The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×
Regular ArticleFull Access

Decreased Benzodiazepine Receptor Binding in Prefrontal Cortex in Combat-Related Posttraumatic Stress Disorder

Abstract

OBJECTIVE: Animals exposed to stress exhibit a decrease in benzodiazepine receptor binding in the frontal cortex. No studies have examined central benzodiazepine receptor binding in patients with posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). The purpose of this study was to examine measures of benzodiazepine receptor binding in PTSD.METHOD: From 13 patients with Vietnam combat-related PTSD and 13 case-matched healthy comparison subjects, a quantitative measure related to benzodiazepine receptor binding (distribution volume) was obtained with single photon emission computed tomography (SPECT) imaging of [123I]iomazenil binding and measurement of radioligand concentration in plasma. Distribution volume image data were analyzed by means of statistical parametric mapping.RESULTS: Lower distribution volumes were found in the prefrontal cortex (Brodmann’s area 9) of PTSD patients than in comparison subjects.CONCLUSIONS: These findings of lower values for the benzodiazepine receptor binding measure of distribution volume are consistent with fewer benzodiazepine receptors and/or reduced affinity of receptor binding in the medial prefrontal cortex in patients with PTSD. Alterations in benzodiazepine receptor function in this area may underlie many of the symptoms of PTSD.

In 1980, posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) was first established as a diagnosis in DSM-III under the anxiety disorders category. Although there are important differences between PTSD and the other anxiety disorders—and the proper classification of these disorders continues to be debated—PTSD and other anxiety disorder patients have in common increased levels of anxiety symptoms that are responsive to treatment with benzodiazepine medications. The efficacy of benzodiazepines in the treatment of anxiety disorders has led to an effort to develop animal models, such as inescapable stress, to understand the mechanisms involved.

Animals exposed to chronic inescapable stress develop behaviors that indicate excessive fear and anxiety, such as increased fearfulness, increased defecation, and avoidance of novel situations (e.g., an open field). Exposure to inescapable stressors such as foot shock or forced swim also resulted in a 20%–30% decrease in benzodiazepine receptor binding in the frontal cortex (1, 2) and cerebral cortex (37), with some studies showing a decrease in the hippocampus (3, 4, 6, 7, but see references 2, 5, 8, and 9). Exposure to stress has no effects on benzodiazepine receptor binding in the pons, striatum, thalamus, cerebellum, midbrain, or occipital cortex (1, 35, 712). One study that used a different model of stressor, defeat stress, showed an increase in benzodiazepine receptor binding in the cortex (12). These studies are consistent with a role for alterations in benzodiazepine binding in anxiety (13), with findings showing a specific decrease in the frontal cortex and, although not as consistently, a decrease in the hippocampus. The findings suggest that decreased benzodiazepine receptor binding in the frontal cortex may play a role in the symptoms of PTSD.

Studies in human subjects are also consistent with a relationship between stress and alterations in benzodiazepine receptor binding (1416). FG7142, a benzodiazepine receptor inverse agonist, induced severe anxiety that resembled panic attacks and biological characteristics of anxiety in healthy subjects (14). Patients with combat-related PTSD did not exhibit a greater behavioral response (i.e., anxiety) with the benzodiazepine receptor antagonist flumazenil than did healthy subjects (16); however, flumazenil has no pharmacological effect on the receptor per se. Future studies are required with other agents, such as inverse agonists, that have an effect on the benzodiazepine receptor. Benzodiazepine administration has been shown to be efficacious in the treatment of symptoms of chronic PTSD and anxiety (1719), although it has not been shown to prevent the development of chronic PTSD (20). These findings suggest that alterations in benzodiazepine receptor function may underlie the symptoms of anxiety and, more specifically, PTSD.

Recently, neuroimaging techniques have been developed that provide quantitation of neuroreceptors in living human brain. Neuroreceptor imaging, however, has not yet been applied to the study of PTSD. One area of research that holds promise for the study of anxiety disorders is quantitation of benzodiazepine receptor binding. Iomazenil is a compound that binds with high affinity to the benzodiazepine receptor. Iomazenil (2123) and other compounds (24) have been used to image the benzodiazepine receptor in patients with other anxiety disorders besides PTSD. Single photon emission computed tomography (SPECT) with [123I]iomazenil was demonstrated to provide reliable and valid quantitation of benzodiazepine receptors in human subjects (2527). No studies to date have used nuclear imaging to examine central benzodiazepine receptor binding in PTSD. The purpose of this study was to measure benzodiazepine receptor binding by means of SPECT and [123I]iomazenil in patients with PTSD and matched normal comparison subjects. On the basis of the aforementioned preclinical studies, we hypothesized a decrease in benzodiazepine receptor binding in the frontal cortex and hippocampus, but not in other areas (pons, striatum, thalamus, cerebellum, or midbrain) in patients with PTSD relative to normal comparison subjects.

Method

Thirteen subjects seeking outpatient treatment for Vietnam combat-related PTSD were recruited for the study. All PTSD patients were male, right-handed, and met criteria for PTSD as determined by the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID) (28). Eleven (85%) of the 13 patients were white, and two (15%) were black. At study entry, PTSD patients had not been taking any psychotropic medications for at least 4 weeks. The patients had no history of benzodiazepine usage in the preceding 6 months. Urinary toxicology screens were performed, and patients in whom the presence of benzodiazepines or other substances were detected were excluded from the study. PTSD patients with a history of alcohol or drug abuse or dependence in the 6 months preceding the study, meningitis, traumatic brain injury, loss of consciousness of greater than 1 minute, other neurological disorder, or psychotic disorder determined during administration of the SCID were excluded. Patients who had a history of foreign bodies that would preclude MRI scanning were also excluded.

Comparison subjects were healthy subjects without a history of medical or psychiatric disorder who were case-matched to the PTSD patients. Comparison subjects were recruited by newspaper advertisement and were selected to be matched to the patients by race and sex. In addition comparison subjects were selected to be similar in age to PTSD patients. All comparison subjects were male, right-handed, and were not taking any psychotropic medication nor had they in the preceding 4 weeks; 11 (85%) of the 13 comparison subjects were white, and two (15%) were black. There was no significant difference in age between the PTSD patients (mean=49 years, SD=2) and the comparison subjects (mean=44 years, SD=9). In addition to the exclusion criteria outlined above for the PTSD patients, comparison subjects with a history of psychiatric disorder as determined by the non-patient version of the SCID were excluded. All subjects in this study provided written informed consent for participation.

PTSD patients were evaluated with the SCID to determine the presence of psychiatric diagnoses. None of the 13 patients had a history of a psychiatric disorder that preceded the onset of PTSD, and in all cases PTSD was the primary diagnosis. Comorbid conditions detected in the 13 PTSD patients included panic disorder with agoraphobia (current: 54% [N=7]; lifetime: 69% [N=9]), social phobia (current: 31% [N=4]; lifetime: (38% [N=5]), simple phobia (current: 8% [N=1]; lifetime: 15% [N=2]), generalized anxiety disorder (current: 8% [N=1]; lifetime: 15% [N=2]), bulimia (current: 8% [N=1]; lifetime: 15% [N=2]), and major depression (current: 38% [N=5]); lifetime: 85% [N=11]). None of the patients met criteria for current or lifetime anorexia, obsessive-compulsive disorder, schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, somatization disorder, somatic pain disorder, undifferentiated somatization disorder, or hypochondriasis. Two patients (15%) met criteria for past alcohol dependence, and one (8%) met criteria for past alcohol abuse.

All subjects were evaluated at the time of the scan with the Panic Attack Symptom Scale (29), Hamilton Anxiety Rating Scale, and the Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale (BPRS). PTSD patients had higher levels of anxiety symptoms than the comparison subjects, as measured by the Panic Attack Symptom Scale (mean=3.7 [SD=4.4] versus 1.4 [SD=2.9], respectively) and the Hamilton anxiety scale (mean=17.3 [SD=8.2] versus 2.9 [SD=4.2]); they also had higher levels of general psychiatric symptoms as measured with the BPRS (mean=29.3 [SD=5.8] versus 19.4 [SD=1.1]).

All subjects underwent a single SPECT [123I]iomazenil scan for measurement of benzodiazepine receptors with the equilibrium technique on a Ceraspect camera (Digital Scintigraphics, Inc., Waltham, Mass.). The methods of the equilibrium technique and properties of the Ceraspect camera have been previously described in detail (2527). Briefly, a bolus dose of 6 mCi [123I]iomazenil was administered followed by a continuous infusion of radiotracer over 8 hours (ratio of bolus/infusion rate=3.8). Starting at 6 hours after administration of the bolus, subjects underwent SPECT scanning once radioligand uptake into the brain reached equilibrium, as determined by no change in brain tissue concentration over three consecutive scans. At equilibrium (as determined by this method), rate of transfer of radioligand (i.e., [123I]iomazenil) onto the receptor is assumed to be equal to rate of transfer off of receptor. Subjects underwent three 10-minute acquisitions. SPECT images were reconstructed by using methods previously described (2527). Venous blood was drawn at the time of the image acquisition (i.e., 6 hours after initiation of bolus plus constant infusion protocol) for measurement of total and free (or non-protein-bound) [123I]iomazenil parent compound in plasma by using methods previously described (30). There was no difference between PTSD and comparison subjects in plasma concentrations of free non-protein-bound parent compound (mean=0.0014 [SD=0.0008] and 0.0012 [SD=0.0002]μCi/ml, respectively), total parent compound (mean=0.0041 [SD=0.0024] and 0.0035 [SD=0.0009] μCi/ml), or in the fraction of free parent in plasma (mean=0.34 [SD=0.06] and 0.32 [SD=0.02]).

Magnetic resonance (MR) images were obtained in all subjects for coregistration with SPECT. The MR acquisition protocol, method for coregistration, and use of MR for identification of regions of interest on functional images have been previously described in detail (31).

Global radioactivity in the three scans was examined to determine that the patient was at equilibrium (retrospectively determined by the fact that there was not greater than 10% difference in global distribution volume between the three scans). This criterion was used to determine equilibrium for all subjects (consistent with our prior findings of the time course to equilibrium). The three SPECT scans were realigned to the first scan in the series and summed into a single image (Figure 1). SPECT scans were then converted from counts per pixel per minute to μCi/ml by using calibration factors based on phantom studies. Distribution volumes, which are equal to the ratio of tissue concentration of radiotracer to plasma concentration of non-protein-bound parent radiotracer at equilibrium, were then calculated from the SPECT and plasma data. In the three-compartment model, the first compartment consists of unmetabolized radioligand (i.e., [123I]iomazenil) in plasma that is available for uptake into the brain, the second compartment is free and nonspecifically bound radioligand, and the third compartment is radioligand specifically bound to the receptor. Brain tissue activity as determined from the SPECT image includes radioligand specifically bound to receptor, nonspecifically bound to brain tissue, and free radioligand in brain tissue. However, the primary contribution to the image is from radioligand specifically bound to receptor (32), and the contributions from nonspecific binding and free radioligand in brain tissue are assumed to be negligible. On the basis of this assumption, distribution volume is equal to the ratio of the concentration of bound radiotracer in brain to the concentration of free nonprotein bound radiotracer in plasma. This ratio of bound to free, also described as the binding potential, is equal to the ratio of receptor number (Bmax) to affinity (KD), and is also equal to (K1 * K3)/(k2 * k4 * f1), where K1 = rate of transfer from blood to brain; k2 = rate of transfer from brain to blood; K3 = rate of transfer from nonspecific or unbound to radioligand specifically bound to receptor; k4 = rate of transfer from specifically bound to nonspecifically bound, and; f1 = the fraction of free parent compound in plasma that is non-protein-bound and available for uptake into the brain. Data were analyzed by using both protein- and non-protein-bound plasma concentrations of radioligand. Identical results were obtained using both methods—data presented are based on protein-bound plasma concentrations of radioligand.

SPECT images in which values for individual voxels (or picture elements) within the brain corresponded to distribution volume values were then constructed by dividing values for radioactivity in each voxel by the plasma concentration of unmetabolized radioligand determined for that particular subject. These SPECT distribution volume images were stripped of extracranial artifact and coregistered to MR images. Coregistered MR images were used to determine normalization parameters for transformation of SPECT images into a common anatomical space. This method takes advantage of the superior delineation of anatomical detail by MR imaging in comparison to SPECT. Images were then smoothed by using a three-dimensional Gaussian filter to 16 mm full width at half maximum.

Statistical parametric mapping was used to compare distribution volumes (the measure of regional benzodiazepine receptor binding) in patients with PTSD versus comparison subjects (33). Analyses were performed by using analysis of covariance with global benzodiazepine receptor binding considered as a confounding covariate. Statistical parametric mapping allows a statistical comparison between patients and comparison subjects on a voxel-by-voxel basis (voxels are the individual picture elements of a brain image, which correspond to a discrete location within the brain). With this method there is a statistical value (e.g., t statistic) for the difference between groups assigned to every voxel in the brain. Values for t statistic in individual voxels were transformed into z scores and displayed as an image made up of multiple voxels (or picture elements) that corresponded to individual anatomical areas of the brain, which demonstrated areas of statistically significant differences between the groups. Statistical parametric mapping analysis provides a global survey of areas of statistically significant difference throughout the brain. This approach has the inherent risk of falsely finding a significant difference due to multiple comparisons. To limit the risks of multiple comparisons, we used research findings from animal studies to identify a priori brain areas that would be expected to show a decrease in benzodiazepine receptor binding in PTSD. Regions selected included the hippocampus and prefrontal cortex, since two or more studies of the effects of stress on benzodiazepine receptor binding in animals have found decreased binding in these areas. Several studies have reported a decrease in the cerebral cortex, although studies that looked specifically at the occipital cortex found no change with stress. Since the cerebral cortex is nonspecific (and also includes the frontal cortex), the prefrontal cortex was identified as the a priori region of interest. This method of identifying a priori regions of interest has limitations, such as the fact that findings in rats may not be applicable to humans, the human brain is obviously more developed, and other brain areas may be affected in PTSD. However, in the absence of any literature on benzodiazepine receptor binding in PTSD, animal studies were used to develop hypotheses. Areas of decreased benzodiazepine receptor binding in PTSD within these hypothesized regions were identified by using a threshold of p<0.001 (z score >3.2). Other brain areas of significant difference outside of the hypothesized regions (if present) were identified for hypothesis generation for future studies. Location of areas of significant difference were identified as the distance from the anterior commissure in mm, with x, y, and z coordinates, by using the standard Talairach coordinate space (34) with statistical maps displayed in radiological convention (patient’s left is image right). Pearson correlations were used to examine the correlation between benzodiazepine receptor binding in the prefrontal cortex and panic symptoms (measured with the clinician-rated Panic Attack Symptom Scale), anxiety symptoms (measured with the Hamilton anxiety scale), and general psychiatric symptoms (measured with the BPRS) at the time of scan.

Results

In the PTSD patients, distribution volume (the benzodiazepine receptor binding measure) was 41% lower in the prefrontal cortex (Brodmann’s area 9) relative to comparison subjects (Talairach coordinates: x=–4, y=56, z=28; z score=3.56, p<0.001) (Figure 2 and Figure 3). Distribution volumes rescaled to a nominal global mean of 50 were mean=31.0 (SD=10.1) in the PTSD patients versus mean=52.7 (SD=15.0) in the comparison subjects (t=4.3, df=24, p=0.0003) (Figure 3); the effect size for the difference between groups was 1.65. There was no difference in other brain areas not expected to differ between patients and comparison subjects (pons, striatum, thalamus, cerebellum, or midbrain).

There were no areas of greater benzodiazepine receptor binding values in PTSD relative to comparison subjects. The absolute quantitative whole brain distribution volume between PTSD patients (mean=19.5, SD=13.4) and comparison subjects (mean=23.2, SD=11.1) was not significantly different.

General psychiatric symptom level at the time of the scan as measured with the BPRS was correlated with benzodiazepine receptor binding in the prefrontal cortex (Brodmann’s area 9) (Figure 4). This positive correlation was not hypothesized a priori. There was not a significant correlation between benzodiazepine receptor binding in this prefrontal cortical area of the PTSD patients and anxiety symptom levels as measured with the Hamilton anxiety scale or the Panic Attack Symptom Scale.

Discussion

SPECT imaging of [123]iomazenil binding showed that PTSD patients had 41% lower distribution volumes (the measure of benzodiazepine receptor binding) in the prefrontal cortex (Brodmann’s area 9) relative to comparison subjects. There were no other areas of decreased binding or increased binding in PTSD patients relative to comparison subjects in this study.

These findings are consistent with preclinical studies that have shown a decrease in benzodiazepine receptor binding in the frontal cortex following stress. The findings could be related to a down-regulation of benzodiazepine receptor binding following exposure to the stress of war. Other possible explanations are that stress results in changes in receptor affinity, changes in an endogenous benzodiazepine ligand (the existence of which is controversial), or stress-related alterations in GABAergic transmission or neurosteroids that affect benzodiazepine receptor binding. An alternative explanation is that a preexisting low level of benzodiazepine receptor binding in the prefrontal cortex increases the risk for the development of PTSD following exposure to the stress of combat. Although a loss of neurons in prefrontal cortex in PTSD could explain a decrease in binding, the results are not explainable by differences in blood flow, since the equilibrium method eliminates differences in flow and tracer delivery from influencing the outcome measure of receptor binding.

The prefrontal cortex mediates several cognitive and behavioral processes that are potentially relevant to PTSD (35). The medial prefrontal cortex has been implicated in the inhibition of cognition (which may relate to intrusive memories in PTSD) and emotional and social behavior (36, 37). Positron emission tomography studies are consistent with medial prefrontal cortex dysfunction in PTSD, which provides a possible neural correlate of dysregulation of emotional and social behavior in PTSD (3840). In one study of abuse-related PTSD (39), traumatic reminders resulted in increased activation in Brodmann’s area 9 of the prefrontal cortex, the specific area of the prefrontal cortex implicated in this study. These findings point to the importance that alterations in the medial prefrontal cortex play in PTSD, although the role of specific subregions of this area in symptoms of this disorder are not yet clear.

There was a positive correlation between psychiatric symptom level at the time of the scan as measured with the BPRS and decreased benzodiazepine receptor binding in the prefrontal cortex (Brodmann’s area 9). We hypothesized a priori a negative correlation between symptom level and prefrontal binding (not a positive correlation as our data showed). One interpretation of the findings is that chronic stress is associated with benzodiazepine receptor binding reduction, whereas acute increases in psychiatric symptom states are associated with increased benzodiazepine receptor binding in the prefrontal cortex (possibly related to down-regulation of receptors in response to repeated episodes of state anxiety). These findings require replication.

Several other limitations of this study are worth mentioning. There are assumptions inherent in the SPECT equilibrium technique for quantitation of neuroreceptor binding that need to be considered in the interpretation of the results. The method assumes that there is perfect equilibration between free activity in plasma and non-receptor-bound activity in the brain. The method also assumes the nonspecific binding in the brain is negligible (given the 20:1 ratio of specific to nonspecific binding of this radioligand); however, as much as 13% of radioligand is nondisplaceable (25). Changes in the outcome measure (distribution volume) may be related to changes in neuroreceptor number or affinity. However, other factors, such as changes in endogenous ligands competing for the receptor site, could influence this parameter. Violations of any of these assumptions may influence quantitation of neuroreceptor binding, although this should not introduce a systematic confound in a study like the current one. It should be noted that there are advantages to the equilibrium technique, primarily because of the greater feasibility related to the fact that arterial sampling is not required. PTSD patients had other comorbid conditions, which is a typical finding in this disorder. Two of the PTSD patients had a past history of alcohol dependence, which may have contributed to the findings of decreased benzodiazepine receptor binding in the frontal cortex. There were high rates of anxiety disorders (particularly panic disorder) and depression, as previously reported. In all cases, the onset of PTSD preceded the onset of anxiety disorders and depression by several years or more. In a prior study, we reported decreased left hippocampal benzodiazepine receptor binding in panic disorder (41), with no differences in the prefrontal cortex between patients and comparison subjects, although a correlation was found in a similar region of the prefrontal cortex in the panic patients. In our clinical judgment, symptoms that technically met criteria for panic disorder in the PTSD group were related to trauma exposure and were secondary to PTSD, and the patients in the two studies had very different clinical presentations. Therefore, it would be difficult to attribute findings of the current study to panic disorder. No studies have examined benzodiazepine receptor binding in depression. Again, however, patients in this study all had depression that followed the onset of PTSD and was clinically secondary as a diagnosis to PTSD. The current study did not include a combat-exposed non-PTSD control group. Therefore, it is possible that the findings are related to a nonspecific effect of extreme stress that is not specific to PTSD. Future studies are needed that use such a control. In the current study, we selected patients who were free of benzodiazepine usage or other substances as determined by toxicology screen and self-report. Although to the best of our knowledge patients had been free of benzodiazepines in the preceding 6 months, more remote histories were difficult to obtain.

Received June 9, 1999; revision received Dec. 21, 1999; accepted Jan. 27, 2000. From the Departments of Diagnostic Radiology and Psychiatry, Yale University School of Medicine, VA Connecticut Healthcare System; the National Center for PTSD, West Haven, Conn.; and the Yale Psychiatric Institute, New Haven, Conn. Address reprint requests to Dr. Bremner, VA Connecticut Healthcare System (115a), 950 Campbell Ave., West Haven, CT 06516; (e-mail). The authors thank Helen Sayward, M.S., Eric Anderson, B.S., and Jennifer Valenti, B.S., for image processing and data analysis; Sandi Capelli, R.N., Gail Banks, R.N., and Louise Brenner, R.N., for patient recruitment and assessment; Michelle Early, C.N.M.T., Gary Wyznewski, C.N.M.T., and Eileen Smith, C.N.M.T., for assistance in SPECT acquisition; Hedy Sarofin for assistance in MRI acquisition; and Louis Amici, M.S., and Melyssa Madrak, B.S., for assistance in radiosynthesis and plasma analysis.

Figure 1.

Figure 1. Axial SPECT Image of [123I]Iomazenil Binding in the Brain of a Comparison Subjecta

aRadioligand is primarily distributed in cortical areas, with a 20:1 ratio of gray to white matter.

Figure 2.

Figure 2. Benzodiazepine Receptor Binding (distribution volume) in 13 Patients With PTSD Relative to That of 13 Healthy Comparison Subjectsa

aImage is a statistical parametric map overlaid on an axial MR template image. In the hypothesized area (Brodmann’s area 9), significantly lower benzodiazepine receptor binding was found in the prefrontal cortex of the PTSD patients (z score >3.00, p<0.001).

Figure 3.

Figure 3. Benzodiazepine Receptor Binding in the Prefrontal Cortex (Brodmann’s Area 9) of 13 Patients With PTSD and 13 Healthy Comparison Subjects

aSignificantly lower mean distribution volume than that of the healthy comparison subjects (z=3.56, p<0.001).

Figure 4.

Figure 4. Relationship Between Benzodiazepine Receptor Binding in the Prefrontal Cortex and BPRS Score in 13 Patients With PTSDa

aThe BPRS was administered to measure general psychiatric symptoms at the time of the scan. There was a significant correlation between receptor binding in the prefrontal cortex (Brodmann’s area 9) and BPRS score in patients with PTSD (r=0.68, df=8, p<0.05).

References

1. Lippa AS, Klepner CA, Yunger L, Sano MC, Smith WV, Beer B: Relationship between benzodiazepine receptors and experimental anxiety in rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 1978; 9:853–856Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

2. Weizman R, Weizman A, Kook KA, Vocci F, Deutsch SI, Paul SM: Repeated swim stress alters brain benzodiazepine receptors measured in vivo. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 1989; 249:701–707MedlineGoogle Scholar

3. Medina JH, Novas ML, De Robertis E: Changes in benzodiazepine receptors by acute stress: different effect of chronic diazepam or RO 15-1788 treatment. Eur J Pharmacol 1983; 96:181–185Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

4. Medina JH, Novas ML, Wolfman CNV, De Stein ML, De Robertis E: Benzodiazepine receptors in rat cerebral cortex and hippocampus undergo rapid and reversible changes after acute stress. Neuroscience 1983; 9:331–335Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

5. Drugan RC, Basile AC, Crawley JN, Paul SM, Skolnick P: Inescapable shock reduces [3H]Ro 5-4864 binding to peripheral type benzodiazepine receptors in the rat. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 1986; 24:1673–1677Google Scholar

6. Drugan RC, Morrow AL, Weizman R, Weizman A, Deutsch SI, Crawley JN, Paul SM: Stress-induced behavioral depression in the rat is associated with a decrease in GABA receptor-mediated chloride ion flux and brain benzodiazepine receptor occupancy. Brain Res 1989; 487:45–51Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

7. Weizman A, Weizman R, Kook KA, Vocci F, Deutsch SI, Paul SM: Adrenalectomy prevents the stress-induced decrease in in vivo [ 3H]Ro 15-1788 binding to GABAA benzodiazepine receptors in the mouse. Brain Res 1990; 519:347–350Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

8. Skerritt JH, Trisdikoon P, Johnston GAR: Increased GABA binding in mouse brain following acute swim stress. Brain Res 1981; 215:398–403Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

9. Havoundjian H, Paul SM, Skolnick P: Rapid, stress-induced modification of the benzodiazepine receptor-coupled chloride ionophore. Brain Res 1986; 375:401–406Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

10. Braestrup C, Nielsen M, Nielsen EB, Lyon M: Benzodiazepine receptors in the brain as affected by different experimental stresses: the changes are small and not unidirectional. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1979; 65:273–277Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

11. LeFur G, Guilloux F, Mitrani N, Mizoule J, Uzan A: Relationship between plasma corticosteroids and benzodiazepines in stress. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 1979; 211:305–308MedlineGoogle Scholar

12. Miller LG, Thompson ML, Greenblatt DJ, Deutsch SI, Shader RI, Paul SM: Rapid increase in brain benzodiazepine receptor binding following defeat stress in mice. Brain Res 1987; 414:395–400Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

13. Guidotti A, Baraldi M, Leon A, Costa E: Benzodiazepines: a tool to explore the biochemical and neuro-physiological basis of anxiety. Fed Proc 1990; 39:1039–1042Google Scholar

14. Dorow R, Horowski R, Paschelke G, Amin M, Braestrup C: Severe anxiety induced by FG 7142, a β-carboline ligand for benzodiazepine receptors (letter). Lancet 1983; 2:98–99Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

15. Weizman R, Laor N, Karp L, Dagan E, Reiss A, Dar DE, Wolmer L, Gavish M: Alteration of platelet benzodiazepine receptors by stress of war. Am J Psychiatry 1994; 151:766–767LinkGoogle Scholar

16. Weizman R, Tanne Z, Granek M, Karp L, Golomb M, Tyano S, Gavish M: Peripheral benzodiazepine binding sites on platelet membranes are increased during diazepam treatment of anxious patients. Eur J Pharmacol 1987; 138:289–292Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

17. Bremner JD, Charney DS: Anxiety disorders, in Conn’s Current Therapies. Edited by Rakel RE. Philadelphia, WB Saunders, 1994, pp 1103–1107Google Scholar

18. Bremner JD, Southwick SM, Darnell A, Charney DS: Chronic PTSD in Vietnam combat veterans: course of illness and substance abuse. Am J Psychiatry 1996; 153:369–375LinkGoogle Scholar

19. Risse SC, Whitters A, Burke J, Chen S, Scurfield RM, Raskind MA: Severe withdrawal symptoms after discontinuation of alprazolam in eight patients with combat-induced posttraumatic stress disorder. J Clin Psychiatry 1990; 51:206–209MedlineGoogle Scholar

20. Gelpin E, Bonne O, Peri T, Brandes D, Shalev AY: Treatment of recent trauma survivors with benzodiazepines: a prospective study. J Clin Psychiatry 1996; 57:390–394MedlineGoogle Scholar

21. Kaschka W, Feistel H, Ebert D: Reduced benzodiazepine receptor binding in panic disorder measured by iomazenil SPECT. J Psychiatr Res 1995; 29:427–434Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

22. Kuikka JT, Pitkanen A, Lepola U, Partanen K, Vainio P, Bergstrom KA, Wieler HJ, Kaiser KP, Mittlebach L, Koponen H: Abnormal regional benzodiazepine receptor uptake in the prefrontal cortex in patients with panic disorder. Nucl Med Commun 1995; 16:273–280Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

23. Schlegel S, Steinert H, Bockisch A, Hahn K, Schloesser R, Benkert O: Decreased benzodiazepine receptor binding in panic disorder measured by iomazenil-SPECT: a preliminary report. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 1994; 244:49–51Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

24. Malizia AL, Cunningham VJ, Bell CJ, Liddle PF, Jones T, Nutt DJ: Decreased brain GABA-A benzodiazepine receptor binding in panic disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1998; 55:715–720Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

25. Abi-Dargham A, Laruelle M, Seibyl J, Rattner Z, Baldwin RM, Zoghbi SS, Zea Ponce Y, Bremner JD, Hyde TM, Charney DS, Hoffer PB, Innis RB: SPECT measurement of benzodiazepine receptors in human brain with [123I]iomazenil: kinetic and equilibrium paradigms. J Nucl Med 1994; 35:228–238MedlineGoogle Scholar

26. Abi-Dargham A, Gandelman M, Zoghbi SS, Laruelle M, Baldwin RM, Randall P, Zea-Ponce Y, Charney DS, Hoffer PB, Innis RB: Reproducibility of SPECT measurement of benzodiazepine receptors in human brain with [123I]iomazenil. J Nucl Med 1995; 36:167–175MedlineGoogle Scholar

27. Bremner JD, Baldwin R, Horti A, Staib LH, Ng CK, Tan P-Z, Zea-Ponce Y, Zoghbi S, Seibyl JP, Soufer R, Charney DS, Innis RB: Quantitation of benzodiazepine receptor binding with PET [11C]iomazenil and SPECT [123I]iomazenil: preliminary results of a direct comparison in healthy human subjects. Psychiatry Res Neuroimaging 1999; 91:79–91Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

28. First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JBW, Janet B: Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders (SCID), Clinical Version: User’s Guide. Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Press, 1996Google Scholar

29. Southwick SM, Krystal JH, Morgan CA, Johnson D, Nagy LM, Nicolau A, Heninger GR, Charney DS: Abnormal noradrenergic function in posttraumatic stress disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1993; 181:31–37Google Scholar

30. Zoghbi SS, Baldwin RM, Seibyl JP, Al-Tikriti MS, Zea-Ponce Y, Laruelle M, Sybirska EH, Woods SW, Goddard AW, Malison RT, Zimmerman R, Charney DS, Smith EO, Hoffer PB, Innis RB: Pharmacokinetics of the SPECT benzodiazepine radioligand [123I]iomazenil in human and nonhuman primates. Nucl Med Biol 1992; 19:881–888Google Scholar

31. Bremner JD, Bronen RA, de Erasquin G, Vermetten E, Staib L, Ng CK, Soufer R, Charney DS, Innis RB: Development and reliability of a method for using magnetic resonance imaging for the definition of regions of interest for positron emission tomography. Clin Positron Imaging 1998; 1:145–159Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

32. Sybirska E, Seibyl JP, Bremner JD, Baldwin RM, Al-Tikriti MS, Bradberry C, Malison RT, Zea-Ponce Y, Zoghbi S, During M, Goddard AW, Woods SW, Hoffer PB, Charney DS, Innis RB: [123I]Iomazenil SPECT imaging demonstrates significant benzodiazepine receptor reserve in human and non-human primate brain. Neuropharmacology 1993; 32:671–680Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

33. Friston K, Frith C, Liddle P, Franckowiak R: Comparing functional (PET) images: the assessment of significant change. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 1991; 11:690–699Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

34. Talairach J, Tournoux P: Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain: Three-Dimensional Proportional System. Stuttgart, Germany, Georg Thieme, 1988Google Scholar

35. Goldman-Rakic PS: Topography of cognition: parallel distributed networks in primate association cortex. Annu Rev Neurosci 1988; 11:137–156Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

36. Damasio H, Grabowski T, Frank R, Galaburda AM, Damasio AR: The return of Phineas Gage: clues about the brain from the skull of a famous patient. Science 1994; 264:1102–1105Google Scholar

37. Morgan MA, Romanski LM, LeDoux JE: Extinction of emotional learning: contribution of medial prefrontal cortex. Neurosci Lett 1993; 163:109–113Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

38. Bremner JD, Staib L, Kaloupek D, Southwick SM, Soufer R, Charney DS: Neural correlates of exposure to traumatic pictures and sound in Vietnam combat veterans with and without posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD): a positron emission tomography study. Biol Psychiatry 1999; 45:806–816Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

39. Bremner JD, Narayan M, Staib LH, Southwick SM, McGlashan T, Charney DS: Neural correlates of memories of childhood sexual abuse in women with and without posttraumatic stress disorder. Am J Psychiatry 1999; 156:1787–1795Google Scholar

40. Bremner JD, Innis RB, Ng CK, Staib L, Duncan J, Bronen R, Zubal G, Rich D, Krystal JH, Dey H, Soufer R, Charney DS: PET measurement of cerebral metabolic correlates of yohimbine administration in posttraumatic stress disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1997; 54:246–256Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

41. Bremner JD, Innis RB, White T, Fujita M, Silbersweig D, Goddard AW, Staib LH, Stern E, Cappiello A, Woods S, Baldwin R, Charney DS: SPECT [123I]iomazenil measurement of the benzodiazepine receptor in panic disorder. Biol Psychiatry 2000; 47:96–106Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar