The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×
Brief ReportFull Access

Responses to the Seasonal Pattern Assessment Questionnaire in Different Seasons

Abstract

OBJECTIVE: The study investigated whether results on the Seasonal Pattern Assessment Questionnaire, which is used for diagnosis of seasonal affective disorder, differed by the season in which the questionnaire was completed. METHOD: Every third month from March 1997 to February 1998, a population-based panel of 200 men and women age 27–72 years in Gamvik, northern Norway, completed a standardized questionnaire that included all items from the Seasonal Pattern Assessment Questionnaire. RESULTS: The average score for seasonal affective disorder changed over the year. The difference between the highest score, in March, and the lowest, in September, was 8.8%. CONCLUSIONS: Results on the Seasonal Pattern Assessment Questionnaire varied by season, but did not vary by seasonal differences in the amount of daylight.

The prevalence of seasonal affective disorder in the general population has been studied in many countries and at different latitudes, since the major theory explaining the disorder is that it is caused primarily by lack of daylight in winter. However, the prevalence estimates have varied considerably among studies, and there is no general agreement on the linkage to latitude (1). These differences could be related to study methods and, in particular, could be related to the psychometric properties of the Seasonal Pattern Assessment Questionnaire (2), which is used for diagnosis of seasonal affective disorder. In an earlier publication (3), we argued that the Seasonal Pattern Assessment Questionnaire has not been validated for different climatic and geographic conditions. In a recent review of the literature on prevalence and latitude, Mersch et al. (4) concluded that “if latitude influences prevalence, this influence is only weak.” Mersch et al. also discussed possible reasons for the great variability in reported prevalence. However, they did not discuss the possibility that scores on the Seasonal Pattern Assessment Questionnaire could be influenced by the time of the year in which the study is conducted. To investigate whether time of the year when the Seasonal Pattern Assessment Questionnaire is completed influences the results, we administered the questionnaire in the four different seasons to subjects from the general population of a small fishing community in northern Norway.

Method

Participants were recruited in Gamvik, Norway, as part of the National Screening Institute’s national screening program for cardiovascular diseases. Gamvik is located at 70 degrees north latitude, close to the North Sea and east of North Cape, and is one of the northernmost communities in Norway. In winter the sun is below the horizon continuously from mid-November to mid-January, and in summer it is above the horizon continuously from mid-May to mid-July. Invitations for participation in the cardiovascular screening program were mailed to all inhabitants born during the period 1925–1970 (N=550). The screening team used a mobile unit and stayed in Gamvik for three days in March 1997. The weather conditions during these days were extremely bad, with a snowstorm making all travel difficult. This reduced the participation rate somewhat, but about two-thirds of the invited inhabitants did attend.

Of those who attended the cardiovascular screening, 250 participants were invited to participate in a study of melatonin. Of that group, 231 (92.4%) gave informed consent to participate, and 200 (80.0%) answered the study questionnaire in one or more seasons. After the screening, the participants were mailed a questionnaire at approximately 3-month intervals, in June, September, and January 1998. Since some participants did not answer immediately, the sampling period at each interval could be as long as 2 months. The two-page questionnaire mainly asked participants about their experiences with daily life, including sleep, physical activities, coffee drinking, etc., on the day the questionnaire was completed.

The questionnaire contained all questions from the Seasonal Pattern Assessment Questionnaire. This instrument asks the subject to rate his/her pattern of seasonal variation in six areas—sleep, social activity, mood, weight, appetite, and energy level—on a 5-point scale from 0, no changes, to 4, extremely marked changes. The six scores are summed to produce a global seasonality score, which can range from 0 to 24. A subject is considered to have a seasonal affective disorder if the global seasonality score is 11 or more, the subject feels worst in January and February, and the seasonal changes are experienced as a problem of at least moderate degree.

The study was approved by the regional ethics committee of north Norway. After complete description of the study to the subjects, written informed consent was obtained. The Norwegian Data Inspectorate approved the collection and storage of the data.

Results

The female-to-male ratio among the 200 study participants was 1:2, and 52.5% of the subjects were age 50 or older. The percentages of the subjects who had a global seasonality score of 11 or more and the percentages of subjects who met the full seasonal affective disorder criteria at the four time points in the study are shown in Figure 1. The percentage who met criteria for seasonal affective disorder varied from 5.6% to 14.4% during the year. The highest prevalence values occurred in March, and lowest in September, with a difference of 8.8% (95% confidence interval=2.6%–15.0%). The same pattern was found when we analyzed the data for only those individuals who answered the questionnaire in all four seasons (data not shown).

The interobserver values for the Seasonal Pattern Assessment Questionnaire showed kappa values between 0.47, for March versus June, and 0.19, for March versus January.

Discussion

Our study found that the results on the Seasonal Pattern Assessment Questionnaire showed a clear seasonal pattern. In addition, the test had low-to-medium reliability when it was repeated several times over 1 year. This study was undertaken in one of the northernmost populated areas of Norway, where there are extreme shifts in the amount of daylight during the year. The highest and lowest scores did not coincide with the seasonal light rhythm. The number of people who met criteria for seasonal affective disorder was highest in March, when the day and night have about equal lengths, and lowest in September, which is darker. This result could be an effect of the bad weather in March during the year the study was conducted. Thus, the questionnaire could be invalidated by general weather conditions.

A seasonal pattern in responses to the Seasonal Pattern Assessment Questionnaire and differences in the seasons when previous studies were conducted could explain the differences between these studies in the estimated prevalence of seasonal affective disorder. Only three studies that we are aware of have tried to evaluate the effect of month of completion of the questionnaire on the estimated prevalence (57). These studies suggest that response to the questionnaire does not vary with the season, but in our view the question remains open.

In addition, the subjects who participated at all four time points throughout the year in our study did not give consistent responses, despite the fact that the Seasonal Pattern Assessment Questionnaire clearly asks for a general rating of symptoms. Our results suggest that subjects may misinterpret the questionnaire and provide a rating of symptoms for the season in which they are responding rather than an overall rating.

We conclude that the Seasonal Pattern Assessment Questionnaire is not reliable for comparison between populations in different geographic areas unless it is adjusted for seasonal effects in the performance of the test.

Received Jan. 6, 2000; revisions received June 6 and July 12, 2000; accepted Aug. 4, 2000. From the Institute of Community Medicine, University of Tromsø. Address reprint requests to Dr. Lund, Institute of Community Medicine, University of Tromsø, 9037 Tromsø, Norway; (e-mail). Supported by a grant from the Norwegian Ministry of Health and Social Services.

Figure 1.

Figure 1. Seasonal Variations Over 1 Year in Results on the Seasonal Pattern Assessment Questionnaire for the General Population of Gamvik, Norwaya

aThe Seasonal Pattern Assessment Questionnaire provides a global seasonality score from 0 to 24 that summarizes the subject’s self-ratings of seasonal variation in sleep, social activity, mood, weight, appetite, and energy level. Criteria for seasonal affective disorder are a global seasonality score of 11 or more, the presence of worse symptoms in January and February, and the experience of seasonal changes as a problem of at least moderate degree.

References

1. Magnusson A, Axelsson J: The prevalence of seasonal affective disorder is low among descendants of Icelandic emigrants in Canada. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1993; 50:947–951Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

2. Rosenthal NE, Bradt GH, Wehr TA: Seasonal Pattern Assessment Ouestionnaire. Bethesda, Md, National Institute of Mental Health, 1984Google Scholar

3. Hansen V, Lund E, Smith-Sivertsen T: Self-reported mental distress under the shifting daylight in the high north. Psychol Med 1998; 28:447–452Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

4. Mersch PP, Middendorp HM, Bouhuys AL, Beersma DG, van den Hoofdakker RH: Seasonal affective disorder and latitude: a review of the literature. J Affect Disord 1999; 53:35–48Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

5. Terman M, Botticelli SR, Link BG, Link MJ, Quitkin FM, Hardin TE, Rosenthal NE: Seasonal symptom patterns in New York: patients and population, in Seasonal Affective Disorder. Edited by Thompson C. London, CNS, 1989, pp 77–95Google Scholar

6. Hardin TA, Wehr TA, Brewerton T, Kasper S, Berretini W, Rabkin J, Rosenthal NE: Evaluation of seasonality in six clinical populations and two normal populations. J Psychiatr Res 1991; 25:75–87Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

7. Mersch P, Middendorp H, Bouhuys AL, Beersma D, van den Hoofdakker RH: The prevalence of seasonal affective disorder in the Netherlands: a prospective and retrospective study of seasonal mood variation in the general population. Biol Psychiatry 1999; 45:1013–1022Google Scholar