The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×

Cognitive deficits are widely believed to limit recovery in schizophrenia ( 1 ). Attempts at remediation have focused on attention, memory, and problem solving, with little or no emphasis on social-cognitive difficulties ( 2 ). Although modest neurocognitive training effects have been found, little evidence exists regarding their durability. How neuropsychological change is "linked" to changes in behavior, beyond simple correlations, is even less understood ( 3 ).

Editor's note: This special section, focused on severe and persistent mental disorders, is dedicated to the memory of Wayne Fenton, Gerard Hogarty, and Ian Falloon, who died this past year. All three were major contributors to the literature on psychosocial treatment of people with these disorders. Hogarty, who was posthumously awarded the Alexander Gralnick Research Investigator Award by the American Psychiatric Association this year, is remembered with a paper in this section that he submitted to us last fall and with a Taking Issue tribute by Anthony Lehman and Thomas McGlashan. Falloon is also a coauthor of another article in this section.

These leaders made other important contributions to Psychiatric Services. Wayne Fenton, a devoted member of our editorial board, reviewed all but one of the articles in this section, and Ian Falloon reviewed another of the papers. In each instance they had criticisms, but in each case they eventually supported publication, providing invaluable advice to the authors and to the editor. Without their ultimate enthusiasm for these papers, we might not have published them.

A selection of their comments, listed below, illustrates the importance of sage advice in the review process.—Howard H. Goldman, M.D., Ph.D., Editor, Psychiatric Services

"Pragmatic studies of this kind are essential in the process of disseminating evidence-based practice." (Falloon)

"This is an excellent piece of work. Cognitive enhancement therapy has not received as much attention as it deserves." (Fenton)

"This is a unique contribution that demonstrates that it is possible to provide humane care for nearly all (if not all) people with mental illness in a catchment area without relying upon a large institution." (Fenton)

"The overall emphasis on positive and supportive influences is a welcome antidote to the expressed emotion perspective." (Fenton)

Cognitive enhancement therapy (CET) is a holistic, small-group approach (six to eight patients) to the remediation of neurocognitive and social-cognitive deficits among patients with schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorder in the postacute, recovery phase of illness. In a randomized trial, CET was compared with a state-of-the-art enriched supportive therapy (EST) ( 4 ). Over two years of treatment, CET demonstrated significant differential effects on composite indices of processing speed, neurocognition, cognitive style, social cognition, and social adjustment. (There was no difference on a residual-symptom composite measure because most patients were clinically stable at baseline and all were provided EST illness management strategies.)

For this follow-up study, we hypothesized that the effects of two years of CET would be sustained for at least one year posttreatment. Further, we investigated whether a possible mechanism or "mediator" for these effects could be identified. Historically, there has been an unfortunate tendency for many psychosocial treatment effects in schizophrenia to end once the active treatment ends ( 5 ). Among cognitive rehabilitation approaches, the maintenance of effects appears more encouraging, but the evidence is limited. Of the 17 cognitive rehabilitation studies recently reviewed by Twamley and colleagues ( 2 ), only three follow-up reports were identified. A three-week posttreatment assessment of an eight-session approach ( 6 ) and a 48-hour follow-up of a single training session ( 7 ) indicated that earlier improvement was sustained. A four-week follow-up of a problem-solving approach also showed that a differential effect persisted on an independent measure of problem solving ( 8 ). Since this review, other investigators have described the maintenance of a cognitive rehabilitation effect on a test of working memory at six months posttraining ( 9 ); however, no information was offered on the durability of other neuropsychological test effects that had been reported earlier ( 10 ). Additionally, a recent study of early-onset psychosis that controlled for IQ found an isolated effect of cognitive remediation on visual information processing among 14 participants one year after hospital discharge ( 11 ). However, no differential effects were reported during the controlled phase of treatment. Otherwise, the "delta" question raised by Green and Neuchterlein ( 3 ) remains a challenge: "whether changes in neurocognitive functioning are directly linked to changes in functional outcome." Reports have primarily described correlations between neurocognitive measures and behavior ( 1 ).

Methods

Participants and procedures

A description of the study design and two-year results ( 4 ), together with the theory ( 12 ), practice principles ( 13 ), and CET manual ( 14 ), are available elsewhere. Briefly, 121 patients with schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder were randomly assigned to either CET (N=67) or EST (N=54) and treated for two years, between January 1995 and February 2002. Patients' mean±SD age was 37.3±8.9 years. They had been ill for a mean of 15.7±9.3 years and had 5.96±5.97 previous hospitalizations. Most (99 patients, or 82 percent) had never been married. Seventy-one patients (59 percent) were male, 108 (89 percent) were white, and 13 (11 percent) were African American.

Patients were clinically stable at baseline and thus were at reduced risk of relapse ( 15 ). Eligibility required an IQ of at least 80, and the mean IQ of the patient group was 97.2±11.5. Although nearly one-third had completed college (38 patients, or 31 percent), most patients were not working (83 patients, or 69 percent). Participants had not worked for a median of four years, and 30 (25 percent) had not worked in more than ten years. All met criteria for social-cognitive and neurocognitive disability ( 4 ). [A Methods supplement to this article that describes the eligibility criteria related to cognitive style and social cognition is available on the journal's Web site at http://ps.psychiatryonline.org.]

Upon recruitment (baseline) participants were assessed with the measures described below. They were readministered at one and two years during treatment and again one year after treatment ended. Between months 4 and 6, four CET and four EST participants left the study, as did another four EST and two CET participants at 12 months ( 4 ). Of the 107 participants eligible for follow-up, data were collected for 106. (Because of missing data for some composite measures, the range of participants was 100 to 104.) On the key characteristics examined, these 106 participants did not differ from the original 121 participants at baseline. (It was deemed inappropriate to carry forward to 36 months the assessments of the 14 patients who left the study during the first year.)

All patients provided informed consent, and the study was reviewed annually by the University of Pittsburgh Institutional Review Board.

Treatment

CET is a multidimensional, developmental approach to the remediation of social-cognitive and neurocognitive deficits that was influenced by Brenner and colleagues' work ( 16 ) with patients with schizophrenia and by the work of Ben-Yishay and colleagues ( 17 ) among patients with traumatic brain injury. CET attempts to increase mental stamina, active rather than passive information processing, and spontaneous and appropriate negotiation of unrehearsed social challenges. Specifically, CET provides experiential exercises designed to facilitate the ability to take the perspective of other people (that is, to determine their thoughts, feelings, and likely behavior) by utilizing verbal and nonverbal cues. Perspective taking is the linchpin around which key aspects of social cognition are addressed: appraising the social context (beyond first impressions), judging affect in others, regulating one's own affect, reevaluating previous interpersonal encounters, using reciprocity, and forming shared understandings.

Through the course of approximately 56 social-cognitive group sessions of 90 minutes each ( 14 ), patients are challenged to think abstractly about one or more of these concepts. CET attempts to shift a reliance on concrete cognitive processing to a "gistful" abstraction of relationship themes ( 18 ). Unlike cognitive-behavioral therapy that seeks to correct faulty self-schemas (particularly regarding the nature of persistent hallucinations or delusions), CET addresses incorrect or incomplete cognitive schemas about other people. CET approaches neurocognitive deficits through approximately 75 one-hour sessions using computer exercises from the attention software of Ben-Yishay and colleagues ( 19 ) and the memory and problem-solving software of Bracy ( 20 ). All CET participants began neurocognitive remediation before entering the social-cognitive group (usually within six months of beginning neurocognitive training).

