The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×
Regular ArticleFull Access

Premorbid Social Functioning in Schizophrenia and Bipolar Disorder: Similarities and Differences

Published Online:

Abstract

OBJECTIVE: This research examined social functioning in childhood and adolescence among patients with schizophrenia and patients with bipolar disorder compared with healthy subjects and investigated the relation between premorbid adjustment and risk factors for psychosis. METHOD: Maternal recall was used to assess the premorbid adjustment of patients with schizophrenia (N=70) and patients with bipolar disorder (N=28) recruited from a survey of consecutive hospital admissions for psychosis and of healthy comparison subjects (N=100) drawn from the same catchment area. RESULTS: The patients with schizophrenia had significantly poorer premorbid adjustment in childhood and adolescence than the comparison subjects and were impaired in both sociability and school adjustment. The patients with bipolar disorder exhibited poorer social impairment in adolescence than the comparison subjects, though to a lesser degree than the schizophrenic subjects, but functioned well at school. There were significant linear trends in the risk of psychosis with worsening premorbid functioning, which was most marked in the schizophrenic group, and a specific linear relation between low birth weight and poor premorbid adjustment among the schizophrenic patients. CONCLUSIONS: Impaired premorbid social functioning is not specific to schizophrenia and is seen also in bipolar disorder. The data support the view that poor premorbid social adjustment is one manifestation of vulnerability to adult psychotic disorders. These results are consistent with other findings pointing to early developmental deficits in patients who subsequently develop psychosis. (Am J Psychiatry 1997; 154:1544–1550)

Investigation of childhood precursors of adult psychosis will clarify our understanding of etiology and may aid strategies for early detection and treatment. Earlier investigations have shown that approximately one-third of schizophrenic patients exhibited obvious premorbid behavioral abnormalities (14). The premorbid adjustment of patients who develop affective psychosis has been studied less frequently. Studies directly comparing the premorbid functioning of schizophrenic patients and patients with affective psychosis have shown greater impairment in the schizophrenic subjects (58). Unfortunately, most studies of premorbid functioning in psychosis have lacked a healthy comparison group and have not been able to examine the distribution of childhood and adolescent social impairment among groups of patients compared with the normal population. In addition, few studies have controlled for possible confounding factors, such as sex, social class, ethnicity, and IQ, in the relation between premorbid social difficulties and schizophrenia. Prospective and retrospective cohort studies have found differences in childhood social and intellectual functioning between preschizophrenic children and the general population (912) but have not (with one exception [13]) found similar significant effects for children destined to develop affective psychosis. However, it has been suggested that poor premorbid functioning is a predictor of vulnerability to psychosis among patients with major depressive disorder (14, 15).

We used data from a large treatment group of psychotic patients to answer the following questions. 1) Do patients with schizophrenia and bipolar disorder show poor social adjustment in childhood and adolescence, compared with healthy subjects, after control for confounding factors? 2) How large are these effects, and which domains of functioning are impaired? 3) Are there temporal changes? 4) Are risk factors for psychosis related to premorbid functioning?

METHOD

One hundred patients with schizophrenia and 49 patients with bipolar disorder were recruited during a survey of all admissions for functional psychotic illness to three psychiatric hospitals in South London between March 1987 and October 1989. That survey, the Camberwell Collaborative Psychosis Study, has been described in detail previously (16). Inclusion criteria were age between 16 and 50 years and presence of psychotic symptoms in clear consciousness (17). Subjects were interviewed within 3 days of admission and diagnosed according to DSM-III criteria. Severity of illness was not significantly different in the two diagnostic groups. The mean age at first admission was 23.4 years (SD=5.4) for the patients with schizophrenia and 23.3 years (SD=5.7) for those with bipolar disorder; the mean number of hospital admissions was 3.5 (SD=3.5) and 3.8 (SD=2.9), respectively.

The comparison subjects were chosen to represent the population aged 16–50 years who would be admitted to any of the three index psychiatric hospitals if they became psychotic. Persons with minor injuries who came to the casualty department of Kings College Hospital, a large general hospital in the same catchment area, were identified as a suitable population from which to draw our comparison group. Potential subjects were approached in the waiting area, the purpose of the study was explained, and they were asked whether they wished to participate. Of 330 persons approached during the study period, 202 individuals (104 male and 98 female) were not willing or eligible to participate: 74 were “just not interested,” 32 were called away by the doctor or were too ill to talk, 31 did not want to involve their mothers, 63 were ineligible because their mothers were dead, non-English-speaking, or living abroad, and two were excluded because they suffered from schizophrenia and had previously been interviewed as possible case subjects. Written informed consent, including permission to contact their mothers, was obtained from the remaining 128 persons after a complete description of the study had been given. A brief interview to obtain basic demographic details was carried out in an adjoining room, and subjects were asked to complete the New Adult Reading Test (18) as an estimate of premorbid IQ. Social class was defined by parental occupation at the time of the subject's birth (19). Of the 128 individuals who completed the proband interview, 28 could not be included in the study because their mothers subsequently refused to participate. The final comparison group thus comprised 100 individuals for whom complete maternal interviews were available.

There were significant differences between the three subject groups in sex, social class, ethnicity, age, years of education, and premorbid IQ (table 1). The patients with bipolar disorder were more likely to be female and from social classes I and II than the schizophrenic patients or the comparison subjects and were older at the time of recruitment. The patients with bipolar disorder also had a significantly higher premorbid IQ than the schizophrenic patients but not the comparison subjects. The patients with schizophrenia had a lower premorbid IQ and fewer years of education than the patients with bipolar disorder or the comparison subjects and were less likely to be of Caucasian origin. These group differences were taken into account in the statistical analysis.

The mothers of all subjects who had completed the proband interview were asked to take part in the study. One hundred mothers of healthy comparison subjects, 70 mothers of schizophrenic patients, and 28 mothers of patients with bipolar disorder gave their consent to be interviewed. Interviews took place over the telephone or in the mother's home and were carried out by one of two interviewers (M.C. and A.F) who were blind to case diagnosis and to the results of the proband interview. Interrater reliability was assessed by means of a set of 10 audiotaped interviews; an interrater correlation coefficient of 0.71 was obtained.

The semistructured maternal interview consisted of three parts.

1. The Premorbid Social Adjustment scale (5), an adapted version of the Cannon-Spoor scale (20), was used to assess five areas of adjustment (referred to as items): sociability, peer relations, scholastic performance, adaptation to school, and interests. Each subject received a score for each item, rated on a 7-point Likert scale that ranged from 1 (excellent adaptation) to 7 (extremely poor adaptation) (appendix 1). Each item was rated separately for childhood (5–11 years) and adolescence (12–16 years).

2. The obstetric complications scale of Lewis et al. (21) was used to collect information on pregnancy and birth history. This scale was constructed for scoring a limited range of birth complications uncovered through retrospective parental reporting.

3. Information on family history of psychiatric illness in first-degree relatives was obtained with the Family History Research Diagnostic Criteria (FH-RDC) schedule (22), which involves obtaining information from one relative about all other family members.

In the statistical analysis, between-group differences in childhood, adolescent, and total Premorbid Social Adjustment scale scores were examined by one-way analysis of variance followed by post hoc t tests with Bonferroni corrections for multiple tests. Log transformations of Premorbid Social Adjustment scale scores were used to fulfill the assumption of equal variances. Change scores were computed by subtracting the childhood score from the adolescent score to examine premorbid deterioration with age (23). A principal-components analysis, with varimax rotation, was performed on the Premorbid Social Adjustment item scores from both age periods and for all three groups of subjects and revealed two factors with eigenvalues greater than 1, which explained 67.8% of the variance. Items relating to socialization, peer relations, and interests loaded onto one factor, sociability (eigenvalue=4.8). Items relating to school performance and school adjustment loaded onto a second factor, schooling (eigenvalue=1.9). Items from both periods that related to social adjustment (items 1, 2, and 5) (appendix 1) and to school adjustment (items 3 and 4) were then summed and examined as separate subscores, as they appeared to represent distinct domains of adjustment.

Since there were differences between groups in sex, social class, ethnicity, and premorbid IQ (table 1) and since these factors can influence social and school adjustment, these variables were treated as confounders. Logistic regression techniques were used to examine the association between Premorbid Social Adjustment scale scores and adult psychiatric outcome adjusted for confounding factors. Using the epidemiological methods of Jones et al. (24), we found that the distribution of Premorbid Social Adjustment scale scores in the comparison group was divided into four equal categories, and the scores of the patients with schizophrenia and bipolar disorder were examined in terms of where they lay within this distribution, with use of the lowest scores (i.e., best adjustment) as baseline. Results were expressed as adjusted odds ratios, tested with –2 log likelihood chi-square tests. Dummy variables were used to obtain odds ratios for each quartile, and a linear trend was tested by entering the quartile term into the model as a continuous variable. Analyses were performed with the statistical package Stata (25). Genetic and obstetric risk factors for schizophrenia were categorized as dichotomous variables, present or absent, and mean Premorbid Social Adjustment scale scores were compared between categories by means of t tests. Birth weight was divided into quartiles based on the distribution in the comparison group, and mean Premorbid Social Adjustment scale scores were calculated for each quartile. Standard linear regression techniques were used to examine linear trends of mean Premorbid Social Adjustment scale scores across quartiles of birth weight.

RESULTS

The schizophrenic patients scored significantly worse than both the comparison subjects and the patients with bipolar disorder on childhood, adolescent, and overall adjustment (table 2). The patients with bipolar disorder scored significantly worse than the comparison group only on adolescent and overall adjustment (table 2). The comparison subjects showed a negative mean change score, indicating that social adjustment improved in adolescence. In contrast, both patient groups exhibited positive mean change scores, indicating functional deterioration in adolescence. The schizophrenic subjects exhibited a greater deterioration in functioning than the bipolar subjects. There was no significant relation between Premorbid Social Adjustment scale scores and number of hospital admissions.

The schizophrenic patients scored significantly worse than the comparison subjects on both sociability and schooling (table 2). When the results were adjusted for the confounding factors by means of linear regression, the differences remained significant for both sociability (t=7.2, df=139, p<0.0001) and schooling (t=6.5, df=139, p<0.0001). The bipolar patients differed significantly from the comparison group only on the sociability subscore, not schooling (table 2). After adjustment for the confounding factors, the difference between the bipolar patients and the comparison group remained significant for sociability (t=3.8, df=105, p<0.0001) and just achieved significance for schooling (t=2, df=105, p<0.04). The bipolar patients did not differ from the schizophrenic patients on the sociability subscore, but their performance in school was significantly better.

There were significant linear trends for the schizophrenic patients and, to a lesser extent, for the patients with bipolar disorder to have scores that fell into the quartile indicating the most impaired social adjustment, and these linear trends persisted after adjustment for the confounding factors (table 3). The schizophrenic patients were almost 27 times more likely than the comparison subjects to have scores in the quartile for the worst social adjustment, and the patients with bipolar disorder were 5.6 times more likely than the comparison subjects to have scores in that quartile. The strong evidence for a linear trend in both groups means that this effect operates throughout the entire range of Premorbid Social Adjustment scale scores. There was no evidence of a bimodal distribution of these scores in any of the groups, and including a quadratic term did not improve the fit of the model.

Mean Premorbid Social Adjustment scale scores did not differ significantly between the subjects with a positive family history of psychosis and those with no family history of psychosis in any of the three groups separately or overall. Mean total Premorbid Social Adjustment scale scores did not differ between those with a history of one or more obstetric complications, as measured by the Lewis et al. scale, and those with no history of obstetric complications in any of the three groups separately or overall. However, a post hoc analysis, based on previous work (26, 27), found a significant linear relation between Premorbid Social Adjustment scale score and birth weight among the schizophrenic patients. Patients with schizophrenia in the lowest quartile of birth weight had the highest mean Premorbid Social Adjustment scale scores, and these scores decreased linearly over each quartile, indicating better premorbid adjustment with increasing birth weight (table 4). This linear relationship held for both the sociability and schooling domains of functioning. No such relationship was observed among the patients with bipolar disorder or the healthy comparison subjects (table 4).

DISCUSSION

The results of this study confirm a clear association between poor social functioning in childhood and adult psychosis. Significant differences in premorbid social ability and school functioning between the schizophrenic patients and the normal comparison subjects were demonstrated. A lesser, but still significant, difference was observed between the patients with bipolar disorder and the normal subjects in childhood social ability but not in school functioning. The risk of psychosis was spread throughout the study group such that the poorer one's social adjustment, the greater the risk of developing schizophrenia or, to a lesser extent, bipolar disorder.

The main strengths of our study design were the inclusion of a large group of normal comparison subjects assessed with the same instruments used for the patients and the use of mothers as the source of information on premorbid adjustment. Other studies have used chart reviews (2830) or interviews with patients (3, 31, 32), but we considered that mothers are best placed to give accurate retrospective information about a child's early development. However, there may be a problem of recall bias when such a source of retrospective data is used, namely, that the mother's knowledge of her child's adult outcome may influence her memory of childhood behavior.

We have considered this methodological problem from several viewpoints. 1) The patients with bipolar disorder and those with schizophrenia did not differ from each other in severity of adult illness, as measured by mean age at onset and mean number of hospital admissions. If mothers' replies were influenced by the current adult status of the offspring or the current perceived burden of care, then both groups of patients should have been rated more or less the same on childhood adjustment. In fact, the bipolar patients were consistently rated by their mothers as less severely impaired in childhood. 2) The public perception of schizophrenia as a more long-term and chronic illness than bipolar disorder may have influenced the mothers' expectations of a poorer prognosis in schizophrenia. If so, this factor is likely to be most important for a mother whose child has been admitted to the hospital for the first time and should become progressively less important with subsequent admissions. In our study, however, the mean Premorbid Social Adjustment scale scores of the first-admission patients did not differ significantly from the scores of the patients who had had more than one admission, indicating that maternal ratings of childhood adjustment are independent of length of time since diagnosis. 3) There is also the contrasting possibility of “idealization” of the childhood behavior of ill offspring among some mothers, which would tend to underestimate the differences found between subject groups and which may “neutralize” the possible sources of bias we have mentioned. 4) Finally, studies of premorbid functioning in schizophrenia that are free from recall bias, such as school record studies (3, 11) and studies of national birth cohorts (10, 12), show results similar to ours and support the validity of our findings.

Known risk factors for psychosis did not appear to influence premorbid social functioning in the patients with schizophrenia or bipolar disorder. We found no relation between premorbid social adjustment and genetic risk of psychosis in this study, thus replicating previous work (33 and manuscript by M. Taylor et al. submitted for publication). However, it is possible that use of the FH-RDC method to assess genetic risk may have led to underdetection of illness among relatives and reduced power to find an effect. Previous studies comparing the family history method with the family study method—a method that involves direct interview of all available relatives—have shown that up to 60% of relatives with major affective illness and 40% of cases of psychosis may be missed by using the FH-RDC (3436). We also did not find a relation between overall number of birth complications and premorbid adjustment, which again agrees with previous work (33). However, we did find a linear relation between low birth weight and poor premorbid functioning that was specific to schizophrenia. This finding adds to the evidence suggesting a continuum of abnormality extending from prenatal development through childhood social difficulties to adult schizophrenia (12, 26, 27, 3740).

In conclusion, our study shows that both schizophrenic patients and patients with bipolar disorder exhibit premorbid social maladjustment in comparison with a group of normal subjects, but there are some differences between the groups. The degree of functional deterioration among patients with bipolar disorder is not as severe as that seen in schizophrenic patients. In addition, bipolar patients show relative preservation of school functioning despite deterioration of social functioning in adolescence. Patients with schizophrenia, on the other hand, are impaired socially from a young age, with a marked deterioration during adolescence, and they show poor school adjustment, even when differences between groups in premorbid IQ are taken into account. We propose that poor social adjustment in adolescence is an early manifestation of vulnerability to adult psychotic illness. One possible explanation is that pathogenic processes, possibly of neurodevelopmental origin (41), leading to psychosis in adulthood, can also predispose a person to attentional difficulties, distorted perceptions, unusual thought processes, and decreased empathic ability, thus compromising social functioning. The reasons for the earlier onset and more global nature of the premorbid impairment in schizophrenia compared with bipolar disorder are not yet understood and would repay further study.

APPENDIX 1. Premorbid Social Adjustment Scale (Foerster et al. [5], adapted from Cannon-Spoor et al. [20])

Standardized entry questions are used for each item. Scoring is on a scale from 1 to 7 for each of the five items. Examples of the descriptive anchor points to aid scoring are shown, with corresponding scores given in parentheses. Each item is scored separately for childhood (5–11 years) and adolescence (12–16 years).

1. Sociability and isolation

Would you describe X between ages 5 and 11 as outgoing and liking the company of others or as shy and withdrawn?(1) Not withdrawn, active social interaction(3) Mild withdrawal, enjoyed socialization when involved—occasionally sought opportunities to socialize(5) Moderately withdrawn, given to daydreaming and excessive fantasy, did not seek contact(7) Unrelated to others, isolated, avoided contacts

2. Peer relations

Did X make friends easily during childhood? How many friends did X have? Were there any really close friends?(1) Many friends, close relationships(3) Casual friends only(5) Deviant friendship patterns: only friendly with children older or younger(7) Socially isolated, not even superficial relationships

3. Scholastic performance

What sort of student was X between ages 5 and 11? Did X come at the top or bottom of his/her class?(1) Excellent student, top of class(3) Average student(5) Failing all classes(7) Required special education

4. Adaptation to school

Did X get into trouble at school during childhood? How much and what kind of trouble?(1) Good adaptation, enjoyed school, no discipline problems(3) Fair adaptation, occasional discipline problems, not very interested in school(5) Poor adaptation, disliked school, frequent truancy and discipline problems(7) Refused to have anything to do with school—delinquency or vandalism directed against school

5. Interests

During childhood did X have many interests and hobbies? Did his/her interests involve others?(1) Active, involved in a range of school, sporting, and social activities and hobbies(3) Involved in one school, sporting, or social activity with other young people(5) Introverted interests—one or a few hobbies which required no contacts with others(7) No interests—withdrawn and indifferent toward interests of the average youngster

TABLE 1
TABLE 2
TABLE 3
TABLE 4

Presented at the joint meeting of the Association of European Psychiatrists and the Royal College of Psychiatrists, London, July 8–12, 1996. Received Sept. 24, 1996; revisions received Feb. 13 and April 15, 1997; accepted April 24, 1997. From the Department of Psychological Medicine and Centre for Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry. Address reprint requests to Dr. C annon, Department of Psychological Medicine, Institute of Psychia try, DeCrespigny Park, Denmark Hill, London SE5 8AF, United Kingdom. Supported in part by the Theodore and Vada Stanley Foundation and a Hayes Grove Priory Hospital Research Fellowship. Dr. Cannon was supported by a Wellcome Trust research training fellowship in clinical epidemiology. The authors thank the mothers who participated in the study.

References

1. Offord D, Cross L: Behavioral antecedents of adult schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1969; 21:267–283Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

2. Rutter M: Psychopathology and development, 1: childhood antecedents of adult psychiatric disorder. Aust NZ J Psychiatry 1984; 18:225–234Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

3. Watt NF: Patterns of childhood social development in adult schizophrenics. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1978; 35:160–165Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

4. Hartman E, Milofsky E, Vaillant G: Vulnerability to schizophrenia: prediction of adult schizophrenia using childhood information. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1984; 41:1050–1056Google Scholar

5. Foerster A, Lewis S, Owen M, Murray R: Pre-morbid adjustment and personality in psychosis: effects of sex and diagnosis. Br J Psychiatry 1991; 158:171–176Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

6. Gureje O, Aderibigbe YA, Olley O, Bamidele RW: Premorbid functioning in schizophrenia: a controlled study of Nigerian patients. Compr Psychiatry 1994; 35:437–440Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

7. Dalkin T, Murphy P, Glazebrook C, Medley I, Harrison G: Premorbid personality in first-onset psychosis. Br J Psychiatry 1994; 164:202–207Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

8. Van Os J, Takei N, Castle D, Wessley S, Der G, Murray RM: Premorbid abnormalities in mania, schizomania, acute schizophrenia and chronic schizophrenia. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 1995; 30:274–278Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

9. Angst J, Clayton P: Premorbid personality of depressive, bipolar, and schizophrenic patients with special reference to suicidal issues. Compr Psychiatry 1986; 27:511–532Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

10. Done DJ, Crow TJ, Johnson EC, Sacker A: Childhood antecedents of schizophrenia and affective illness: social adjustment at ages 7 and 11. BMJ 1994; 309:699–703Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

11. Lewine RRJ, Watt NF, Prentky RA, Fryer JH: Childhood social competence in functionally disordered psychiatric patients and in normals. J Abnorm Psychol 1980; 89:132–138Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

12. Jones PB, Rodgers B, Murray RM, Marmot MG: Child developmental risk factors for adult schizophrenia in the British 1946 birth cohort. Lancet 1994; 344:1398–1402Google Scholar

13. Van Os J, Jones PB, Lewis G, Murray RM: Developmental origins of chronic affective disorder: childhood risk factors in the 1946 British birth cohort. Arch Gen Psychiatry (in press)Google Scholar

14. Coryell W, Endicott J, Keller M: The importance of psychotic features to major depression: course and outcome during a 2-year follow-up. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1987; 75:78–85Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

15. Sands JR, Harrow M: Vulnerability to psychosis in unipolar major depression: is premorbid functioning involved? Am J Psychiatry 1995; 152:1009–1015Google Scholar

16. Jones PB, Bebbington P, Foerster A, Lewis SW, Murray RM, Russell A, Sham PC, Toone BK, Wilkins S: Premorbid social underachievement in schizophrenia: results from the Camberwell Collaborative Psychosis Study. Br J Psychiatry 1993; 162:65–71Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

17. Spitzer RL, Endicott J, Robins E: Research Diagnostic Criteria (RDC) for a Selected Group of Functional Disorders, 3rd ed. New York, New York State Psychiatric Institute, Biometrics Research, 1978Google Scholar

18. Nelson HA, O'Connell A: Dementia: the estimation of premorbid intelligence levels using the New Adult Reading Test. Cortex 1978; 14:234–244Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

19. Goldthorpe JH, Hope K: The Social Grading of Occupations—A New Approach and Scale. Oxford, England, Clarendon Press, 1974Google Scholar

20. Cannon-Spoor H, Potkin SG, Wyatt RJ: Measurement of premorbid adjustment in chronic schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 1982; 8:471–484CrossrefGoogle Scholar

21. Lewis SW, Owen MJ, Murray RM: Obstetric complications and schizophrenia: methodology and mechanisms, in Schizophrenia: Scientific Progress. Edited by Schultz SC, Tamminga CA. New York, Oxford University Press, 1989, pp 56–68Google Scholar

22. Endicott J, Andreasen N, Spitzer RL: Family History Research Diagnostic Criteria, 3rd ed. New York, New York State Psychiatric Institute, Biometrics Research, 1978Google Scholar

23. Kelley ME, Gilbertson M, Mouton A, van Kammen DP: Deterioration in premorbid functioning in schizophrenia: a developmental model of negative symptoms in drug-free patients. Am J Psychiatry 1992; 149:1543–1548Google Scholar

24. Jones PB, Harvey I, Lewis S, Toone BK, Van Os J, Williams M, Murray RM: Cerebral ventricle dimensions as risk factors for schizophrenia and affective psychosis: an epidemiological approach to analysis. Psychol Med 1994; 24:995–1011Google Scholar

25. Stata Statistical Software: Release 4.0. College Station, Tex, Stata Corp, 1995Google Scholar

26. Foerster A, Lewis SW, Owen MJ, Murray R: Low birth weight and a family history of schizophrenia predict poor premorbid functioning in psychosis. Schizophr Res 1991; 5:13–20Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

27. Rifkin L, Lewis S, Jones P, Toone B, Murray R: Low birth weight and schizophrenia. Br J Psychiatry 1994; 165:357–362Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

28. Asarnow JR, Ben-Meir S: Children with schizophrenia spectrum and depressive disorders: a comparative study of premorbid adjustment, onset pattern and severity of impairment. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1988; 29:477–488Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

29. Weinberger DR, Cannon-Spoor E, Potkin SG, Wyatt RJ: Poor premorbid adjustment and CT scan abnormalities in chronic schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 1980; 137:1410–1413Google Scholar

30. Von Zerssen D, Pössl J: The premorbid personality of patients with different subtypes of an affective illness. J Affect Disord 1990; 18:39–50Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

31. Gupta S, Rajaprabhakaran R, Arndt S, Flaum M, Andreasen NC: Premorbid adjustment as a predictor of phenomenological and neurobiological indices in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 1995; 16:189–197Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

32. Addington J, Addington D: Premorbid functioning, cognitive functioning, symptoms and outcome in schizophrenia. J Psychiatry Neurosci 1993; 18:18–23MedlineGoogle Scholar

33. McCreadie RG, Connolly MA, Williamson DJ, Athawes RWB, Tilak-Singh D: The Nithsdale schizophrenia surveys, XII: “neurodevelopmental” schizophrenia: a search for clinical correlates and putative aetiological factors. Br J Psychiatry 1994; 165:340–346Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

34. Andreasen NC, Rice J, Endicott J, Reich T, Coryell W: The family history approach to diagnosis: how useful is it? Arch Gen Psychiatry 1986; 43:421–429Google Scholar

35. Zimmerman M, Coryell W, Pfohl B, Strang D: The reliability of the family history method for psychiatric diagnoses. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1988; 45:320–322Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

36. Davies NJ, Sham PC, Gilvarry C, Jones PB, Murray RM: Comparison of the family history with the family study method: report from the Camberwell Collaborative Psychosis Study. Am J Med Genet Neuropsychiatr Genet 1997; 74:12–17Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

37. Hans SL, Marcus J, Henson L, Auerbach JG, Mirsky AF: Interpersonal behavior of children at risk for schizophrenia. Psychiatry 1992; 55:315–335Google Scholar

38. Quitkin F, Rifkin A, Klein DF: Neurologic soft signs in schizophrenia and character disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1976; 33:845–853Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

39. Fish B: Infants at risk for schizophrenia: sequelae of a genetic neurointegrative defect. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1992; 49:221–235Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

40. Walker EF, Grimes KE, Davis DM, Smith AJ: Childhood precursors of schizophrenia: facial expressions of emotion. Am J Psychiatry 1993; 150:1654–1660Google Scholar

41. Weinberger DR: Implications of normal brain development for the pathogenesis of schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1987; 44:660–669Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar