maltreatment-related PTSD revealed a high prevalence of substance-related disorders and depression (17, 15). These disorders were shown to be related to small hippocampal size as well (17, 18). We decided to study individuals with a severe burn injury, as burn trauma is less often associated with comorbid conditions, compared with combat- or assault-related trauma (19, 20).

Several studies have demonstrated that the strength of traumatic exposure is significantly related to PTSD severity (2, 3, 21–23). Burn trauma results in PTSD less often than does combat- or assault-related trauma (20, 24). More intense combat exposure increases the risk for developing PTSD (19). In contrast, burns over a large body surface do not imply a heightened risk for developing PTSD (20, 25), thus facilitating the recruitment of PTSD and non-PTSD study groups with a comparable severity of traumatic exposure.

In the present investigation the hippocampal volumes of 15 adult burn patients with PTSD and 15 adult burn patients without PTSD were compared to those of matched healthy subjects (N=15). The goals of the study were 1) to investigate whether hippocampal size is smaller in trauma-exposed subjects who do not show evidence of confounding comorbid conditions, 2) to investigate whether hippocampal size is smaller in trauma-exposed subjects without PTSD, 3) to investigate whether hippocampal size differs between subjects with and without PTSD, and 4) to analyze the relationship between traumatic stress-related variables and hippocampal size.

Method

Subjects

Thirty men who had been exposed to a severe burn trauma between 1999 and 2000 participated in the study (Table 1). Of the injuries, 60% occurred at work, 27% occurred at home, and 13% happened elsewhere. Explosions caused 30% of the injuries, and scalding accounted for 30%, flames for 17%, electric current for 10%, and other agents for 13%.

All trauma-exposed subjects were recruited from three regional burn centers (Berufsgenossenschaftliches Unfallkrankenhaus Hamburg, Berufsgenossenschaftliche Unfallklinik Berlin-Marzahn, Städtische Kliniken Dortmund Nord-Unfallklinik). Of 214 men contacted, 132 were interested in participating in the study. Of these subjects, 91 were excluded because of a neurological, substance-related, or depressive disorder; because of a history of previous trauma; because they sustained brain injury as part of the trauma; or because their medical or personal condition did not allow traveling and a 2-day stay in the Psychiatric Hospital of the University of Göttingen for the study. Forty-one subjects were invited to participate. Of these subjects, three refused to participate and two were excluded because a neurological disorder became evident during the investigation. Fifteen subjects met the criteria for PTSD, and 21 subjects did not have PTSD. Six of the subjects without PTSD were not included in the analyses described in this report because they could not be adequately matched to the subjects with PTSD according to age, height, education, age at time of the traumatic event, and burn size.

Fifteen subjects met the DSM-IV criteria for chronic PTSD on the basis of interviews with the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID). In all of these subjects, PTSD occurred shortly after the traumatic event. The other 15 subjects confirmed diagnostic criterion A, but none or at most one of criteria B–D. All subjects were free of any other current or lifetime axis I or axis II DSM-IV disorder according to the SCID interview. At the time of assessment, 80% of the subjects with PTSD and 80% of the subjects without PTSD were employed full- or part-time. None of the subjects received psychotropic medication.

The trauma-exposed subjects were compared with 15 healthy male subjects who were matched with the trauma-exposed subjects for age, height, and years of education. The comparison subjects were recruited by means of an advertisement in a local newspaper and leaflets distributed in the hospital of the University of Göttingen and in the town of Göttingen. Only subjects without a history of neurological or psychiatric disorder (as assessed by the SCID) were included. After a complete description of the study to the subjects, written informed consent was obtained.

Clinical Assessment

The Posttraumatic Diagnostic Scale (26) was used as a measure of PTSD severity. Depressive symptoms were assessed with the Beck Depression Inventory (27). The global severity index of the Symptom Checklist (SCL-90-R) (28) was used as a measure of the overall level of distress. The WAIS-R (29) and the Wechsler Memory Scale—Revised (30) were used to assess intellectual and mnemonic functions. Subjects also received a more comprehensive neuropsychological (a computer-driven test battery for attentional functions and various memory tests) and psychopathological (stress coping and personality questionnaires) assessment. Results of these assessments will be reported elsewhere.

The objective severity of the traumatic stressor was assessed by the total burned body surface area (Table 1). Seventeen burn patients received analgesic/sedative therapy because of repeated surgery or severe pain caused by the removal and replacing of bandaging material or because of smoke poisoning or head burns that made endotracheal intubation for maintenance of an imperiled airway necessary. We assessed the duration (days) of analgesic/sedative therapy to evaluate the effects of this medical treatment (Table 1).

Magnetic Resonance Image (MRI) Acquisition and Analysis

All subjects received an MR scan, obtained by using a 1.5-T Philips Gyroscan machine (Philips Medical Systems, Best, the Netherlands). Scanning parameters of the T1-weighted three-dimensional sequence were as follows: TE=6.0 msec, TR=24.0 msec, flip angle=30°, field of view=256, sagittal slice plane, matrix=256×256, slice thickness=1.3 mm, slice number=130, three-dimensional acquisition mode. Volumetric analysis was done on the basis of three-dimensional MR images. The images were transferred to a computer workstation and processed by using the CURRY software (version 4.5, Neurosoft, Inc., El Paso, Tex.). Images were reformatted into continuous slices of 1-mm thickness.

Total brain volume was calculated with automated multistep algorithms and three-dimensional region growing methods that are limited by gray matter value thresholds. Volumetric analysis of the hippocampus was done by using three-dimensional software (CURRY) that allows simultaneous analysis of sagittal, coronal, and horizontal images. The hippocampus was disarticulated from surrounding tissue on coronal slices by means of manual tracing according to a standardized protocol (31) and by aid of the serial sections provided by Duvernoy (32). The horizontal and sagittal views were consistently employed in addition to the coronal view for the demarcation of the borders of the hippocampus. All analyses were done by a single rater who was blind to the subjects’ diagnostic information. For defining the intrarater reliability of the demarcation of the hippocampus, one hemisphere image for 11 randomly chosen subjects was reassessed. The intraclass correlation coefficient for this procedure was 0.94.

Statistical Analysis

Because of the small sizes of the study groups, group comparisons were performed by using nonparametric statistical methods. Kruskal-Wallis analyses of variance (ANOVAs) and U tests were applied to compare differences between groups in demographic, clinical, and cognitive variables and in total brain volume. Partial correlation coefficients with adjustment for total brain volume were used to examine the relationship between hippocampal size and demographic, clinical, and cognitive variables.

The hippocampal volumes of the trauma-exposed subjects and the healthy subjects were compared by an overall nonparametric three-by-two analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) (33), with group as the between-subjects factor (trauma subjects with PTSD, trauma subjects without PTSD, comparison subjects) and hemisphere as the within-subjects factor (left, right) and with adjustment for total brain volume. This model was followed up by separate two-by-two (group-by-hemisphere) ANCOVAs and univariate ANCOVAs for the left and right hippocampus volumes, respectively. The relationship between traumatic stress-related variables and hippocampal volumes was further examined with multiple regression analyses.

All analyses were two-tailed, and alpha was defined as p<0.05. Statistical computations were performed by using SAS for Windows, Version 8.01 (SAS Institute, Cary, N.C.), nonparametric ANCOVAs (http://www.ams.med.uni-goettingen.de/de/sof/ld/NPAR.SAS), and SPSS for Windows, Version 10.0 (SPSS, Inc., Chicago).

Results

Subject Characteristics

The groups did not differ with respect to demographic variables or total brain volume (Table 1 and Table 2). Both trauma-exposed groups had significantly lower general intelligence and memory test scores, compared with the healthy subjects. The trauma-exposed subjects with PTSD showed significantly higher scores on the Posttraumatic Diagnostic Scale, Beck Depression Inventory, and SCL-90-R global severity index, compared with the trauma-exposed subjects without PTSD. According to normative data from the Posttraumatic Diagnostic Scale (26), one of the PTSD subjects had very severe PTSD, five subjects had severe PTSD, eight had moderate PTSD, and one had mild PTSD. There were no significant relationships between burn type (see Method section, Subjects subsection) and hippocampal volume or any clinical variable (ANCOVAs, ANOVAs).

Seventeen trauma-exposed subjects received analgesic/sedative therapy as part of the medical treatment (Table 1). In all of these cases, a ketamine/benzodiazepine combination and an opioid/benzodiazepine combination were applied in an alternating manner. Subjects who did not receive analgesic/sedative treatment sometimes received short anesthesia for surgery or for replacing of bandaging material. In the majority of these cases, anesthesia was performed with opioids. Subjects with analgesic/sedative treatment had higher Posttraumatic Diagnostic Scale symptom scores, compared with subjects without analgesic/sedative treatment (intrusions: mean=5 [SD=4] versus mean=2 [SD=2] [p<0.04, U test]; avoidance: mean=6 [SD=5] versus mean=2 [SD=2] [p<0.03, U test]; arousal: mean=4 [SD=4] versus mean=2 [SD=2] [p<0.03, U test]). Comparisons of these two subgroups on all other variables were not significant.

Hippocampal Volume: Group Comparisons

Relative to the healthy comparison subjects, trauma-exposed subjects had smaller volumes of the right hippocampus (subjects with PTSD: –12%; subjects without PTSD: –13%) and left hippocampus (subjects with PTSD: –6%; subjects without PTSD: –8%).

The overall three-by-two (group-by-hemisphere) ANCOVA yielded significant effects of group, hemisphere, and group-by-hemisphere, indicating significantly smaller hippocampal size in trauma-exposed subjects, relative to the comparison subjects (Table 2). The follow-up two-by-two (group-by-hemisphere) ANCOVAs for each trauma group and the comparison group confirmed these results. The two-by-two (group-by-hemisphere) ANCOVA for the two trauma groups did not reveal any significant effects, indicating similar hippocampal size in both groups. Univariate ANCOVAs revealed that the smaller hippocampal size of both trauma groups was statistically significant for the right hemisphere, but not for the left hemisphere. The detailed results of all statistical comparisons are given in Table 2.

Relative to the comparison subjects, the trauma-exposed subjects who received analgesic/sedative treatment (N=17) (Table 1) had smaller volumes of the left (–5%) and right (–9%) hippocampus. However, the difference in hippocampal volume for the trauma-exposed subjects who did not receive analgesic/sedative treatment was larger (left hippocampus: –10%, right hippocampus: –17%). Univariate ANCOVAs with adjustment for total brain volume revealed that the subjects without analgesic/sedative treatment (T=15.67, df=1, p<0.0001) but not those who received analgesic/sedative treatment (T=2.91, df=1, p=0.0898) had significantly smaller right hippocampal size, relative to the comparison subjects. The difference between the two trauma-exposed groups was not significant (p>0.10). The same was true for all comparisons of left hippocampal volumes (p>0.30).

Relationship Between Hippocampal Volume and Traumatic Stress-Related Variables

The total burned body surface area, global severity index, Posttraumatic Diagnostic Scale total score, and Beck Depression Inventory score were entered into stepwise multiple regression analyses (significance level for selecting variables: p=0.05). Total brain volume was entered as a further predictor to control for the influence of brain size. Considering all trauma-exposed subjects, total brain volume (proportion of explained variance [R2]=0.45, p<0.0001) and total burned body surface area (R2=0.11, p<0.03) significantly predicted left hippocampal volume, indicating smaller hippocampal volumes in subjects with larger burns. The addition of the other variables did not significantly improve the prediction.

Multiple regression analyses were also conducted for the 17 subjects who received analgesic/sedative treatment. Total burned body surface area, global severity index, Posttraumatic Diagnostic Scale total score, Beck Depression Inventory score, total brain volume, and duration (days) of analgesic/sedative treatment were used as predictors. Total brain volume (R2=0.56, p<0.0001), Posttraumatic Diagnostic Scale score (R2=0.11, p<0.05), and total burned body surface area (R2=0.09, p<0.05) significantly predicted left hippocampal volume, indicating smaller hippocampal volumes in subjects with larger burns and more severe PTSD symptoms. Total brain volume (R2=0.63, p<0.0001), Posttraumatic Diagnostic Scale score (R2=0.15, p=0.007), and duration of analgesic/sedative treatment (R2=0.06, p<0.04) significantly predicted right hippocampal volume, indicating smaller hippocampal volumes in subjects with shorter duration of analgesic/sedative treatment and more severe PTSD symptoms.

Relationship Between Hippocampal Volume and Demographic, Clinical, and Cognitive Variables

In trauma-exposed subjects (N=30), left and right hippocampal volumes were not significantly related to age, education, age at the time of the traumatic event, past time since the traumatic event, duration of hospitalization, duration of intensive care treatment, IQ, and memory test score, respectively (all p>0.15). The same was true when the subgroup of subjects who received analgesic/sedative treatment (N=17) was considered (p>0.10).

Discussion

Our results demonstrate small hippocampal size in individuals who experienced a severe burn trauma and complement earlier findings of small hippocampal size in individuals who were exposed to combat or maltreatment. Consistent with previous findings in persons who were exposed to trauma as adults (1, 3, 15), we found predominantly small right hippocampal volumes in this group of adult burn patients. In contrast, small left hippocampal size is frequently observed in persons with prepubertal traumatic experiences (2, 4, 7).

The trauma-exposed subjects in our study, although as well educated as the healthy comparison subjects, showed significantly lower intellectual and mnemonic functions (compare with Table 1). Neuropsychological impairments in the domain of intelligence and memory were also reported for Vietnam combat veterans (1, 21) and adult survivors of childhood physical or sexual abuse (21, 34).

In contrast to PTSD study groups who were exposed to combat or maltreatment (17, 15), the burn patients in the present study did not present with substance-related disorders or major depression, which have been shown to be related to small hippocampal size as well (17, 18). Our results clearly argue against the assumption that trauma-related small hippocampal size may be solely an effect of these confounding comorbid conditions.

Small Hippocampal Size in Trauma-Exposed Individuals Without PTSD

The results of our study demonstrate that trauma-exposed subjects without PTSD (or any other current or lifetime psychiatric diagnosis) have smaller hippocampal size, relative to healthy comparison subjects. Thus, PTSD may not be a necessary condition for small hippocampal size in trauma-exposed individuals. A study of Vietnam combat veterans did not find significantly smaller hippocampal size in combat veterans without PTSD, relative to healthy comparison subjects (3). However, in that study, the difference in size of the right hippocampus of the combat veterans without PTSD, relative to healthy comparison subjects, was similar in magnitude (–11%) to that of the burn patients without PTSD in the current study (–13%) and may have failed to reach statistical significance because of the small number of subjects (combat veterans: N=7; comparison subjects: N=8).

In the current study, the trauma-exposed subjects with PTSD did not differ in hippocampal size from the trauma-exposed subjects without PTSD. Similar results were obtained in individuals who were exposed to accidents (16). PTSD severity, as measured with the Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale, was moderate in this PTSD study group (mean score=58) (16). A moderate PTSD severity was also present in the PTSD group in the current study. In contrast, in two studies of Vietnam combat veterans that found smaller hippocampal size in subjects with PTSD, compared to subjects without PTSD, the mean Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale scores of the study groups with PTSD were >70 (3, 15). These findings raise the possibility that more severe, long-lasting, and unremitting forms of PTSD are more likely to be associated with small hippocampal size. This conclusion is supported by our multiple regression analyses, which revealed an association of PTSD symptoms and hippocampal size only in the subgroup of trauma-exposed subjects who received analgesic/sedative treatment. This subgroup presented with significantly stronger PTSD symptoms, compared to the subjects who did not receive analgesic/sedative treatment.

Small Hippocampal Size as a Result of Traumatic Stress

We found a relationship between larger burn size and smaller hippocampal volume in the trauma-exposed subjects in our study. Therefore, it could be assumed that traumatic stress by itself results in some kind of damage to the hippocampus. A similar relationship between severity of traumatic stress and hippocampal size was obtained in Vietnam combat veterans (3) and in subjects with childhood physical or sexual abuse (5). However, other studies failed to find a relationship (1, 2, 4, 7, 15). Our results suggest the need to explore the extent of physical trauma in PTSD study subjects who have sustained significant physical injuries.

None of the trauma-exposed subjects in our study displayed any neurological or neuropsychological abnormalities at time of our assessment. Furthermore, subjects who sustained brain injury as part of the trauma were rigorously excluded (see Method section, Subjects subsection). However, encephalopathy or delirium due to hypoxia or multiple metabolic aberrations is a common event in burn victims (35, 36), although the majority of patients recover from these conditions. There is also evidence that burn trauma leads to substantially increased cortisol levels (37). Probably, the reduced hippocampal volumes of the burn patients in our study are related to these factors. The hippocampus is known to be especially vulnerable to hypoxia and metabolic insults (32).

Multiple regression analyses showed a negative relationship between left hippocampal volume and burn size and a positive relationship between right hippocampal volume and duration of analgesic/sedative treatment. Single correlations, with adjustment for total brain volume, between burn size or analgesic/sedative treatment and left or right hippocampal volume were significant (r=–0.43, r=0.50, and r=0.54, p<0.05), except for the relationship between burn size and right hippocampal volume (r=–0.05, p>0.80). The functional significance of this finding remains to be elucidated.

Effects of the Analgesic/Sedative Treatment

We observed a relationship between longer duration of analgesic/sedative treatment and larger hippocampal volume. Group comparisons revealed that the difference in hippocampal size in the subjects with analgesic/sedative treatment, relative to the healthy subjects, was of less magnitude than the difference in the subjects without analgesic/sedative treatment. It could be speculated that the analgesic/sedative treatment might have had a protective effect on the hippocampal neurons of the burn patients in the present study. Animal studies demonstrated that N-methyl-d-aspartic acid (NMDA) glutamate receptor activation inhibits hippocampal neurogenesis (38) and that NMDA antagonists protect hippocampal neurons from acute ischemic injury (39). Ketamine, which was used as an analgesic/sedative agent in the burn patients in this study, is an NMDA antagonist. Possibly, the pertinent use of combined ketamine/benzodiazepine sedation offers a powerful tool for neuroprotection while largely preventing the unwanted side effects of NMDA antagonists (40).

Trauma-exposed subjects who received analgesic/sedative treatment presented with significantly stronger PTSD symptoms than subjects who did not receive this treatment. However, the subjects who received analgesic/sedative treatment did not differ in burn size or with regard to any other variable from the subjects who did not receive analgesic/sedative treatment. Possibly, the enduring posttraumatic application of the NMDA antagonist ketamine increased the vulnerability of the burn patients to develop PTSD symptoms. The subanesthetic application of ketamine in humans is known to produce dissociative cognitive and perceptual alterations (41). Preclinical data suggest that NMDA antagonists may produce these effects by a transient stimulation of corticolimbic glutamate release (42).

Conclusions

We found smaller hippocampal size in a group of burn patients without any confounding comorbid psychiatric disorders. Our results indicate that PTSD is not a necessary condition for small hippocampal size in trauma-exposed individuals. In addition, our results suggest the need to explore the extent of physical trauma in trauma-exposed study subjects who have sustained significant physical injuries. Furthermore, our results suggest that the posttraumatic application of NMDA antagonists probably protects against hippocampal damage induced by traumatic stressors but may increase the risk for developing PTSD symptoms. Future studies are needed to explore the therapeutic potential of drugs that attenuate glutamate release in individuals who have been exposed to traumatic stress.

TABLE 1
TABLE 2

Received Sept. 8, 2003; revision received Jan. 6, 2004; accepted Feb. 9, 2004. From the Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University of Göttingen. Address reprint requests to Prof. Dr. Irle, Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University of Göttingen, Von-Siebold-Str. 5, D-37075 Göttingen, FRG; (e-mail). Supported by grants IR 15/8-1 and IR 15/8-2 from Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft. The authors thank the subjects for their participation in the study; Drs. D. Pitzler, H. Lemke, and B. Böhme for permission to study patients under their care and for providing information concerning diagnoses and medical treatment; and I. Lausen and S. Schoof for assistance in neuropsychological assessments.

References

1. Bremner JD, Randall P, Scott TM, Bronen RA, Seibyl JP, Southwick SM, Delaney RC, McCarthy G, Charney DS, Innis RB: MRI-based measurement of hippocampal volume in patients with combat-related posttraumatic stress disorder. Am J Psychiatry 1995; 152:973–981LinkGoogle Scholar

2. Bremner JD, Randall PR, Vermetten E, Staib L, Bronen RA, Mazure C, Capelli S, McCarthy G, Innis RB, Charney DS: Magnetic resonance imaging-based measurement of hippocampal volume in posttraumatic stress disorder related to childhood physical and sexual abuse—a preliminary report. Biol Psychiatry 1997; 41:23–32Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

3. Gurvits TV, Shenton ME, Hokama H, Ohta H, Lasko NB, Gilbertson MW, Orr SP, Kikinis R, Jolesz FA, McCarley RW, Pitman RK: Magnetic resonance imaging study of hippocampal volume in chronic, combat-related posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol Psychiatry 1996; 40:1091–1099Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

4. Stein MB, Koverola C, Hanna C, Torchia MG, McClarty B: Hippocampal volume in women victimized by childhood sexual abuse. Psychol Med 1997; 27:951–959Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

5. Driessen M, Herrmann J, Stahl K, Zwaan M, Meier S, Hill A, Osterheider M, Petersen D: Magnetic resonance imaging volumes of the hippocampus and the amygdala in women with borderline personality disorder and early traumatization. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2000; 57:1115–1122Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

6. Villarreal G, Hamilton DA, Petropoulos H, Driscoll I, Rowland LM, Griego JA, Kodituwakku PW, Hart BL, Escalona R, Brooks WM: Reduced hippocampal volume and total white matter volume in posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol Psychiatry 2002; 52:119–125Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

7. Vythilingam M, Heim C, Newport J, Miller AH, Anderson E, Bronen R, Brummer M, Staib L, Vermetten E, Charney DS, Nemeroff CB, Bremner JD: Childhood trauma associated with smaller hippocampal volume in women with major depression. Am J Psychiatry 2002; 159:2072–2080LinkGoogle Scholar

8. De Bellis MD, Keshavan MS, Shifflett H, Iyengar S, Beers SR, Hall J: Brain structures in pediatric maltreatment-related posttraumatic stress disorder: a sociodemographically matched study. Biol Psychiatry 2002; 52:1066–1078Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

9. Fennema-Notestine C, Stein MB, Kennedy CM, Archibald SL, Jernigan TL: Brain morphometry in female victims of intimate partner violence with and without posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol Psychiatry 2002; 51:1089–1101CrossrefGoogle Scholar

10. Sapolsky RM, Uno H, Rebert CS, Finch CE: Hippocampal damage associated with prolonged glucocorticoid exposure in primates. J Neurosci 1990; 10:2897–2902Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

11. Gould E, Tanapat P, McEwen BS, Flügge G, Fuchs E: Proliferation of granule cell precursors in the dentate gyrus of adult monkeys is diminished by stress. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1998; 95:3168–3171Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

12. Bremner JD: Hypotheses and controversies related to effects of stress on the hippocampus: an argument for stress-induced damage to the hippocampus in patients with posttraumatic stress disorder. Hippocampus 2001; 11:75–81Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

13. Yehuda R: Are glucocorticoids responsible for putative hippocampal damage in PTSD? how and when to decide. Hippocampus 2001; 11:85–89Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

14. Pitman RK: Hippocampal diminution in PTSD: more (or less?) than meets the eye. Hippocampus 2001; 11:73–74Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

15. Gilbertson MW, Shenton ME, Ciszewski A, Kasai K, Lasko NB, Orr SP, Pitman RK: Smaller hippocampal volume predicts pathologic vulnerability to psychological trauma. Nat Neurosci 2002; 5:1242–1247Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

16. Bonne O, Brandes D, Gilboa A, Gomori JM, Shenton ME, Pitman RK, Shalev AY: Longitudinal MRI study of hippocampal volume in trauma survivors with PTSD. Am J Psychiatry 2001; 158:1248–1251LinkGoogle Scholar

17. Argatz I, Momenan R, Rawlings RR, Kerich MJ, Hommer DW: Hippocampal volume in patients with alcohol dependence. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1999; 56:356–363Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

18. Sheline YI, Sanghavi M, Mintun MA, Gado MH: Depression duration but not age predicts hippocampal volume loss in medically healthy women with recurrent major depression. J Neurosci 1999; 19:5034–5043Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

19. Green BL, Grace MC, Lindy JD, Gleser GC, Leonard A: Risk factors for PTSD and other diagnoses in a general sample of Vietnam veterans. Am J Psychiatry 1990; 147:729–733LinkGoogle Scholar

20. Roca RP, Spence RJ, Munster AM: Posttraumatic adaptation and distress among adult burn survivors. Am J Psychiatry 1992; 149:1234–1238LinkGoogle Scholar

21. Gurvits TV, Gilbertson MW, Lasko NB, Tarhan AS, Simeon D, Macklin ML, Orr SP, Pitman RK: Neurological soft signs in chronic posttraumatic stress disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2000; 57:181–186Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

22. Macklin ML, Metzger LJ, Litz BT, McNally RJ, Lasko NB, Orr SP, Pitman RK: Lower precombat intelligence is a risk factor for posttraumatic stress disorder. J Consult Clin Psychol 1998; 66:232–326CrossrefGoogle Scholar

23. McNally RJ, Shin LM: Association of intelligence with severity of posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms in Vietnam combat veterans. Am J Psychiatry 1995; 152:936–938LinkGoogle Scholar

24. Breslau N, Kessler RC, Chilcoat HD, Schultz LR, Davis GC, Andreski P: Trauma and posttraumatic stress disorder in the community. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1998; 55:626–632Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

25. Perry S, Difede J, Musngi G, Frances AJ, Jacobsberg L: Predictors of posttraumatic stress disorder after burn injury. Am J Psychiatry 1992; 149:931–935LinkGoogle Scholar

26. Foa EB: Posttraumatic Stress Diagnostic Scale Manual. Minneapolis, National Computer Systems, 1995Google Scholar

27. Hautzinger M, Bailer M, Worall H, Keller F: Beck Depressions Inventar (BDI). Bern, Switzerland, Huber, 1995Google Scholar

28. Franke GH: Die Symptom Checkliste von Derogatis—Deutsche Version. Weinheim, Germany, Beltz, 1995Google Scholar

29. Tewes U: HAWIE-R: Hamburg-Wechsler-Intelligenztest für Erwachsene, Revision 1991. Bern, Switzerland, Huber, 1991Google Scholar

30. Wechsler D: Wechsler Memory Scale—Revised. San Antonio, Tex, Psychological Corp, 1987Google Scholar

31. Pruessner JC, Li LM, Serles W, Pruessner M, Collins DL, Kabani N, Lupien S, Evans AC: Volumetry of hippocampus and amygdala with high-resolution MRI and three-dimensional analysis software: minimizing the discrepancies between laboratories. Cereb Cortex 2000; 10:433–442Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

32. Duvernoy HM: The Human Hippocampus: Functional Anatomy, Vascularization, and Serial Sections With MRI. Berlin, Springer, 1998Google Scholar

33. Brunner E, Domhof S, Langer F: Nonparametric Analysis of Longitudinal Data in Factorial Experiments. New York, John Wiley & Sons, 2002Google Scholar

34. Bremner JD, Randall PR, Scott TM, Capelli S, Delaney R, McCarthy G, Charney DS: Deficits in short-term memory in adult survivors of childhood abuse. Psychiatry Res 1995; 59:97–107Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

35. Andreasen NJ, Hartford CE, Knott JR, Canter A: EEG changes associated with burn delirium. Dis Nerv Syst 1977; 38:27–31MedlineGoogle Scholar

36. Mohnot D, Snead OC, Benton JW: Burn encephalopathy in children. Ann Neurol 1982; 12:42–47Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

37. Jeffries MK, Vance ML: Growth hormone and cortisol secretion in patients with burn injury. J Burn Care Rehabil 1992; 13:391–395Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

38. Cameron HA, McEwen BS, Gould E: Regulation of adult neurogenesis by excitatory input and NMDA receptor activation in the dentate gyrus. J Neurosci 1995; 15:4687–4692Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

39. Simon RP, Swan JH, Griffiths T, Meldrum BS: Blockade of N–methyl-d–aspartate receptors may protect against ischemic damage in the brain. Science 1984; 226:850–852Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

40. Olney JW, Labruyere J, Wang G, Wozniak DF, Price MT, Sesma MA: NMDA antagonist neurotoxicity: mechanism and prevention. Science 1991; 254:1515–1518Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

41. Krystal JH, Karper LP, Seibyl JP, Freeman GK, Delaney R, Bremner JD, Heninger GR, Bowers MB, Charney DS: Subanesthetic effects of the noncompetitive NMDA antagonist, ketamine, in humans. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1994; 51:199–214Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

42. Chambers RA, Bremner JD, Moghaddam B, Southwick SM, Charney DS, Krystal JH: Glutamate and post-traumatic stress disorder: toward a psychobiology of dissociation. Semin Clin Neuropsychiatry 1999; 4:274–281MedlineGoogle Scholar