EST relied on most practice principles of the basic and intermediate phases of the demonstrably effective personal therapy approach, which fosters illness management through psychoeducation and control of stress ( 21 ). All participants received stress reduction strategies and education, either individually (EST) or through the social-cognitive group (CET), and all were maintained on antipsychotic medication approved by the U.S. Food and Drug Administration. Both treatments were supervised for fidelity. No differences were found between treatment groups in the type or dosage of medication or clinician-rated compliance. Three posttreatment social-cognitive group follow-up sessions were offered to CET participants in the year after the active phase of treatment, primarily for staff to learn about CET successes or implementation difficulties.

Measures

Highly reliable, multivariate composite measures were constructed from new and existing scales ( 4 ). [A Methods supplement to this article that describes the construction of the composite measures and specific scales is available on the journal's Web site at http://ps.psychiatryonline.org.] Four behavioral composite measures were derived from clinicians who were not blind to patients' treatment group and from patient ratings. Cognitive style was assessed using our newly developed Cognitive Style Inventory and clinical ratings on our cognitive style eligibility criteria ( 14 ); these instruments assess aspects of impoverished, disorganized, and rigid patterns of thinking. The assessment of social cognition included factor score measures of self-confidence, social perception, supportiveness, and tolerance from our newly developed Social Cognition Profile ( 14 ) as well as global measures of vocational and interpersonal effectiveness, foresight, and adjustment to disability taken from our social-cognitive eligibility criteria ( 14 ).

Social adjustment represented broad assessments of employment, major role performance, and overall adjustment as assessed by the Major Role Adjustment Inventory ( 22 ), Global Assessment Scale ( 23 ), and selected measures from the Social Security Administration's functional disability criteria ( 24 ). Symptom measures included patients' ratings of mood and self-esteem, as well as selected factors from the Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale ( 25 ) and total scores from the Raskin Depression ( 26 ) and Wing Negative Symptom ( 27 ) scales. All measures for these behavioral composite indices were completed by clinicians trained in their use who were not blind to patients' treatment group.

Two neuropsychological composite measures were derived from standard tests on which patients had not been trained. Processing speed relied on measures of visual scanning ability ( 19 ) and on simple reaction time ( 19 ) and choice reaction time ( 28 ). The neurocognition composite index included measures from the Wechsler Memory Scale ( 29 ), California Verbal Learning Test ( 30 ), Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised ( 31 ), Trails B ( 32 ), and Wisconsin Card Sorting Test ( 33 ). Neuropsychological tests were administered systematically by a psychometrician who was uninvolved in treatment. Clinicians were blind to the annual test results.

Data analysis

Maintenance of effects was tested by a stringent linear trend analysis that compared the amount and rate of improvement between the treatment groups over all three years. This trend analysis also tested for four potential moderators at baseline—sex, length of illness (greater or less than 15 years), psychosis level (greater or less than mild), and IQ (greater or less than 98)—to determine whether certain individuals benefited from treatment more than others. A moderator can indicate for whom a treatment works best. A mediator, on the other hand, might suggest a mechanism by which a treatment produces a favorable outcome ( 34 ). Such mechanisms of change were explored by the mediator analytic strategy of Kraemer and colleagues ( 34 ). This strategy is useful for identifying how improvements that occur during a treatment influence its later outcomes and points to the mechanisms by which a treatment achieves its effects.

In Kraemer and colleagues' framework, a mediator must occur before the observed changes in the outcome that is mediated; the mediator must be affected by treatment and must directly affect or interact with treatment to influence the mediated outcome. Full mediation is said to exist if the effects of treatment are completely reduced after changes in the mediator are accounted for. In turn, partial mediation exists if the effects of treatment are only partially reduced after accounting for the mediator, indicating that other changes that occur during treatment also influence outcome in addition to changes in the mediator ( 35 ). It is important to remember that not all mediators represent causal mechanisms of change ( 34 ). They do, however, provide the basis for future tests of a treatment that capitalize on the enhancement of efficacy of a possible mediator.

Results

Maintenance of effects

As shown in Table 1 , except for the neurocognition composite index, the four remaining composite measures that demonstrated a significant CET effect at two years ( 4 ) continued to be significant at 36 months. (Analyses of covariance of the 36-month data, which used baseline measures as the covariates, were also highly significant, except for differential improvement in neurocognition.) Visual inspection of the 36-month means shown in Table 1 also reaffirms the maintenance of these effects, with two-year CET effects on cognition and behavior enduring but not significantly improving after the end of treatment, compared with the group receiving EST. In terms of the nonsignificant neurocognition composite, CET participants did not deteriorate between years 2 and 3; rather, neurocognitive functioning of EST participants continued to improve over time despite the absence of cognitive training. As was true at one and two years, no differential treatment effect on the symptom composite was evident at three years.

Table 1 Maintenance of treatment effects among 67 patients with schizophrenia assigned to cognitive enhancement therapy (CET) or 54 assigned to enriched supportive therapy (EST)
Table 1 Maintenance of treatment effects among 67 patients with schizophrenia assigned to cognitive enhancement therapy (CET) or 54 assigned to enriched supportive therapy (EST)
Enlarge table

Other "real-world" main and moderator effects

When attendance in the three CET follow-up sessions during the year after treatment was adjusted for, the between-group difference in engagement in a social, recreational, or therapeutic group activity during year 3 was significant. Eighteen CET participants (30 percent) but only four EST participants (9 percent) engaged in these activities ( χ2 =6.92, df=1, p= .009). The difference in engagement in some type of vocational rehabilitation experience was also significant—24 CET participants (40 percent) compared with seven EST participants (16 percent) ( χ2 =7.38, df=1, p= .007). Although no significant group differences were found in the proportion of patients who had paid employment, 16 CET participants (27 percent) but only two EST recipients (4 percent) performed in a volunteer role during year 3, ( χ2 =10.25, df=2, p=.006). Because patients were at reduced risk of relapse, differences in relapse rates were not expected or observed over the three years; nine EST and five CET patients relapsed. Receipt of clozapine (N=47), a second-generation antipsychotic (N=41), or a first-generation antipsychotic (N=12) was nearly identical for the EST and CET samples during year 3. The groups did not differ in the number of medication visits, clinician-rated medication compliance, or the number of individual psychotherapy sessions or family sessions.

We found evidence for moderator effects of IQ, length of illness, and psychosis level, but not sex, across several cognitive and behavioral outcomes. Although no significant main effect was found for neurocognition at three years, a significant interaction was noted between treatment and IQ (F=4.27, df=1 and 99, p=.042), indicating that the neurocognitive deficits of participants with higher IQs (≥98) improved more with CET than with EST.

Among patients who had been ill for less than 15 years, CET had a greater effect than EST on ability to shop for basic necessities (F=5.34, df=1 and 87, p=.023) and a marginally greater effect in regard to household cleaning (F=3.79, df=1 and 87, p=.055), as assessed by the Social Security Administration's standards for functional disability ( 24 ), as well as significantly greater effects on patients' adjustment to their disability (F=8.43, df=1 and 89, p=.005) and on impoverished thinking (F=12.91, df= 1 and 89, p=.001). Among patients who were less psychotic at baseline, those who received CET also were better at managing finances than those who received EST (F=4.37, df=1 and 87, p=.039).

Mechanism of action

Using the criteria of Kraemer and colleagues ( 34 ), we first sought to identify potential mediator variables that improved early with CET and thus preceded significant changes in other long-term outcome measures. The neuropsychological composites—processing speed and neurocognition—were the only measures to improve with CET at a highly significant level (p<.01) during year 1 ( 4 ), before the highly significant (p<.01) CET effects on social cognition, cognitive style, and social adjustment that occurred during years 2 and 3 ( 4 ) ( Table 1 ). (We did not consider the symptom composite because it showed no treatment effect at any rating period.) Improvement in processing speed immediately followed attention training at six months and persisted for the next 2.5 years. EST participants did not improve on processing speed ( Table 1 ). Nearly all improvement in processing speed among CET patients occurred by 12 months. This regressed change score in processing speed became the first "mediator" candidate. To a lesser extent, the same pattern was true for the neurocognition composite measure (memory, problem solving, and executive functions), because although neurocognitive training did not begin until six months after intake, the most significant CET improvement in neurocognition (p< .003) also occurred at 12 months ( 4 ). This first-year regressed change score in neurocognition became the second potential mediator of long-term treatment outcome.

