The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×
Letter to the EditorFull Access

More Questions About Recovered Memories

A recent article (1) purported to “provide further evidence supporting the occurrence of amnesia for childhood traumatic experiences and the subsequent recovery of memory” (p. 754). In a key but methodologically problematic finding, “[a] majority of participants were able to find strong corroboration of their recovered memories” (p. 749). This “strikingly high” corroboration rate, however, was based on self-reported information recalled by the participants and accepted as recounted. Among 19 participants claiming complete amnesia who had attempted to confirm memories of sexual abuse, 89% (N=17) provided “corroborations” consisting of their memory of “verbal validation” alone (p. 753).

Although the authors acknowledged that a “major methodological limitation” of the study was the fact that “retrospective…self-reports were potentially subject to distortion and inaccuracies” (p. 754), there was no assessment describing the nature and quality of the self-reported corroborations, which would appear crucial to drawing conclusions about the veridicality of the recovered memories. Since retrospective verbal self-reports might have included pseudocorroborations representing confirmation bias, suggestion and belief paradigms, situational demand characteristics, and source amnesia (2, 3), the high corroboration rates could bespeak pseudomemories or screen memories masking other trauma (4).

Furthermore, even if “grossly improper therapeutic practices” (1, p. 754) were not a significant factor in memory recovery, unintended suggestive influences within the study itself may have biased the findings. Participants were asked “if there was a period during which they ‘did not remember that this [traumatic] experience happened’” (p. 751). With this question alone, the actuality of the traumatic experience was inherently validated by the investigators, and the experience of not remembering it was implicitly suggested. The fact that participants were recruited from a unit specializing in the treatment of posttraumatic and dissociative disorders could mean that suggestive influences and affiliative needs swayed group answers (3, p. 58). Questions about the “circumstances of first recovered memory” (1, p. 751) may have elicited autosuggestive responses. There apparently were no control questions or conditions. Ordinarily, patients might be confused about whether their recall of early traumatic experience is veridical (2, 4), yet the report does not indicate if participants ever had any doubt whether the events of the recovered memories actually occurred as remembered.

That “the vast majority of participants…did not recall any overt suggestion before the first recovered memory” (1, p. 752) does not rule out direct or indirect suggestive influence, whether inside or outside therapy sessions (2). Reading popular books, viewing or reading media, or talking with others on the subject of recovered memory may have influenced recollection. The actual time of suggestive effect could have followed the recalled time of recovery, which unwittingly may have been temporally displaced for narrative consistency.

These comments do not dispute the possibility of amnesia for traumatic experience that is later recalled or the discovery of information that confirms the veridicality of the memory. Consistent with suitable clinical technique, the retrieval of independent data is essential for investigating the objective-versus-subjective truth of early memories (5). However, without corroborative detail for readers to trace the study’s conclusions, generalizations about recovered memories hinging solely on self-reported “actual independent confirmation” (p. 753) should be viewed with scientific skepticism.

References

1. Chu JA, Frey LM, Ganzel BL, Matthews JA: Memories of childhood abuse: dissociation, amnesia, and corroboration. Am J Psychiatry 1999; 156:749–755AbstractGoogle Scholar

2. Good MI: Suggestion and veridicality in the reconstruction of sexual trauma, or can a bait of suggestion catch a carp of falsehood? J Am Psychoanal Assoc 1996; 44:1189–1224Google Scholar

3. Brenneis CB: Recovered Memories of Trauma: Transferring the Present to the Past. Madison, Conn, International Universities Press, 1997Google Scholar

4. Good MI: Screen reconstructions: traumatic memory, conviction, and the problem of verification. J Am Psychoanal Assoc 1998; 46:149–183Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

5. Good MI: The reconstruction of early childhood trauma: fantasy, reality, and verification. J Am Psychoanal Assoc 1994; 42:79–101Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar