The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×
Letter to the EditorFull Access

Checklist Psychiatry’s Effect on Psychiatric Education

To the Editor: In his questionnaire study of psychiatric residents’ understanding of mood and affect (1), Michael Serby, M.D., identified an important problem in modern-day American psychiatric education. However, his conclusions were unnecessarily conservative. Dr. Serby highlighted a problem that goes beyond mere semantics and conceptual confusion about a particular terminus technicus. His study points to a three-pronged structural flaw in psychiatric residency training (and thus in psychiatry in general): 1) undue and preeminent focus on patient self-reports of cross-sectional symptoms; 2) little, if any, reliance on the mental status examination; and 3) the consequent inability to make well-substantiated syndromal diagnoses. Too often, patients are permitted (perhaps encouraged) to simply enumerate symptoms rather than describe their experiences phenomenologically. In this enterprise, the psychiatrist is frequently an indictable coconspirator in equating the layman’s vernacular (e.g., “depressed,” “mood swings,” and “racing thoughts”) with an expert’s diagnostic jargon (2). Examples are not only limited to symptoms and experiences in the affective domain but include many more (e.g., paranoia, obsession, anxiety, and trauma). In essence, the patient tells the physician what his or her diagnosis is.

Unfortunately, there is no easy solution since this state of affairs is the direct result of teaching psychiatry by means of a coding manual—DSM—as opposed to teaching the descriptive psychopathology of clinical syndromes (that only later are coded according to some consensus scheme). While obviously not intended by the DSM’s framers, this document renders symptom hunting by checklist the modus operandi of the modern American psychiatrist. A critical mass of stereotypically worded symptoms is elicited or solicited and seldom merged with an understanding of the patient’s actual subjective experience and ostensibly objective mental status. Of course, the DSM does not stand in contrast or opposition to clinical diagnoses if used wisely, but in order to use the DSM, one ought to have knowledge of psychiatry first. Our specialty hands out a list of symptoms and essentially tells students and residents to “ask away.” One only has to look at the clipboard that medical students carry around on their psychiatric rotations or the DSM-centered multiple-choice questions that purport to assess medical students’ and residents’ (and board candidates’) knowledge of psychiatry to appreciate our observation. Any supposed mantle of respectability or objectivity that this bestows upon psychiatry occurs at the expense of its patients and its own intellectual integrity.

References

1. Serby M: Psychiatric resident conceptualizations of mood and affect within the mental status examination. Am J Psychiatry 2003; 160:1527–1529LinkGoogle Scholar

2. Kontos N, Freudenreich O, Querques J, Norris E: The consultation psychiatrist as effective physician. Gen Hosp Psychiatry 2003; 25:20–23Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar