The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×

To the Editor: Schizophrenia researchers wonder if schizophrenia is a neurodevelopmental or neurodegenerative condition. Data from our life-course study suggest it may be both. We previously reported that children who later develop schizophrenia show slowed growth in fluid cognitive abilities across ages 7 to 13 years (1), consistent with a neurodevelopmental model of schizophrenia. With new data at midlife (2), we think we have documented that these same individuals show some cognitive degeneration (i.e., loss of acquired cognitive abilities) from before to after the onset of schizophrenia.

Usually, the mental abilities of an individual are interpreted against a standard of performance that is normative, where normative is defined as age typical. Childhood and adult tests tap the same abilities, but they often use age-appropriate test materials. However, the clearest evidence that neuropsychological decline in schizophrenia represents a loss of acquired cognitive abilities would be seen if the test given at follow-up precisely matched the initially administered test, as Bora (3) notes. In the Dunedin Multidisciplinary Health and Development Study, two tests given in adulthood (age 38) were identical to those given in childhood (age 13): the Grooved Pegboard Test and the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test. Regarding the Grooved Pegboard Test, the schizophrenia group took 4 seconds longer in adulthood than in childhood to complete the same test; thus, the schizophrenia group lost some of the motor ability they once had in childhood. (In contrast, the healthy and persistent depression groups completed the test about 2.5 seconds more quickly in adulthood than in childhood, thus showing that motor abilities normatively improve slightly from childhood to adulthood.) Regarding the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test, the schizophrenia group recalled seven fewer words over the four trials (Rey total recall) as adults than they had as children on the same test; thus, the schizophrenia group lost some of their ability to learn. (In contrast, the healthy and persistent depression groups recalled three fewer words as adults than they had as children, thus showing that ability to learn normatively declines from childhood to adulthood. Individuals with schizophrenia showed accelerated decline in ability to learn.)

In summary, we previously demonstrated that in childhood, when the normative trajectory of cognitive ability is one of growth, children who later develop schizophrenia exhibit slowed growth in fluid cognitive abilities (1). Here, we demonstrate that in adulthood, when fluid cognitive abilities start to normatively decline (4), individuals with schizophrenia in our cohort exhibited early degeneration and accelerated decline. Granted, these latter findings are based on only two tests, but to us, they blur the distinction between development and degeneration. Moreover, unlike fluid abilities, deficits in crystallized abilities were apparent as early as age 7 and remained stable to age 38 years (1, 2). Fluid abilities are thought to support the acquisition of crystallized skills, with the developmental trajectory of crystallized abilities lagging behind that for fluid abilities (5). Therefore, it is somewhat surprising that deficits in crystallized abilities emerged before deficits in fluid abilities and did not worsen over time. Whether different pathophysiological mechanisms underlie cognitive deficits in fluid and crystallized abilities in schizophrenia is a key question for future research.

From the Department of Psychology, Arizona State University, Tempe, Ariz.; the Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, Duke University, Durham, N.C.; the Institute for Genome Sciences and Policy, Duke University, Durham; the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Duke University Medical Center, Durham; the Social, Genetic, and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London; and the Dunedin Multidisciplinary Health and Development Research Unit, Department of Preventive and Social Medicine, School of Medicine, University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand.

The authors’ disclosures accompany the original article.

References

1 Reichenberg A, Caspi A, Harrington H, Houts R, Keefe RS, Murray RM, Poulton R, Moffitt TE: Static and dynamic cognitive deficits in childhood preceding adult schizophrenia: a 30-year study. Am J Psychiatry 2010; 167:160–169LinkGoogle Scholar

2 Meier MH, Caspi A, Reichenberg A, Keefe RS, Fisher HL, Harrington H, Houts R, Poulton R, Moffitt TE: Neuropsychological decline in schizophrenia from the premorbid to the postonset period: evidence from a population-representative longitudinal study. Am J Psychiatry 2014; 171:91–101LinkGoogle Scholar

3 Bora E: Developmental lag and course of cognitive deficits from the premorbid to postonset period in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 2014; 171:369LinkGoogle Scholar

4 Salthouse T: Consequences of age-related cognitive declines. Annu Rev Psychol 2012; 63:201–226Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

5 Li SC, Lindenberger U, Hommel B, Aschersleben G, Prinz W, Baltes PB: Transformations in the couplings among intellectual abilities and constituent cognitive processes across the life span. Psychol Sci 2004; 15:155–163Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar