The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×
EditorialsFull Access

The Eyes as Window to the Mind

In this issue, Moriuchi et al. report on a fascinating study (1) on a long-standing question: Why do children with autism look less at other people’s eyes, relative to typically developing children? The authors pit two theories against each other: gaze aversion and gaze indifference. Their results point to the latter, not the former, although the question of why remains.

One now quite old idea is that children with autism don’t see other people’s eyes as a window to the mind (2). Their well-established difficulties in attributing mental states to other people may mean that they see other people’s eyes as parts within the face that move, but they struggle to interpret what these moving parts mean. Whereas the typical child pays a lot of attention to where others are looking because they understand that what someone looks at indicates their mental state (what that person is interested in), children with autism may not appreciate the mentalistic significance of a person’s eyes. Nor might people with autism understand that a person’s eyes are a reflection of a range of mental states—cognitive, volitional, or affective.

Consider how when we try to interpret a person’s basic emotions, such as happy, sad, angry, afraid, disgust, or surprise (Figure 1A), people with or without autism score equally well if they are presented with the eyes or the mouth, although seeing the whole face leads to better performance. But when the task is to judge a complex emotion, such as arrogant, guilt, thoughtful, or revenge (Figure 1B), seeing the eyes alone results in as good performance as seeing the whole face, and both eyes and whole face are better than the mouth alone. Moreover, on such challenging tests, adults with Asperger’s syndrome show impaired performance when shown just the eyes.

FIGURE 1.

FIGURE 1. Examples of Basic and Complex Emotional Stimulia

a Panels A and B show full face, eye, and mouth stimuli for basic and complex emotions, respectively. From Baron-Cohen et al. (3).

Using the Reading the Mind in the Eyes test, collecting data online, our group found that the impairment in autism is very clear, as is the sex difference (female superiority) in typical participants and the absence of any sex difference in autism (4), confirming earlier studies (5, 6) (Figure 2).

FIGURE 2.

FIGURE 2. Results From a Study in Which the “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” Test Was Administered to Males and Females With and Without Autisma

a Data from Baron-Cohen et al. (4). Panel A shows the mean scores on the test for males and females with and without autism. Panel B is a sample item from the test; participants were asked to select which word best describes what the pictured individual is thinking or feeling.

Performance on the Eyes test is associated with activity in the amygdala and in the left inferior frontal gyrus (7, 8); the female advantage on the test is associated with greater activity in the left inferior frontal gyrus, and the autistic deficit is associated with reduced activity in this same area. This deficit is also seen in parents of such children, suggesting that it arises for genetic reasons (9, 10). Lesion studies confirm the importance of these brain regions in mentalizing the eyes (11).

Candidate gene studies suggest that sex steroid pathway genes play a role in performance on the Eyes test (12), and a whole genome study of more than 88,000 people (13) found an association between performance on the Eyes test and chromosome 3p26.1 in typical females. The heritable aspect of mentalizing the eyes is apparent in at-risk infants who are siblings of children with autism (14). A unique study (15) also showed that performance on the Eyes test is negatively associated with levels of prenatal testosterone, which may explain the typical sex differences on this test. Administering testosterone to typical women reduces their scores on the Eyes test to typical male levels (16) and reduces brain functional connectivity (17). Administering the hormone oxytocin seems to have the opposite effect, improving scores on the Eyes test (18). Naturally, individual differences in reading the mind in the eyes are not just a function of our biology, as postnatal social experience likely amplifies these prenatal determinants. For this reason, we should expect that impaired performance on this test might be observed in a range of clinical groups, not just in autism, for diverse reasons.

From the Autism Research Centre, Cambridge University, Cambridge, U.K.
Address correspondence to Dr. Baron-Cohen ().

The author reports no financial relationships with commercial interests.

References

1 Moriuchi JM, Klin A, Jones W: Mechanisms of diminished attention to eyes in autism. Am J Psychiatry 2017; 174:26–35LinkGoogle Scholar

2 Baron-Cohen S: Perceptual role taking and protodeclarative pointing in autism. Br J Dev Psychol 1989; 7:113–127CrossrefGoogle Scholar

3 Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Jolliffe T: Is there a “language of the eyes”? Evidence from normal adults and adults with autism or Asperger syndrome. Vis Cogn 1997; 4:311–331CrossrefGoogle Scholar

4 Baron-Cohen S, Bowen DC, Holt RJ, et al.: The “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” test: complete absence of typical sex difference in ∼400 men and women with autism. PLoS One 2015; 10:e0136521Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

5 Baron-Cohen S, Jolliffe T, Mortimore C, et al.: Another advanced test of theory of mind: evidence from very high functioning adults with autism or Asperger syndrome. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1997; 38:813–822Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

6 Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Hill J, et al.: The “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” test revised version: a study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2001; 42:241–251Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

7 Baron-Cohen S, Ring HA, Wheelwright S, et al.: Social intelligence in the normal and autistic brain: an fMRI study. Eur J Neurosci 1999; 11:1891–1898Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

8 Holt RJ, Chura LR, Lai M-C, et al.: “Reading the Mind in the Eyes”: an fMRI study of adolescents with autism and their siblings. Psychol Med 2014; 44:3215–3227Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

9 Baron-Cohen S, Ring H, Chitnis X, et al.: fMRI of parents of children with Asperger syndrome: a pilot study. Brain Cogn 2006; 61:122–130Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

10 Baron-Cohen S, Hammer J: Parents of children with Asperger syndrome: what is the cognitive phenotype? J Cogn Neurosci 1997; 9:548–554Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

11 Shaw P, Bramham J, Lawrence EJ, et al.: Differential effects of lesions of the amygdala and prefrontal cortex on recognizing facial expressions of complex emotions. J Cogn Neurosci 2005; 17:1410–1419Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

12 Chakrabarti B, Dudbridge F, Kent L, et al.: Genes related to sex steroids, neural growth, and social-emotional behavior are associated with autistic traits, empathy, and Asperger syndrome. Autism Res 2009; 2:157–177Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

13 Warrier V, Grasby K, Uzefovsky F, et al.: Genome-wide meta-analysis of cognitive empathy: heritability, and correlates with sex, neuropsychiatric conditions, and brain anatomy. (bioRxiv preprint first posted online Oct 19, 2016)Google Scholar

14 Elsabbagh M, Mercure E, Hudry K, et al.: Infant neural sensitivity to dynamic eye gaze is associated with later emerging autism. Curr Biol 2012; 22:338–342Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

15 Chapman E, Baron-Cohen S, Auyeung B, et al.: Fetal testosterone and empathy: evidence from the empathy quotient (EQ) and the “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” test. Soc Neurosci 2006; 1:135–148Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

16 van Honk J, Schutter DJ, Bos PA, et al.: Testosterone administration impairs cognitive empathy in women depending on second-to-fourth digit ratio. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2011; 108:3448–3452Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

17 Bos PA, Hofman D, Hermans EJ, et al.: Testosterone reduces functional connectivity during the “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” test. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2016; 68:194–201Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

18 Guastella AJ, Einfeld SL, Gray KM, et al.: Intranasal oxytocin improves emotion recognition for youth with autism spectrum disorders. Biol Psychiatry 2010; 67:692–694Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar