The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×

Abstract

OBJECTIVE: The 22q11.2 deletion syndrome (DiGeorge/velocardiofacial syndrome) is associated with attentional problems and executive dysfunction, and is one of the highest known risk factors for schizophrenia. These behavioral manifestations of 22q11.2 deletion syndrome could result from haploinsufficiency of the catechol O-methyltransferase (COMT) gene, located within the 22q11 region. The goal of the present study was to examine COMT genotype as a predictor of prefrontal cognitive function in patients with 22q11.2 deletion syndrome. METHOD: Patients with confirmed 22q11.2 deletions (N=44) underwent neurocognitive testing following Val158Met genotyping (Met hemizygous: N=16; Val hemizygous: N=28). RESULTS: Analyses of covariance revealed that Met-hemizygous patients performed significantly better on a composite measure of executive function (comprising set-shifting, verbal fluency, attention, and working memory) than did Val-hemizygous patients. CONCLUSIONS: These data are consistent with those of previous studies in normal individuals, suggesting that a functional genetic polymorphism in the 22q11 region may influence prefrontal cognition in individuals with COMT haploinsufficiency.

The enzyme catechol O-methyltransferase (COMT) is critical in the metabolic degradation of synaptic dopamine and norepinephrine (1), key neurotransmitters hypothesized to influence human cognitive function (2). The COMT gene contains a functional polymorphism (Val158Met) that determines high and low activity of this enzyme (1). Homozygosity for the low-activity (Met) allele is associated with a three- to fourfold reduction of COMT enzyme activity compared with homozygotes for the high-activity (Val) variant, resulting in reduced degradation of synaptic catecholamines in individuals with the Met allele (3). Recent evidence suggests that in both healthy volunteers and schizophrenia patients, the Met allele is associated with superior performance on measures of prefrontal cortical function (2, 4).

The 22q11.2 deletion syndrome (DiGeorge/velocardiofacial syndrome) results from a hemizygous deletion in chromosome 22 (5) and is characterized by dysmorphia, cleft palate, and cardiac anomalies (6). Patients also display a unique behavioral phenotype involving particular deficits in executive function, attention, and abstraction (7); visuospatial cognition (8); and elevated rates of ADHD and psychosis (911). Because the COMT gene maps to the deleted region, the characteristic behavioral manifestations of this syndrome may be related to dopamine dysregulation resulting from COMT haploinsufficiency (3). Moreover, it is unknown whether COMT genotype in the intact chromosome in patients with 22q11.2 deletion syndrome has a similar influence on executive cognition to that observed in other populations. While prior research has suggested an association between the Met allele and psychopathology in patients with 22q11.2 deletions (11), no previous work has investigated the effect of COMT polymorphism on neurocognitive function in this population. Thus, the goal of this research was to assess prefrontal cognition in relation to COMT genotype in patients with the 22q11.2 deletion syndrome.

Method

Participants were recruited through the Clinical Genetics Center at the Children’s Hospital of Philadelphia. Genetic diagnosis was confirmed by fluorescence in situ hybridization with the N25(D2S75) molecular probe. Complete neuropsychological and genotype data were available for 44 patients with 22q11.2 deletion syndrome (27 female, 17 male; mean age=11.1 years [SD=3.2]; Val hemizygous: N=28 [64%], Met hemizygous: N=16 [36%]). Forty-two participants were Caucasian, one was black, and one was Asian. All provided written informed consent/assent.

The neurocognitive battery included measures of general intellectual function, memory, language, attention, executive functions, and visuomotor skills. A complete description of the battery and test results are published in detail elsewhere (7, 8).

COMT genotype was determined by polymerase chain reaction/restriction fragment length polymorphism analysis, as described previously (1, 12).

The objective of our statistical analysis was to examine the association of COMT genotype with measures of prefrontal cognition. We selected the following executive function measures from our larger battery on the basis of previous factor analytic studies (13): verbal category fluency (animal naming), Trails B, WISC-3 Arithmetic, and Digit Span tasks. Variables were rescaled to z score equivalents, and an executive function domain score was computed by averaging the z scores on contributing variables. Two-tailed analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) was used to compare the two genotype groups (Met hemizygous versus Val hemizygous). Secondly, we examined individual executive function tests as predictors of allele status. Because there was a tendency toward higher full-scale IQ in Val-hemizygous patients than in Met-hemizygous patients (mean=77.6 [SD=10.5] versus 71.8 [SD=11.4], respectively; F=2.98, df=1, 42, p=0.09), statistical analyses controlled for full-scale IQ.

Results

Met-hemizygous and Val-hemizygous subjects were similar with regard to age, sex, and race. In the ANCOVA for executive function composite score, IQ was a highly significant covariate (F=32.48, df=1, 41, p<0.001). Notably, genotype remained a significant predictor of executive function score after effects of IQ were controlled, with Met-hemizygous patients performing better than Val-hemizygous patients (Figure 1). Met allele was associated with significantly better performance (after IQ was controlled) on the digit span task and superior performance on the Trails B test. Performance on verbal fluency and arithmetic tasks did not differ between groups. There was no main effect of gender nor a genotype-by-gender interaction.

In addition, because the digit span and arithmetic tasks contribute to the full-scale IQ measure, we recalculated our composite using only those tests unrelated to full-scale IQ (Trails B, verbal fluency) and obtained a similar result (F=3.40, df=1, 41, p=0.07).

Discussion

The principal finding of this study is that Met-hemizygous patients performed significantly better than Val-hemizygous patients on a composite measure of executive cognition. Post hoc analyses indicated that this difference was principally driven by performance on the digit span and Trails B tests. This finding appears qualitatively similar to that observed in individuals with no 22q11.2 deletion. However, because our executive function measure involved a broader range of tests than those used in previous studies, which examined the Wisconsin Card Sort as the only dependent measure (2, 4), it is unclear if COMT genotype explains a greater portion of variance in executive cognition in patients with 22q11.2 deletion syndrome.

In this study group there was a tendency toward higher full-scale IQ in Val-hemizygous patients. Because this relationship was not observed in previous studies of normal subjects (2, 4), we opted to control for it statistically. However, it is conceivable that this difference is representative of the 22q11.2 deletion syndrome population and thus worth considering in future samples.

To our knowledge, no previous study has investigated cognitive measures in relation to COMT genotype in patients with 22q11.2 deletion syndrome. Investigators have reported increased prevalence of schizophrenia associated with the deletion (9, 10) and identified schizophrenia susceptibility loci within the 22q11 region (14), although Murphy et al. (10) detected no association between COMT genotype and schizophrenia diagnosis in adults with 22q11.2 deletion syndrome. The functional implications of COMT haploinsufficiency are unclear, although patients should be particularly susceptible to the development of psychosis (via increased brain dopamine levels) if the nondeleted chromosome encodes the low-activity (Met) variant of COMT (3). This is in contrast with the reported association of Val allele load with impaired prefrontal function, and increased risk of schizophrenia (2). However, the actual effects of 22q11.2 deletion on catecholamine neurotransmission are not known. Future research should examine more direct measures of catecholaminergic turnover in order to quantify prefrontal dopamine uptake as a function of genotype in patients with 22q11.2 deletion syndrome.

Presented in part at the ninth International Congress for Schizophrenia Research, Colorado Springs, Colo., March 30–April 2, 2003. Received April 30, 2003; revision received Dec. 2, 2003; accepted Dec. 15, 2003. From the Children’s Hospital of Philadelphia Departments of Child Development, Biostatistics, Human Genetics and Molecular Biology, and Neurology; and the Department of Psychiatry, Hospital of the University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia. Address reprint requests to Dr. Bearden, UCLA Department of Psychiatry and Biobehavioral Sciences, 300 UCLA Medical Plaza, Room 2265, Los Angeles, CA 90095; (e-mail). Supported in part by NIH grant PO1-DC-02027 (Dr. Emanuel) and a Beeson Award from the American Federation for Aging Research (Dr. Lynch) The authors thank M.F. Woodin for assistance with data collection, J.R. Monterosso for comments on the manuscript, and the 22q11.2 deletion syndrome patients for their participation in the study.

Figure 1.

Figure 1. Executive Functioning in 44 Patients With 22q11.2 Deletion Syndrome Categorized by Genotype at the Val158Met Locus of the COMT Gene

aValues represent least estimate mean z-scores (with 95% confidence intervals) after full-scale IQ was controlled.

bSignificant between-group difference (F=5.03, df=1, 41, p<0.05).

cSignificant between-group difference (F=4.38, df=1, 41, p<0.05).

dDifference between groups approached significance (F=3.57, df=1, 41, p=0.07).

References

1. Lachman HM, Papolos DF, Saito T, Yu YM, Szumlanski CL, Weinshilboum RM: Human catechol-O-methyltransferase pharmacogenetics: description of a functional polymorphism and its potential application to neuropsychiatric disorders. Pharmacogenetics 1996; 6:243–250Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

2. Egan MF, Goldberg TE, Kolachana BS, Callicott JH, Mazzanti CM, Straub RE, Goldman D, Weinberger DR: Effect of COMT Val108/158Met genotype on frontal lobe function and risk for schizophrenia. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2001; 98:6917–6922Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

3. Graf WD, Unis AS, Yates CM, Sulzbacher S, Dinulos MB, Jack RM, Dugaw KA, Paddock MN, Parson WW: Catecholamines in patients with 22q11.2 deletion syndrome and the low-activity COMT polymorphism. Neurology 2001; 57:410–416Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

4. Malhotra AK, Kestler LJ, Mazzanti C, Bates JA, Goldberg T, Goldman D: A functional polymorphism in the COMT gene and performance on a test of prefrontal cognition. Am J Psychiatry 2002; 159:652–654LinkGoogle Scholar

5. Grossman MH, Emanuel BS, Budarf ML: Chromosomal mapping of the human catechol-O-methyltransferase gene to 22q11.1--q11.2. Genomics 1992; 12:822–825Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

6. McDonald-McGinn DM, LaRossa D, Randall P, Emanuel BS, Zackai EH: The 22q11.2 deletion: screening, diagnostic workup, and outcome. Genet Test 1997; 1:99–108Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

7. Woodin M, Wang PP, Aleman D, McDonald-McGinn D, Zackai E, Moss E: Neuropsychological profile of children and adolescents with the 22q11.2 microdeletion. Genet Med 2001; 3:34–39Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

8. Bearden CE, Woodin M, Wang PP, Moss E, McDonald-McGinn D, Zackai E, Emanuel B, Cannon TD: Neuropsychological function in the 22q deletion syndrome. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2001; 23:447–464Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

9. Bassett AS, Chow EW: 22q11 deletion syndrome: a genetic subtype of schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 1999; 46:882–891Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

10. Murphy KC, Jones LA, Owen MJ: High rates of schizophrenia in adults with velo-cardio-facial syndrome. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1999; 56:940–945Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

11. Papolos DF, Faedda GL, Veit S, Goldberg R, Morrow B, Kucherlapati R, Shprintzen RJ: Bipolar spectrum disorders in patients diagnosed with velo-cardio-facial syndrome: does a hemizygous deletion of chromosome 22q11 result in bipolar affective disorder? Am J Psychiatry 1996; 153:1541–1547LinkGoogle Scholar

12. Lynch DR, Mozley D, Sokol S, Maas NMC, Balcer LJ, Siderowf AD: Lack of effect of polymorphisms in dopamine metabolism related genes on imaging of TRODAT-1 in striatum of asymptomatic volunteers and patients with Parkinson’s disease. Movement Disord 2003; 18:804–812Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

13. Lezak M: Neuropsychological Assessment. New York, Oxford University Press, 1995Google Scholar

14. Karayiorgou M, Gogos JA, Galke BL, Wolyniec PS, Nestadt G, Antonarakis SE, Kazazian HH, Housman DE, Pulver AE: Identification of sequence variants and analysis of the role of the catechol-O-methyl-transferase gene in schizophrenia susceptibility. Biol Psychiatry 1998; 43:425–431Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar