The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×
EditorialsFull Access

For Women With PMDD, Acute Changes in Ovarian Steroids, But Not Enduring Higher Levels, Precipitate Symptoms

The times they are a-changin’.

—Bob Dylan

In the seminal study by Schmidt et al. (1), the authors tested the hypothesis that the acute change in ovarian steroid levels versus stable levels of ovarian steroids above a critical threshold triggered symptoms of premenstrual dysphoric disorder (PMDD) after gonadotropin-releasing hormone (GnRH) ovarian suppression. They found that it was the change in levels of estradiol and progesterone from low to high, and not the steady-state levels, that was associated with the onset of PMDD symptoms. The findings have important clinical and theoretical implications not only for understanding the pathophysiology and treatment of PMDD, but also for brain function in its adaptive versus nonadaptive response to internal and external perturbations.

The investigators studied 22 women, ages 30–50 years, with PMDD, diagnosed by using DSM-IV criteria, prospectively confirming the timing and severity of mood symptoms over 3 months by means of a four-item visual analog scale for irritability—one of the most predominant premenstrual complaints before and after 40 years of age (2)—sadness, anxiety, and mood swings. After 2–3 months of GnRH-agonist-induced ovarian suppression with monthly injections of 3.75 mg of leuprolide, which after an initial stimulation suppresses ovarian function, 12 women who experienced symptom remission received 1 month of single-blind (participant only) placebo and then 3 months of continuous combined estradiol/progesterone. (Effects of estradiol versus progesterone were not differentiated in this study.) Outcome on primarily mood measures was assessed by using rater and self-ratings on the Premenstrual Tension Scale (3). Scores on these scales increased (the participants were more symptomatic) during the first month of estradiol/progesterone compared with all other months (last month of leuprolide alone, placebo, second and third months of estradiol/progesterone), and there were no significant differences in symptom severity scores (self or rater) between the last month of leuprolide and the placebo month or the second or third month of estradiol/progesterone, nor between the second and third months of estradiol/progesterone treatment, demonstrating that it was the dynamic hormonal events (i.e., the changing ovarian hormone levels), rather than the prolonged exposure to a threshold level of ovarian steroids, that was critical in triggering PMDD symptoms.

Although of different timing and magnitude, other examples of the brain responding to changes rather than amplitude—of ovarian steroids, other neuroendocrine hormones, or other internal or external stimuli (zeitgebers)—to elicit affective or behavioral changes abound: In women with PMDD and without PMDD, under the influence of progesterone in the luteal phase, melatonin circadian rhythms phase-delay (shift later) compared with the follicular phase, but only women with PMDD become symptomatic with this phase change in the luteal phase (4). Correcting the timing disturbance between melatonin and sleep by shifting the timing of melatonin by means of critically timed bright light interventions, or shifting the timing and duration of sleep by using wake therapy, improves mood in PMDD (46). In antenatal depression the increase of ovarian steroids, and in postnatal depression the decrease in ovarian steroids, is associated with the precipitation of mood symptoms in susceptible individuals (7). During the perimenopause, the increased onset and recurrence of mood disorders occurring then is associated with the change in reproductive hormonal status (8).

This phenomenon of neuroendocrine change eliciting mood and behavioral change also characterizes other hormonal systems and internal and external zeitgeber perturbations in other mood disorders. For example, in seasonal affective disorder the decrease in photoperiod (day length) during the winter, which elicits an increase in melatonin production that suppresses the hypothalamic-pituitary-gonadal axis in seasonally breeding species (9), in humans may trigger the onset of a major depressive episode in vulnerable individuals, particularly women during times of more rapidly changing photoperiods (10). Changing the decreased duration of the photoperiod either naturally, with the increased duration of photoperiod associated with the spring and summer months, or by administering bright light at critical times elicits antidepressant responses (11).

The distinct contribution of the study by Schmidt and colleagues, however, is to distinguish that at least in women with PMDD, it is the acute change in ovarian steroids, not the altered amplitude, that elicited the depressive mood changes. That distinction remains to be made with other hormonal, neuroendocrine, or zeitgeber signals, a challenging task indeed given the rigor and specific challenge paradigms employed by Schmidt et al. to test their hypotheses.

The mechanisms by which the acute perturbations in ovarian steroids elicit mood changes and why PMDD symptom recurrence is self-limited, with symptoms remitting despite continuing stable ovarian steroid levels (in subsequent months after the estradiol/progesterone addback), remain elusive. Perhaps relevant to the acute change in ovarian steroids in some women with PMDD is the suggestion by McEwen that in response to stress, epigenetic changes in brain circuitry occur, eliciting physiological responses that with allostasis can lead to adaptation, but that with a repeated dysregulated stress response lead to pathophysiology (12).

Modifying variables affecting response to neuroendocrine change may include, in addition to vulnerability for mood disorder, genetic predisposition and age. In the Schmidt et al. study, it would be helpful to know about the family history of mood disorder (in the responders versus nonresponders to the leuprolide and estradiol/progesterone interventions in women with PMDD) and whether symptom provocation during estradiol/progesterone addback (seven of 12 women) was a function of age, although there was a limited range in this study (30–50 years) and age did not distinguish leuprolide responders and nonresponders. Depressed individuals may behave more like aging individuals in their response to acute neuroendocrine changes. In the symptomatic luteal phase, but not the asymptomatic follicular menstrual cycle phase, women with PMDD, but not healthy comparison women, demonstrate a blunted response to light-induced phase shifts (13). Age affects light-induced phase shifting in behavioral rhythms in mice (14), although this effect has not been demonstrated in humans (15). Depressed, compared with healthy, elderly individuals, in response to the probe of sleep deprivation, have a more protracted course of recovery sleep that compromises the augmentation of sleep continuity and slow wave (restorative) sleep, reflecting an impaired recovery or reversal of sleep, perhaps analogous to the slower adaptation of hormonal change in some women with PMDD (16). The rapidity and duration of the cortisol response to a circadian signal also is affected by both age and gender (17). As Eastman has demonstrated in shift workers, although “squashing” the amplitude of the circadian temperature rhythm with bright artificial light produced a faster phase shift than “nudging” it, it occurred at the cost of increased fatigue and greater mood disturbance (18).

These observations have treatment implications: As in clinical practice, to allow certain patients to adapt to changes in medications, it is advisable to “start low and go slow” (referring to starting at a low dose and titrating upward gradually). Perhaps in women with PMDD a more gradual change in ovarian steroids might mitigate the provocation of symptoms elicited by acute changes, as does a dawn simulator (mimicking the gradual increase in light intensity at sunrise) in some patients with seasonal affective disorder (19). This approach may serve to shift the allostatic load toward adaption rather than pathogenesis. As in Aesop’s Fable “The Tortoise and the Hare,” the tortoises among us may just need more time to get to our destination!

From the Department of Psychiatry, University of California–San Diego, La Jolla, Calif.
Address correspondence to Dr. Parry ().

Dr. Parry reports no financial relationships with commercial interests.

The author thanks Fernando Martinez, B.A., for help with the references.

References

1 Schmidt PJ, Martinez PE, Nieman LK, et al.: Premenstrual dysphoric disorder symptoms following ovarian suppression: triggered by change in ovarian steroid levels but not continuous stable levels. Am J Psychiatry 2017; 174:980–989LinkGoogle Scholar

2 Kuan AJ, Carter DM, Ott FJ: Premenstrual complaints before and after 40 years of age. Can J Psychiatry 2004; 49:215Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

3 Steiner M, Haskett RF, Carroll BJ: Premenstrual tension syndrome: the development of research diagnostic criteria and new rating scales. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1980; 62:177–190Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

4 Parry BL, Meliska CJ, Martinez LF, et al.: Late, but not early, wake therapy reduces morning plasma melatonin: relationship to mood in premenstrual dysphoric disorder. Psychiatry Res 2008; 161:76–86Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

5 Parry BL, Mahan AM, Mostofi N, et al.: Light therapy of late luteal phase dysphoric disorder: an extended study. Am J Psychiatry 1993; 150:1417–1419LinkGoogle Scholar

6 Parry BL, Cover H, Mostofi N, et al.: Early versus late partial sleep deprivation in patients with premenstrual dysphoric disorder and normal comparison subjects. Am J Psychiatry 1995; 152:404–412LinkGoogle Scholar

7 Bloch M, Schmidt PJ, Danaceau M, et al.: Effects of gonadal steroids in women with a history of postpartum depression. Am J Psychiatry 2000; 157:924–930LinkGoogle Scholar

8 Schmidt PJ, Haq N, Rubinow DR: A longitudinal evaluation of the relationship between reproductive status and mood in perimenopausal women. Am J Psychiatry 2004; 161:2238–2244LinkGoogle Scholar

9 Reiter RJ, Vaughan MK, Rudeen PK, et al.: Melatonin induction of testicular recrudescence in hamsters and its subsequent inhibitory action on the antigonadotrophic influence of darkness on the pituitary-gonadal axis. Am J Anat 1976; 147:235–242Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

10 Rosenthal NE, Sack DA, Gillin JC, et al.: Seasonal affective disorder: a description of the syndrome and preliminary findings with light therapy. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1984; 41:72–80Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

11 Rosenthal NE, Sack DA, Carpenter CJ, et al.: Antidepressant effects of light in seasonal affective disorder. Am J Psychiatry 1985; 142:163–170LinkGoogle Scholar

12 McEwen BS: Allostasis and the epigenetics of brain and body health over the life course: the brain on stress. JAMA Psychiatry 2017; 74:551–552Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

13 Parry BL, Udell C, Elliott JA, et al.: Blunted phase-shift responses to morning bright light in premenstrual dysphoric disorder. J Biol Rhythms 1997; 12:443–456Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

14 Benloucif S, Masana MI, Dubocovich ML: Light-induced phase shifts of circadian activity rhythms and immediate early gene expression in the suprachiasmatic nucleus are attenuated in old C3H/HeN mice. Brain Res 1997; 747:34–42Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

15 Kripke DF, Elliott JA, Youngstedt SD, et al.: Circadian phase response curves to light in older and young women and men. J Circadian Rhythms 2007; 5:4Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

16 Reynolds CF 3rd, Kupfer DJ, Hoch CC, et al.: Sleep deprivation as a probe in the elderly. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1987; 44:982–990Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

17 Van Cauter E, Leproult R, Kupfer DJ: Effects of gender and age on the levels and circadian rhythmicity of plasma cortisol. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 1996; 81:2468–2473MedlineGoogle Scholar

18 Eastman CI: Squashing versus nudging circadian rhythms with artificial bright light: solutions for shift work? Perspect Biol Med 1991; 34:181–195Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

19 Avery DH, Bolte MA, Dager SR, et al.: Dawn simulation treatment of winter depression: a controlled study. Am J Psychiatry 1993; 150:113–117LinkGoogle Scholar