The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×
ArticleFull Access

Adverse Life Events and Cognitive-Personality Characteristics in the Prediction of Major Depression and Antidepressant Response

Published Online:

Abstract

OBJECTIVE: Stressful life events are known to precipitate major depression. However, it remains unclear why some individuals who experience adverse events develop depression whereas others do not, and how the occurrence of life events affects treatment outcome. Emerging models posit that the effect of adverse life events varies by cognitive-personality style. This study examines the direct and interactive effects of stressful life events and cognitive-personality style in predicting 1) episode onset in patients with DSM-IV unipolar depression versus community comparison subjects and 2) depressive symptom severity at the completion of a 6-week standard antidepressant regimen.METHOD: Multivariate models were used to test the effects of adverse life events, cognitive-personality style, and the congruence of event type (interpersonal versus achievement) with cognitive-personality style on depressive onset and treatment outcome in 43 patients with major depression and 43 healthy comparison subjects. Cognitive-personality characteristics were assessed by using Beck’s measures of sociotropy (interpersonal dependency) and autonomy (need for independence and control).RESULTS: Adverse life events, sociotropy, and an autonomy factor need for control were each significantly related to depressive onset and predicted group status for 88% of the subjects. Event types affected outcome differently, and specific life event types interacted with cognitive-personality styles in predicting response to treatment. A multivariate model accounted for 65% of the variance in predicting outcome.CONCLUSIONS: Adverse life events are a potent factor in predicting depression. However, cognitive-personality characteristics also confer susceptibility to depression. Better outcome is associated with occurrence of adverse interpersonal events (e.g., death of a loved one) rather than adverse achievement events (e.g., loss of job) and occurs when the event type is congruent with cognitive-personality style.

The association between stressful life events and major depression has been clearly documented both in patient populations (15) and in community samples (2, 611). The critical issues for contemporary stress research are why some individuals who experience severe events develop adverse health outcomes, such as depression, whereas others do not and how the occurrence of life events affects our treatments for depression.

Initial attempts to predict depression by using multivariate models have found that stressful life events are a potent predictor of depression and that other risk factors independently contribute to the potential for major depression. Such factors include previous history of depression (12, 13), gender (13), age (13), genetics (12), and personality style (12). Emerging models for understanding why life events precipitate depression in some individuals and not in others posit that the effect of events varies by cognitive-personality style. Beck’s cognitive theory of depression, for example, proposes that the cognitive-personality characteristics of sociotropy and autonomy act as “vulnerability markers for depression by sensitizing individuals to certain types of negative life experiences” (14). Interactions between sociotropy (or interpersonal dependency, characterized by a high need for close relationships and concern about disapproval) (15) and negative interpersonal events (e.g., death of a loved one) have been reported to predict depression (16), as has the interaction between autonomy (characterized by a heavy emphasis on personal independence and control) (16) and negative achievement events (e.g., loss of employment) (17). Specifically, prior work has suggested that a negative experience best predicts the onset of depression when a specific type of event (e.g., a negative interpersonal event) affects a personal vulnerability (e.g., concern about disapproval) (18). Further examination of the effects of adverse life events and cognitive-personality characteristics requires consideration of these variables in the context of multiple known risk factors for depression.

The limited number of studies on the relationship of antecedent life stress to treatment response have reported mixed findings. Two studies showed no association between pretreatment life stressors and outcome (19, 20), whereas others have found that pretreatment adverse life events were correlated both with a good treatment response (2124) and with a poor response (25). In general, these studies have used nonstandardized treatments and have not used multivariate models to test the effects of other variables such as cognitive-personality style. Only one placebo-controlled study has examined the relationship of cognitive-personality characteristics to antidepressant response (26). This work, without examining life events, found that autonomous traits were associated with greater response to drug treatment than were sociotropic traits.

The current investigation used a multivariate approach to test how adverse life events, cognitive-personality style, and the congruence of cognitive-personality style with adverse event type were related to 1) the onset of depressive illness (through a case-control, cross-sectional design) and 2) treatment response (through a prospective design) in conjunction with other known risk factors. Of particular interest to us was the interaction of adverse life events and the cognitive-personality style autonomy, characterized by need for control, in predicting onset and response. Current work by our group has found that a reduced sense of personal control, as assessed by a measure of self-efficacy, mediates the effects of stressful life events in predicting depression (27). In addition, personal control in relation to events has long been hypothesized to be a critical factor in determining health outcomes (28, 29).

Method

Subjects

Forty-three patients with a DSM-IV diagnosis of unipolar major depression and 43 age-, race-, and sex-matched community comparison subjects were studied. After a complete description of the study was provided, informed consent was provided by all participants.

Patients (mean age=39.3 years, SD=11.4) were referred for recruitment by local treatment centers and group practices after an initial evaluation by treating clinicians determined both the presence of nonpsychotic unipolar major depression and that antidepressant drug treatment was indicated. Potential patients were then administered the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders (SCID) (30) by a trained interviewer to establish the presence of a current major depressive episode and determine exclusionary diagnoses. Exclusion criteria were any lifetime history of mania, hypomania, psychosis, or a substance abuse history within the past 6 months. For 16 patients, the current depressive episode was their first; 27 patients were diagnosed with recurrent depression. Each patient was then prescribed a standard antidepressant drug regimen by the treating physician. All patients included in the current study completed 6 weeks of the prescribed antidepressant drug regimen. Although different standard regimens could be prescribed, selection of medications, doses, and dosing schedules were consistent with current clinical practice guidelines for treatment of nonpsychotic major depressive disorder (31, 32). As might be anticipated in this design, most patients (N=29) were prescribed selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs). However, other standard acute-phase pharmacotherapy regimens were also used: tricyclic antidepressants (N=6), an SSRI plus a tricyclic (N=3), venlafaxine (N=2), an SSRI plus lithium (N=2), and a monoamine oxidase inhibitor (N=1). The 17-item Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (33) was used to assess severity of depressive symptoms at pretreatment and at the end of 6 weeks of treatment. Entry into treatment required having a pretreatment Hamilton Depression Rating Scale score ≥16. The Yale Depression Inventory, for which the reliability and validity of individual Hamilton Depression Rating Scale items have been previously reported (34), was used to provide a structured format for administering the Hamilton Depression Rating Scale. All patient assessments were conducted in person.

Comparison subjects (mean age=40.2 years, SD=11.6) were selected from New Haven respondents interviewed in 1981 for the Epidemiologic Catchment Area (ECA) study and tracked through the ECA mortality follow-up study in 1990 (35). Only those ECA participants who did not have a history of major depression at time of last contact were considered for matching and recruitment.

To establish a negative history for major depression or other exclusionary diagnoses in potential comparison subjects, we used a short-form version of a previously described modification of the Composite International Diagnostic Interview (36). This interview had been developed to assess the psychiatric status of community-based respondents. Blind comparisons with SCID interviews yielded a predictive value of 0.96 for screening out a lifetime diagnosis of major depression (36). Stringent application of the short-form version has shown similar results in screening out those who meet diagnostic criteria for major depression and for the other disorders listed as exclusions (37). Because of the importance of excluding comparison subjects with current major depression, the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D Scale) (38) was also administered to potential comparison subjects as an additional eligibility measure. A cutoff score of 16 on the CES-D Scale was used to identify nondepressed comparison subjects on the basis of prior work showing that specificity for identifying nondepressed individuals in a community sample was as high as 94% with the use of this threshold (39). Comparison subjects were administered the modified Composite International Diagnostic Interview by telephone; however, all other assessments, including the CES-D Scale, were conducted in person.

Comparison subjects were selected by random within age, sex, and race strata to match the patients with depression. Ages were matched within 4 years except for two subject pairs, who were matched within 6 and 7 years of age, respectively. Socioeconomic status was assessed using education and income level to classify respondents (40). This method allows for four possible levels of classification ranging from low, lower middle, upper middle, and high socioeconomic status. Demographic and clinical characteristics of the two study groups are provided in Table 1.

Measures

Acute stressful life events were assessed for both patients and comparison subjects by means of the Structured Event Probe and Narrative Rating Interview (41, 42). This interview provides a structured method of inquiry, similar to the Life Events and Difficulties Schedule (2), from which the undesirability and severity or disruptiveness of each reported event can be rated. The Structured Event Probe and Narrative Rating Interview allows for assessment of all possible types of life events, enhances subject recall (43), and reduces variability in rating the stressfulness of events by eliciting information about the context in which events occurred (2, 43). The context is constructed to take into account the meaning of an event on the basis of the particular circumstances surrounding the event for an average individual. However, the context does not include an individual’s personal reaction to an event; thus, the Structured Event Probe and Narrative Rating Interview reduces the potential to confound stressors with other variables (e.g., personality) that may prove to be risk factors (44).

Interviewers received extensive training in the use of the Structured Event Probe and Narrative Rating Interview before the onset of the study. Narrative reports of each reported life event were compiled by the interviewer and then rated, in terms of undesirability and severity, by judges (M.L.B. and S.C.J.) who were blind to subject status. Excellent interrater reliability was established across interviewers (undesirability of events, intraclass r=0.93; severity of events, intraclass r=0.93) and across judges (undesirability of events, intraclass r=0.93; severity of events, intraclass r=0.90). Life events assessed as adverse (i.e., undesirable and severe) were used in the analyses. Events were also classified as primarily interpersonal (e.g., death of a loved one) or related to achievement (e.g., loss of employment).

The patients were asked about events that occurred during the 6-month period before the onset of the current depressive episode. The comparison subjects were asked about events during the 6-month time period immediately preceding the interview. The 6-month time frame was used because it has been shown to be the optimal time period for detecting an effect of life events on subsequent depressive onset (45), and other studies have used this standard (46). The rate of adverse events for both groups is presented in Table 1.

The cognitive-personality styles of sociotropy and autonomy were assessed by using the Sociotropy-Autonomy Scale (47). The 30-item measures of sociotropy and autonomy were standardized and used as continuous variables by converting each to z scores. Prior work supports the stability of these characteristics over time (48). In addition, Sociotropy-Autonomy Scale scores were assessed at two time points (at entry into the study and at posttreatment) for 36 of the 43 patients. Scores were found to be stable over time for the 36 patients (sociotropy: t=–1.66, df=35, p=0.10; autonomy, t=–0.48, df=35, p=0.63, two-tailed tests) as well as for 16 of these patients whose depression had remitted at the posttreatment assessment (sociotropy: t=–0.83, df=15, p=0.41; autonomy: t=–1.61, df=15, p=0.12, two-tailed tests).

The technique developed by Hammen and colleagues (16) was used to test how the congruence between event type and cognitive-personality style predicted the onset of depression. Specifically, the term for the interaction between sociotropy and adverse interpersonal events was constructed from the sociotropy measure and a second term derived by examining the difference between the number of adverse interpersonal events and the number of adverse achievement events experienced by the subject. This second term provided a continuous measure of the preponderance of adverse interpersonal events. Similarly, the interaction between autonomy and adverse achievement events was studied by constructing a variable that used the autonomy measure and a measure of the preponderance of adverse achievement events (the difference between the number of adverse achievement events and the number of adverse interpersonal events experienced by the subject). The cognitive-personality styles and congruence with specific adverse life events for the two groups are presented in Table 2.

Models and Statistical Tests

Models were constructed to determine the effects of adverse life events, cognitive-personality characteristics, and the congruence of cognitive-personality characteristics and event type on 1) the onset of major depression and 2) the severity of depressive symptoms at the completion of a 6-week standard antidepressant drug regimen. A case-control, cross-sectional design was used to study the first specific aim, and a prospective design was used to study the second. Within each of these specific aims, two different models were considered.

Risk factors for onset

The probability of onset of a major depressive episode was modeled by using a logistic regression procedure. For model 1, the independent variables were the frequency of adverse events, the degree of sociotropy, the interaction between the degree of sociotropy and adverse interpersonal events, the degree of autonomy, and the interaction between the degree of autonomy and adverse achievement events. Patients and comparison subjects were matched on age, sex, and race; socioeconomic status was entered into the model as a control variable.

Although studies of cognitive-personality styles and event congruency have found an association between depressive symptoms and the interaction of negative interpersonal events and sociotropy, the match between autonomy and negative achievement events has been less well documented in predicting depression (17). One hypothesis for the lack of congruency between autonomy and events is that the assessment of autonomy measures more than a single cognitive-personality style (49, 50). Consequently, the values of the individual autonomy factors from the Sociotropy-Autonomy Scale in predicting depression were examined. These factors reflect an interest in preserving and increasing control over one’s life (i.e., need for control), individualistic achievement, and a solitary self-focused lifestyle (49, 51, 52). Only need for control was significantly associated with depression (Wald χ2=4.98, df=1, p=0.03). Consequently, we used this factor of the composite measure of autonomy in a second model. Model 2 was constructed from model 1 by replacing degree of autonomy with the 12-item factor need for control and by replacing the interaction between degree of autonomy and adverse achievement events with the interaction between need for control and adverse achievement events. The overall fit of each model was assessed by means of its chi-square based on the –2 log L criterion, and the significance of individual terms within each model was assessed by means of its Wald chi-square. For each model, the degree of association between predicted probabilities and observed responses was reported as a rank correlation index c, which is a measure of the proportion of instances in which the model accurately discriminates between patients and comparison subjects (53).

Prediction of treatment response

The level of depressive symptoms for patients at the end of the treatment phase of the study was analyzed by using general linear models. For model 1, the independent variables were the frequency of adverse interpersonal events, degree of sociotropy, the interaction between degree of sociotropy and adverse interpersonal events, the frequency of adverse achievement events, degree of autonomy, the interaction between degree of autonomy and adverse achievement events, episode status (first episode versus recurrent), and sex. In contrast to the onset model, events were divided into interpersonal and achievement events because preliminary analyses suggested that interpersonal events and achievement events appear to play opposite roles in predicting treatment response. Once again, model 2 was constructed from model 1 by replacing degree of autonomy with the factor need for control and by replacing the interaction between degree of autonomy and adverse achievement events with the interaction between need for control and adverse achievement events. The overall fit of each model was assessed by means of an F statistic, and the significance of each individual term within each model was assessed by means of a t statistic. Baseline Hamilton Depression Rating Scale score, age, race, socioeconomic status, and drug regimen were entered as control variables into both models. The design of the current study did not permit a comparison of response across drug types because antidepressants were prescribed on an individual basis by the treating clinician rather than by random assignment. However, drug type (SSRI, tricyclic antidepressant, SSRI plus tricyclic, or other pharmacotherapy) was entered as a control variable to account for possible differential effects by drug class in our analyses.

Results

A comparison of the two models constructed for the probability of onset of major depression is presented in Table 3. In model 1, only adverse life events and degree of sociotropy were significantly associated with a diagnosis of depression, whereas degree of autonomy and the interaction terms between the cognitive-personality measures and event types were nonsignificant. In model 2, however, adverse life events, sociotropy, and the autonomy factor need for control were significant, whereas both interaction terms were nonsignificant. Socioeconomic status was not significant in either model 1 (χ2=1.78, df=1, p=0.18) or model 2 (χ2=1.46, df=1, p=0.23).

The results for predicting severity of depressive symptoms after completion of 6 weeks of antidepressant treatment displayed a different pattern than that observed in the models for onset of depression, as illustrated in Table 4. In model 1, adverse interpersonal life events, the interaction between degree of sociotropy and adverse interpersonal events, the interaction between degree of autonomy and adverse achievement events, and male gender predicted better outcome, whereas adverse achievement events predicted a worse outcome. Degrees of sociotropy and autonomy as well as episode status were all nonsignificant. The control variables of baseline Hamilton Depression Rating Scale score (F=22.90, df=1, 26, p=0.0001) and drug type (F=3.57, df=3, 26, p=0.03) were significant in model 1, but socioeconomic status (F=0.22, df=2, 26, p=0.80), age (F=0.11, df=1, 26, p=0.75), and race (F=3.46, df=1, 26, p=0.07) were nonsignificant. In model 2, adverse interpersonal life events, the interaction between degree of sociotropy and adverse interpersonal events, the interaction between need for control and adverse achievement events, and being male were all significantly associated with better outcome, whereas adverse achievement events were associated with worse outcome. Degree of sociotropy, need for control, and episode status were nonsignificant. The control variables of baseline Hamilton Depression Rating Scale score (F=24.67, df=1, 26, p=0.0001) and drug type (F=4.31, df=3, 26, p=0.01) were significant in model 2. Socioeconomic status (F=0.36, df=2, 26, p=0.70), age (F=0.86, df=1, 26, p=0.36), and race (F=3.98, df=1, 26, p=0.06) were nonsignificant. In both model 1 and model 2, better outcome (lower Hamilton Depression Rating Scale score) was significantly associated with more interpersonal events, fewer achievement events, and a positive match of cognitive-personality style and associated event type.

In the context of the multivariate model, men had a better treatment response than women. Differences were not found between women and men in terms of the variables that significantly predicted treatment response except for a higher occurrence of negative achievement events in men. Despite the higher occurrence of these events, which independently predicted worse outcome, men appeared to benefit more from antidepressant drug treatment. Closer examination of responders by episode status (first episode versus recurrent) indicated an interaction of sex and first versus recurrent episode status within the model. Comparing the least squares means for these groups, we found that women with recurrent depression had a significantly poorer outcome than men with first-episode (t=2.94, df=25, p=0.007) or recurrent (t=2.28, df=25, p=0.03) depression and a nonsignificantly worse outcome than women with first-episode depression (t=1.49, df=25, p=0.15).

Discussion

Results of the current work indicate that occurrence of adverse life events and cognitive-personality styles are risk factors for onset of a unipolar major depressive episode. Both negative interpersonal events and achievement events were found to contribute to onset, and the model was enhanced if events were considered without regard to type (interpersonal versus achievement). Congruency between cognitive-personality style and event type in predicting onset was not found in this study. Some studies have found congruency between a sociotropic style and negative interpersonal life events in predicting depressive symptoms (1618, 54ε6), and one study supported a match between autonomy and negative achievement events (17). However, these studies of congruency almost exclusively used nonclinical study groups, most used checklist measures of life events, and none used a clinical diagnosis as an outcome. The current work differs from prior investigations in that we used a clinically diagnosed group relative to healthy comparison subjects, a comprehensive objective assessment of stress, and a multivariate model that controlled for other risk factors. Although we did not find that interactions between cognitive-personality styles and congruent stressors predicted depression, this does not preclude the possibility that there exists some other form of interplay between adverse life events, cognitive style, and depression. For example, we found that self-efficacy, a measure of perceived control over life circumstances, mediates the effect of negative stressful life events on symptoms of depression for those with prior depression (27).

The current findings are consistent with a large literature on the role of personality in precipitating major depression (e.g., references 57, 58), and the findings specifically support work by Clark and Beck (49), which showed that the cognitive-personality characteristic of sociotropy is significantly associated with depressed mood states. Results of our study indicated that depression was nine times more likely after a major adverse event and was almost three times more likely in the presence of cognitive-personality characteristics that emphasized either concern about disapproval or need for control. It is well known that major depression is not always associated with an antecedent adverse life event. The results of this study are consistent with that observation and suggest that depression might also be dependent upon one’s view of the world as characterized by a cognitive-personality style.

Research investigating cognitive-personality features that are comparable to sociotropy is limited. However, the concept of dependent personality is consistent with that of sociotropy and has been shown to be a risk factor for depression (57, 58). Also, the related concept of rejection sensitivity, which has been incorporated into DSM-IV as a symptom of atypical depression, is defined as “a long-standing pattern of extreme sensitivity to perceived interpersonal rejection.” Thus, it is clear by definition that this behavioral pattern actually would predate the onset of depression and might likely contribute to a depressive onset as well as describe a subtype. Further, two studies that examined interpersonal sensitivity (59, 60) both indicated that depressed participants had elevated scores on an interpersonal awareness factor, a factor that approximates the need for approval feature of sociotropy.

The finding that need for control was associated with depression was of particular interest to us because theoretical formulations and empirical studies have suggested that perceived lack of control over life circumstances is a critical component in the etiology of depression (28). Exposure to uncontrollable (versus controllable) stress has been found to result in behavioral and neurobiological effects consistent with depressive states in animal studies (29, 61, 62) and human investigation (63). Although the actual mechanisms by which these cognitive-personality styles may contribute to depressed states is not known, some have begun to theorize that believing one cannot control the effects of untoward events induces hopelessness, which is likely to be a proximal cause of depressive onset (6466).

In predicting treatment response, adverse interpersonal and achievement events appeared to play opposite roles in that interpersonal events were associated with better outcome, and achievement events were associated with worse outcome. The relationship of specificity of event type to outcome has not been previously studied, and so this finding cannot be compared to prior work. It would be of interest to know whether such specificity might account for at least some of the variability in previous reports regarding the effect of stressors on treatment outcome. One speculation regarding this finding is that those with more negative interpersonal events have larger social networks, and such individuals can avail themselves of support even in the context of negative interpersonal events emanating from that network. Such support may aid in ameliorating or coping with negative interpersonal events and, thus, facilitate antidepressant treatment.

The interaction, or congruency, of cognitive-personality style and event type predicted better outcome when individuals with higher degrees of sociotropy experienced negative interpersonal events and when those with a greater need for control experienced negative achievement events. Prior reports on the relationship of pretreatment life stressors to treatment outcome have not studied these interactions. Because this congruency predicts better outcome, it seems a particularly useful finding to pursue. One speculation regarding this finding is that a congruent event may be better understood as a personal vulnerability to depression, thus facilitating remedial cognitive and behavioral strategies that the patient may initiate in combination with antidepressant treatment.

Despite support for our findings, there are limitations to the current investigation. The examination of risk factors for depressive onset was conducted by means of a cross-sectional study and would be enhanced by a prospective examination of the relationship of cognitive-personality styles to depressive onset. Selection, assessment, and follow-up of nondepressed individuals for the purpose of identifying subsequent cases of diagnosed major depression would be costly and require a very large sample. Nevertheless, a prospective study would provide a more definitive test of cognitive-personality characteristics as a risk factor for depression. In addition, the effect of other variables implicated in the onset of depression and in response to treatment, such as social support, coping, and early trauma (67), were not included in our multivariate model. Future multivariate models should consider these variables and further investigate the effects of gender on outcome. In the context of the multivariate model that predicted outcome, women were found to have a worse response to 6 weeks of antidepressant pharmacotherapy after controlling for other factors related to outcome. More specifically, women with recurrent depression had a significantly poorer outcome than men with either first-episode or recurrent depression as well as a nonsignificantly worse outcome than women with first-episode depression. Finally, our data did not permit examination of whether treatment response was affected by the interaction of drug type by gender. Consideration of this interaction is warranted in future work, since there are emerging data that suggest that response to antidepressant drug type may vary by gender (68).

Multivariate models are needed to understand the complex interplay of factors that contribute to the development of major depression. The current findings indicate that it remains important to examine cognitive-personality characteristics as direct predictors of depression within such models. Cognitive-personality styles are learned patterns that are stable and long-lasting but potentially amenable to change (49). If cognitive-personality styles are modifiable risk factors, then preventive interventions directed at altering styles that pose a risk for depression could be developed. Our data also suggest that specific interventions, aimed at either reducing exposure to certain types of stressors or facilitating compensatory styles of coping in response to those stressors, might be particularly helpful for those at risk after certain types of events (69).

TABLE 1
TABLE 2
TABLE 3
TABLE 4

Received May 3, 1999; revisions received Sept. 3 and Oct. 19, 1999; accepted Nov. 24, 1999. From the Department of Psychiatry and Donaghue Women’s Health Investigator Program, Yale University; and the Department of Psychiatry, Weill Medical College of Cornell University, New York. Address reprint requests to Dr. Mazure, Yale University, P.O. Box 208091, New Haven, CT 06520. Supported by NIMH grant MH-52905 (Dr. Mazure).

References

1. Bebbington P, Tennant C, Hurry J: Adversity and the nature of psychiatric disorder in the community. J Affect Disord 1981; 3:345–366Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

2. Brown GW, Harris T: Social Origins of Depression: A Study of Psychiatric Disorder in Women. New York, Free Press, 1978Google Scholar

3. Paykel E, Myers J, Dienelt M, Klerman G, Lindenthal J, Pepper M: Life events and depression: a controlled study. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1969; 21:753–760Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

4. Roy A, Breier A, Doran AR, Pickar D: Life events in depression: relationships to subtypes. J Affect Disord 1985; 9:143–148Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

5. Shrout P, Link B, Dohrenwend B, Skodol A, Stueve A, Mrotznik J: Characterizing life events as risk factors for depression: the role of fateful loss events. J Abnorm Psychol 1989; 95:460–467CrossrefGoogle Scholar

6. Bebbington PE, Sturt E, Tennant C, Hurry J: Misfortune and resilience: a community study of women. Psychol Med 1984; 14:347–363Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

7. Brown G, Prudo R: Psychiatric disorder in a rural and an urban population, 1: aetiology of depression. Psychol Med 1981; 11:581–599Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

8. Brown G, Andrews B, Harris T, Adler Z, Bridge L: Social support, self-esteem and depression. Psychol Med 1986; 16:813–831Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

9. Campbell E, Cope S, Teasdale J: Social factors and affective disorders: an investigation of Brown and Harris’s model. Br J Psychiatry 1983; 143:548–553Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

10. Finlay-Jones R, Brown GW: Types of stressful life event and the onset of anxiety and depressive disorders. Psychol Med 1981; 11:803–815Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

11. Parry G, Shapiro DA: Social support and life events in working class women: stress buffering or independent effects? Arch Gen Psychiatry 1986; 43:315–323; correction, 1987; 44:168Google Scholar

12. Kendler KS, Kessler RC, Neale MC, Heath AC, Eaves LJ: The prediction of major depression in women: toward an integrated etiologic model. Am J Psychiatry 1993; 150:1139–1148Google Scholar

13. Lewinsohn PM, Hoberman HH, Rosenbaum M: A prospective study of risk factors for unipolar depression. J Abnorm Psychol 1988; 97:251–264Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

14. Beck AT: Cognitive model of depression. J Cognitive Psychotherapy 1987; 1:2–27Google Scholar

15. Clark D, Beck AT, Brown G: Sociotropy, autonomy, and life event perceptions in dysphoric and nondysphoric individuals. Cognitive Therapy and Res 1992; 16:635–652CrossrefGoogle Scholar

16. Hammen C, Ellicott A, Gitlin M, Jamison KR: Sociotropy/autonomy and vulnerability to specific life events in patients with unipolar depression and bipolar disorders. J Abnorm Psychol 1989; 98:154–160Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

17. Segal Z, Shaw B, Vella D, Katz R: Cognitive and life stress predictors of relapse in remitted unipolar depressed patients: test of the congruency hypothesis. J Abnorm Psychol 1992; 101:26–36Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

18. Robins C: Congruence of personality and life events in depression. J Abnorm Psychol 1990; 99:393–397Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

19. Billings A, Moos R: Life stressors and social resources affect posttreatment outcomes among depressed patients. J Abnorm Psychol 1985; 94:140–153Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

20. Lloyd C, Zisook S, Click M, Jaffee KE: Life events and response to antidepressants. J Human Stress 1981; 7:2–15Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

21. Monroe SM, Bellack AS, Hersen M, Himmelhoch JM: Life events, symptom course, and treatment outcome in unipolar depressed women. J Consult Clin Psychol 1983; 51:604–615Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

22. Monroe SM, Thase ME, Hersen M, Himmelhoch JM, Bellack AS: Life events and the endogenous-nonendogenous distinction in the treatment and posttreatment course of depression. Compr Psychiatry 1985; 26:175–184Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

23. Monroe SM, Roberts JE, Kupfer DJ, Frank E: Life stress and treatment course of recurrent depression, II: postrecovery associations with attrition, symptom course, and recurrence over 3 years. J Abnorm Psychol 1996; 105:313–328Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

24. Reno RM, Halaris AE: The relationship between life stress and depression in an endogenous sample. Compr Psychiatry 1990; 31:25–33Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

25. Monroe SM, Kupfer DJ, Frank E: Life stress and treatment course of recurrent depression, I: response during index episode. J Consult Clin Psychol 1992; 60:718–724Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

26. Peselow ED, Sanfilipo MP, Robins CJ, Block P, Fieve RR: Sociotropy and autonomy: relationship to antidepressant drug treatment response and endogeneous-nonendogeneous dichotomy. J Abnorm Psychol 1992; 101:479–486Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

27. Maciejewski PK, Prigerson HG, Mazure CM: Self-efficiacy as a mediator between stressful life events and depressive symptoms: differences based on history of prior depression. Br J Psychiatry 2000; 176:373–378Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

28. Seligman M: Helplessness: On Depression, Development, and Death. San Francisco, WH Freeman, 1975Google Scholar

29. Drugan RC, Basile AS, Ha JH, Healy D, Ferland RJ: Analysis of the importance of controllable versus uncontrollable stress on subsequent behavioral and physiological functioning. Brain Res Brain Res Protoc 1997; 2:69–74Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

30. First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JBW: Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders (SCID). New York, New York State Psychiatric Institute, Biometrics Research, 1995Google Scholar

31. Depression Guideline Panel: Depression in Primary Care, vol 2: Treatment of Major Depression: Clinical Practice Guideline Number 5: AHCPR Publication 93-0551. Washington, DC, Agency for Health Care Policy and Research, April 1993Google Scholar

32. Crismon ML, Trivedi M, Pigott TA, Rush AJ, Hirschfeld RM, Kahn DA, DeBattista C, Nelson JC, Nierenberg AA, Sackeim HA, Thase ME: The Texas Medication Algorithm Project: report of the Texas Consensus Conference Panel on Medication Treatment of Major Depressive Disorder. J Clin Psychiatry 1999; 60:142–156Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

33. Hamilton M: A rating scale for depression. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1960; 23:56–62Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

34. Mazure C, Nelson J, Price L: Reliability and validity of the symptoms of major depressive illness. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1986; 43:451–456Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

35. Bruce ML, Leaf PJ, Rozal GPM, Florio L, Hoff RA: Psychiatric status and 9-year mortality in the New Haven Epidemiologic Catchment Area study. Am J Psychiatry 1994; 151:716–721LinkGoogle Scholar

36. Kessler RC, McGonagle KA, Nelson CB, Hughes M, Swartz M, Blazer DG: Sex and depression in the National Comorbidity Survey, II: cohort effects. J Affect Disord 1994; 30:15–26Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

37. Kessler RC, Andrews G, Mroczek D, Ustun B, Wittchen H-U: The World Health Organization Composite International Diagnostic Interview Short Form (CIDI-SF). Int J Methods in Psychiatr Res 1998; 7:171–185CrossrefGoogle Scholar

38. Radloff LS: The CES-D Scale: a self-report depression scale for research in the general population. J Applied Psychol Measurement 1977; 1:385–401CrossrefGoogle Scholar

39. Boyd J, Weissman M, Thompson W, Myers J: Screening for depression in a community sample. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1982; 39:1195–1200Google Scholar

40. Duncan O, Featherman D, Duncan B: Socioeconomic Background and Achievement. New York, Seminar Press, 1972Google Scholar

41. Dohrenwend BP, Raphael KG, Schwartz S, Stueve A, Skodol A: The structured event probe and narrative rating method for measuring stressful life events, in Handbook of Stress: Theoretical and Clinical Aspects, 2nd ed. Edited by Goldberg L, Breznitz S. New York, Free Press, 1993, pp 174–199Google Scholar

42. Stueve A, Dohrenwend BP, Skodol AE: Relationships between stressful life events and episodes of major depression and nonaffective psychotic disorders: selected results from a New York risk factor study, in Adversity, Stress, and Psychopathology. Edited by Dohrenwend BP. New York, Oxford University Press, 1998, pp 341–357Google Scholar

43. Kessler R, Wetherington E: The reliability of life event reports in a community survey. Psychol Med 1991; 21:723–738Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

44. Paykel E: The interview for recent life events. Psychol Med 1997; 27:301–310Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

45. Surtees P: Adversity and psychiatric disorder: a decay model, in Life Events and Illness. Edited by Brown GW, Harris TO. New York, Guilford, 1989, pp 161–198Google Scholar

46. Brown GW, Harris TO: Depression. Ibid, pp 49–93Google Scholar

47. Beck A: Cognitive therapy of depression: new perspectives, in Treatment of Depression: Old Controversies and New Approaches. Edited by Clayton P, Barrett J. New York, Raven Press, 1983, pp 265–290Google Scholar

48. Moore RG, Blackburn IM: The stability of sociotropy and autonomy in depressed patients undergoing treatment. Cognitive Therapy and Res 1996; 20:69–80CrossrefGoogle Scholar

49. Clark D, Beck A: Personality factors in dysphoria: a psychometric refinement of Beck’s Sociotropy-Autonomy scale. J Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment 1991; 13:369–387CrossrefGoogle Scholar

50. Ouimette PC, Klein DN, Anderson R, Riso LP, Lizardi H: Relationship of sociotropy/autonomy and dependency/self-criticism to DSM-III-R personality disorders. J Abnorm Psychol 1994; 103:743–749Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

51. Arieti S, Bemporad JR: The psychological organization of depression. Am J Psychiatry 1980; 137:1360–1365Google Scholar

52. Blatt SJ: Levels of object representation in anaclitic and introjective depression. Psychoanal Study Child 1974; 24:107–157Google Scholar

53. Bamber D: The area above the ordinal dominance graph and the area below the receiver operating characteristic graph. J Math Psychol 1975; 12:387–415CrossrefGoogle Scholar

54. Lakey B, Ross LT: Dependency and self-criticism as moderators of interpersonal and achievement stress: the role of initial dysphoria. Cognitive Therapy and Res 1994; 18:581–599CrossrefGoogle Scholar

55. Robins C, Block P: Personal vulnerability, life events, and depressive symptoms: a test of a specific interactional model. J Pers Soc Psychol 1988; 54:847–852Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

56. Rude S, Burnham B: Do interpersonal and achievement vulnerabilities interact with congruent events to predict depression? comparison of DEQ, SAS, DAS, and combined scales. Cognitive Therapy and Res 1993; 17:531–548CrossrefGoogle Scholar

57. Hirschfeld RM, Klerman GL, Clayton PJ, Keller MB: Personality and depression: empirical findings. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1983; 40:993–998Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

58. Hirschfeld RM, Cross CK: Personality, life events, and social factors in depression, in Psychiatry Update: The American Psychiatric Association Annual Review, vol 2. Edited by Grinspoon L. Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Press, 1983, pp 382–406Google Scholar

59. Boyce P, Hadzi-Pavlovic D, Parker G, Broadaty H, Hickie I, Mitchell P, Wilhelm K: Depressive type and state effects on personality measures. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1990; 81:197–200Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

60. Boyce P, Parker B, Barnett B, Cooney M, Smith F: Personality as a vulnerability factor to depression. Br J Psychiatry 1991; 159:106–114Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

61. Weiss J: Stress-induced depression: critical neurochemical and electrophysiological changes, in Neurobiology of Learning, Emotion and Affect. Edited by Madden I. New York, Raven Press, 1991, pp 123–154Google Scholar

62. Minor T, Saade S: Poststress glucose mitigates behavioral impairment in rats in the “learned helplessness” model of psychopathology. Biol Psychiatry 1997; 42:324–334Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

63. Breier A: Experimental approaches to human stress research: assessment of neurobiological mechanisms of stress in volunteers and psychiatric patients. Biol Psychiatry 1989; 26:438–462Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

64. Alloy L, Clements C: Hopelessness theory of depression—tests of the symptom component. Cognitive Therapy and Res 1998; 22:303–335CrossrefGoogle Scholar

65. Hilsman R, Garber J: A test of the cognitive diathesis-stress model of depression in children: academic stressors, attributional style, perceived competence, and control. J Pers Soc Psychol 1995; 69:370–380Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

66. Metalsky GI, Joiner J, Thomas E, Hardin TS, Abramson LY: Depressive reactions to failure in a naturalistic setting: a test of the hopelessness and self-esteem theories of depression. J Abnorm Psychol 1993; 102:101–109Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

67. Weiss EL, Longhurst JG, Mazure CM: Childhood sexual abuse as a risk factor for adult depression in women: psychosocial and neurobiological correlates. Am J Psychiatry 1999; 156:816–828LinkGoogle Scholar

68. Yonkers KA: Depressive disorders in women, in Depression in Women Pocketbook. Edited by Yonkers KA, Steiner M. London, Martin Dunitz, 1998Google Scholar

69. Hollon S, DeRubeis R, Seligman M: Cognitive therapy and the prevention of depression. Applied and Preventive Psychol 1992; 1:89–95CrossrefGoogle Scholar