Next, we needed to show that these potential mediators had a significant effect on the behavioral composite indices at three years while accounting for the effects of CET. We accomplished this by examining the main effects of the mediators on three-year behavioral outcomes, after controlling for treatment effects. Early improvement in processing speed (year 1) had a significant effect on year 3 social cognition (F=4.25, df=1 and 98, p=.042) and social adjustment (F=4.16, df=1 and 98; p=.044) but not on cognitive style. Early improvement in neurocognition had no effect on long-term social adjustment and only a marginal effect on social cognition (F=3.89; df=1 and 97, p=.051). Its primary main effect occurred, as one might expect, on cognitive style (F=5.62, df=1 and 97, p=.020).

After finding that early changes in processing speed and neurocognition indeed mediated the long-term behavioral effects of CET, we next evaluated the extent of this mediation. Data presented in Table 2 indicate that changes in processing speed and neurocognition partially mediated the effect of CET on social cognition, social adjustment, and cognitive style. However, processing speed was the stronger "partial mediator" because the effect sizes of CET (Cohen's d) were reduced substantially when the analysis controlled for improvement in early processing speed. (However, CET continued to have its own statistically significant effect on outcome beyond that accounted for by processing speed.) Early improvement in neurocognition was also a partial mediator, but it did not lessen or mediate the long-term CET effect on outcome nearly as much as processing speed.

Table 2 Outcome effects of cognitive enhancement therapy (CET) at 36 months, with and without neuropsychological mediators
Table 2 Outcome effects of cognitive enhancement therapy (CET) at 36 months, with and without neuropsychological mediators
Enlarge table

Discussion

This study found that the effects of CET were broadly maintained. With the exception of the improvement in processing speed that was evident early and exclusively among CET recipients, both treatment groups continued to improve on other composite measures during the follow-up year, which provides continuing support for the effects of personal therapy on adjustment ( 21 ). In terms of the improvement in neurocognition observed for EST participants, Spaulding and colleagues ( 36 ) suggested that control of stress and arousal might lower state-related neurocognitive deficits. Recent evidence shows that stress-related hormone levels are indeed associated with neurocognitive performance ( 37 ). Although one cannot rule out practice effects on the neuropsychological tests as a cause of improvement among participants in the EST group, such effects are unlikely when evaluations are conducted annually.

That psychological techniques to help individuals manage stress and regulate affect might independently improve their neurocognitive functioning provides a challenge to cognitive rehabilitation interventions that rely exclusively on training in memory, problem solving, or executive functions or that exclusively use novel medications to target cognitive deficits. CET effects on "real-world" behaviors (for example, employment and social group participation) continued to be modest during follow-up, largely because patients had found their adjustment niche before treatment and were not inclined to vocational pursuits that might jeopardize disability benefits. Rather, most sought CET as a way to improve their quality of life.

In terms of possible mediators, improvement in processing speed (or attention) might be the necessary condition for a maximum behavioral response to future cognitive rehabilitation interventions. Other investigators have demonstrated the importance of addressing attention deficits ( 38 , 39 , 40 ). As previously noted in the initial CET study ( 4 ), CET effect sizes on cognition and behavior appear to exceed the average effect sizes reported for other cognitive rehabilitation approaches ( 2 ) and medication approaches ( 41 ).

Although there has been informative discussion about possible cognitive mediators ( 3 , 42 , 43 ), we know of no formal mediator analysis of psychosocial treatment outcomes that used neuropsychological variables. Overall, it appears that maximum behavioral change lags behind neuropsychological change, as suggested elsewhere ( 3 ). Given that improvement in processing speed was a strong partial mediator of both social cognition and social adjustment, we can now propose that early improvement in processing speed might be the condition upon which greater-than-expected treatment improvement in these areas depends.

Some models have indicated that changes in basic neurocognition mediate functional outcomes ( 3 ). We feel that processing speed (and the associated components of attention) could profitably be separated from memory, problem solving, perception, and executive functions. It is increasingly recognized that attention is a mental state upon which other cognitive operations depend ( 44 ). Unfortunately, our attention measure proved to be unreliable ( 4 ), and we were reduced to four measures of simple reaction time and choice reaction time as proxy measures of vigilance and selective attention. Processing speed, however, has long been viewed as a precursor to learning ( 45 ), and its unequivocal improvement among CET participants is encouraging.

Neurocognitive improvement had some mediating influence on outcome but not nearly as much as we anticipated. Neurocognitive training might well improve impoverished, disorganized, or inflexible thinking, but we found that the ability of neurocognitive change to independently influence social adjustment longitudinally was modest. It could be argued that early improvement in processing speed accounts for later changes in neurocognition. Because the CET effect on the neurocognition composite at year 3 was not significant, this mediator analysis would be inappropriate because there was essentially no clear effect to be mediated ( 35 ). The issue is further complicated because neurocognitive improvement likely arises from two distinct sources: computer training (remediation) among CET participants and stress reduction strategies (compensation) among all participants. A different study that separates remediation and compensation strategies is needed to answer this question.

Finally, CET was developed for clinically stable but functionally disabled patients in the postacute, recovery phase of illness. Among these patients, neuropsychological and behavioral changes appear to follow a longitudinal course that is largely independent of symptom improvement. The same might not be true for acutely symptomatic patients who receive cognitive training.

A replication of these observations in an ongoing study of CET among patients in the early course of schizophrenia ( 46 ) is eagerly awaited. Whether CET is effective for other mental disorders that implicate social-cognitive deficits in recovery remains an important question for clinicians and researchers.

Conclusions

In this study of the long-term, posttreatment effects of CET, results indicate that most of the effects of the highly efficacious CET on cognition and behavior were maintained one year after treatment ended. Early improvements in processing speed were found to be a partial mediator of these effects, which provides a putative mechanism of action for the effects of CET on long-term behavioral outcomes to guide future clinical trials.

Acknowledgments

The study was supported by grant MH-30750 from the National Institute of Mental Health. The authors thank the former Environmental-Personal Indicators in the Course of Schizophrenia (EPICS) Research Group staff for providing assessments; Richard Ulrich, M.S., for conducting the linear trend analysis; and Susan Hogarty, M.S.N., for helpful comments.

Dr. Greenwald and Mr. Eack are affiliated with the Department of Psychiatry, University of Pittsburgh Medical Center, where the late Mr. Hogarty was also affiliated. Send correspondence to Mr. Eack at the Department of Psychiatry, University of Pittsburgh Medical Center, 3811 O'Hara Street, Pittsburgh, PA 15213 (e-mail: [email protected]). This article is part of a special section honoring the memory of three leaders in the psychosocial treatment of patients with severe and persistent mental disorders: Wayne S. Fenton, M.D., Gerard E. Hogarty, M.S.W., and Ian R. H. Falloon, M.D., D.Sc.

References

1. Green MF, Kern RS, Braff DL, et al: Neurocognitive deficits and functional outcome in schizophrenia: are we measuring the "right stuff"? Schizophrenia Bulletin 26:119-136, 2000Google Scholar

2. Twamley EW, Jeste DV, Bellack AS: A review of cognitive training in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin 29:359-382, 2003Google Scholar

3. Green MF, Nuechterlein KH: Should schizophrenia be treated as a neurocognitive disorder? Schizophrenia Bulletin 25:309-319, 1999Google Scholar

4. Hogarty GE, Flesher S, Ulrich R, et al: Cognitive enhancement therapy for schizophrenia: effects of a 2-year randomized trial on cognition and behavior. Archives of General Psychiatry 61:866-876, 2004Google Scholar

5. Hogarty GE, Anderson CM, Reiss DJ, et al: Family psychoeducation, social skills training, and maintenance chemotherapy in the aftercare treatment of schizophrenia: two-year effects of a controlled study on relapse and adjustment. Archives of General Psychiatry 48:340-347, 1991Google Scholar

6. Meichenbaum D, Cameron R: Training schizophrenics to talk to themselves. Behavior Therapy 4:515-534, 1973Google Scholar

7. Corrigan PW, Hirschbeck JN, Wolfe M: Memory and vigilance training to improve social perception in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 17:257-265, 1995Google Scholar

8. Medalia A, Revheim N, Casey M: Remediation of problem-solving skills in schizophrenia: evidence of a persistent effect. Schizophrenia Research 57:165-171, 2002Google Scholar

9. Bell M, Bryson G, Wexler BE: Cognitive remediation of working memory deficits: durability of training effects in severely impaired and less severely impaired schizophrenia. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 108:101-109, 2003Google Scholar

10. Bell MP, Bryson GJ, Corcoran C, et al: Neurocognitive enhancement therapy with work therapy: effects on neuropsychological test performance. Archives of General Psychiatry 58:763-768, 2001Google Scholar

11. Ueland T, Rund BR: Cognitive remediation for adolescents with early onset psychosis: a 1-year follow-up study. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 108:101-109, 2003Google Scholar

12. Hogarty GE, Flesher S: A developmental theory for a cognitive enhancement therapy of schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin 25:677-692, 1999Google Scholar

13. Hogarty GE, Flesher S: Practice principles of cognitive enhancement therapy. Schizophrenia Bulletin 25:693-708, 1999Google Scholar

14. Hogarty GE, Greenwald DP: Cognitive Enhancement Therapy: The Training Manual. Pittsburgh, Western Psychiatric Institute and Clinic, 2006. Available at www.cognitiveenhancementtherapy.comGoogle Scholar

15. Hogarty GE, Ulrich RF: Temporal effects of drug and placebo in delaying the relapse of schizophrenic patients. Archives of General Psychiatry 34:297-301, 1977Google Scholar

16. Brenner HD, Hodel B, Roder V, et al: Treatment of cognitive dysfunctions and behavior deficits in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin 18:21-26, 1992Google Scholar

17. Ben-Yishay Y, Rattok J, Lakin P, et al: Neuropsychological rehabilitation: quest for a holistic approach. Seminars in Neurology 5:252-259, 1985Google Scholar

18. Brainerd CJ, Reyna VF: Gist is the grist: fuzzy trace theory and the new intuitionism. Developmental Review 10:3-47, 1990Google Scholar

19. Ben-Yishay Y, Piasetsky EB, Rattok J: A systematic method for ameliorating disorders in basic attention, in Neuropsychological Rehabilitation. Edited by Meir MJ, Benton AL, Diller L. New York, Guilford, 1985Google Scholar

20. Bracy O: CET Computer Assisted Training Exercises. Indianapolis, Neuroscience Center of Indianapolis, 2003. Available at www.neuroscience.cnter.com/pss/psscogrehab/index.htmGoogle Scholar

21. Hogarty GE: Personal Therapy for Schizophrenia and Related Disorders: A Guide to Individualized Treatment. New York, Guilford, 2002Google Scholar

22. Hogarty GE, Goldberg SC, Schooler NR: Drug and sociotherapy in the aftercare of schizophrenic patients. III: adjustment of nonrelapsed patients. Archives of General Psychiatry 31:609-618, 1974Google Scholar

23. Endicott J, Spitzer RL, Fleiss JL, et al: The Global Assessment Scale: a procedure for measuring overall severity of psychiatric disturbance. Archives of General Psychiatry 33:766-771, 1976Google Scholar

24. Mental disorders—adult, in Disability Evaluation Under Social Security. SSA pub no 64-039. Washington, DC, US Department of Health and Human Services, 1986Google Scholar

25. Overall JE, Gorham DR: The Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale. Psychological Report 10:799-812, 1962Google Scholar

26. Raskin A, Schulterbrandt J, Reatig N, et al: Replication of factors of psychopathology in interview, ward behavior, and self-report ratings of hospitalized depressives. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 148:87-98, 1969Google Scholar

27. Wing JK: A simple and reliable subclassification of chronic schizophrenia. JAMA 107:862-875, 1961Google Scholar

28. PSSCogReHab program. Indianapolis, Psychological Software Services, Inc, 2003. Available at http://www.neuroscience.cnter.com/pss/index.htmGoogle Scholar

29. Wechsler D: Manual for the Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised. San Antonio, Tex, Psychological Corp, 1987Google Scholar

30. Delis DC, Kramer JH, Kaplan E, et al: California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT) Manual. San Antonio, Tex, Psychological Corp, 1987Google Scholar

31. Wechsler D: Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised. New York, Psychological Corp, 1981Google Scholar

32. Reitan RM, Waltson D: The Halstead-Reitan Neuropsychological Test Battery. Tucson, Ariz, Neuropsychology Press, 1985Google Scholar

33. Heaton RK, Chelune GJ, Talley JL, et al: Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Manual: Revised and Expanded. Odessa, Fla, Psychological Assessment Resources, 1993Google Scholar

34. Kraemer HC, Wilson T, Fairburn CG, et al: Mediators and moderators of treatment effects in randomized clinical trials. Archives of General Psychiatry 59:877-883, 2002Google Scholar

35. Baron RM, Kenny DA: The moderate-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 51:1173-1183, 1986Google Scholar

36. Spaulding WD, Storms L, Goodrich V, et al: Applications of experimental psychopathology in psychiatric rehabilitation. Schizophrenia Bulletin 12:560-577, 1986Google Scholar

37. Halari R, Kumari V, Mehrotra R, et al: The relationship of sex hormones and cortisol with cognitive functioning in schizophrenia. Journal of Psychopharmacology 18:366-374, 2004Google Scholar

38. Medalia A, Aluma M, Tryon W, et al: Effectiveness of attention training in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin 24:147-152, 1998Google Scholar

39. Silverstein SM, Menditto AA, Stuve P: Shaping attention span: an operant conditioning procedure to improve neurocognition and functioning in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin 27:247-257, 2001Google Scholar

40. Wykes T, Stuart E, Katz R: The prediction of rehabilitation success after three years: the use of social, symptom and cognitive measures. British Journal of Psychiatry 157:865-870, 1990Google Scholar

41. Bilder RM, Goldman RS, Volavka J, et al: Neurocognitive effects of clozapine, olanzapine, risperidone, and haloperidol in patients with chronic schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry 159:1118-1028, 2002Google Scholar

42. Spaulding WD, Fleming SK, Reed D, et al: Cognitive functioning in schizophrenia: implications for psychiatric rehabilitation. Schizophrenia Bulletin 25:275-289, 1999Google Scholar

43. Wykes T, Reader C, Corner J, et al: The effects of neurocognitive remediation on executive processing in patients with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin 25:291-307, 1999Google Scholar

44. Levitt T, Johnstone B: The assessment and rehabilitation of attention disorders, in Rehabilitation of Neuropsychological Disorders. Edited by Johnstone B, Stonnington HH. Philadelphia, Psychology Press, 2001Google Scholar

45. Ackerman PL: Individual differences in skill learning: an integration of psychometric and information processing perspectives. Psychological Bulletin 102:3-27, 1987Google Scholar

46. Keshavan M: Rehabilitation, Brain Function and Early Schizophrenia. National Institute of Mental Health grant MH-60902, 2005Google Scholar