The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×

Abstract

Objective:

To understand the role of depressive symptoms in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease, it is essential to define their temporal relationship to Alzheimer’s proteinopathies in cognitively normal older adults. The study objective was to examine associations of brain amyloid beta and longitudinal measures of depression and depressive symptom clusters in a cognitively normal sample of older adults.

Method:

A total of 270 community-dwelling, cognitively normal elderly individuals underwent baseline Pittsburgh compound B (PiB) positron emission tomography (PET) measures of cortical aggregate amyloid beta and annual assessments with the 30-item Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS). The authors evaluated continuous PiB binding as a predictor of GDS score or GDS cluster, calculated as total scores and mean scores for three GDS item clusters (apathy-anhedonia, dysphoria, and anxiety-concentration), across time (1–5 years; mean=3.8 years) in separate mixed-effects models with backward elimination. Initial predictors included PiB binding, age, sex, Hollingshead score, American National Adult Reading Test (AMNART) score, apolipoprotein E ε4 status, depression history, and their interactions with time.

Results:

Higher PiB binding predicted accelerated rates of increase in GDS score over time, adjusting for depression history. Higher PiB binding also predicted steeper rates of increase for anxiety-concentration scores, adjusting for depression history and the AMNART score-by-time interaction. In a post hoc model estimating anxiety scores without concentration disturbance items, the PiB binding-by-time interaction remained significant.

Conclusions:

Higher amyloid beta burden was associated with increasing anxious-depressive symptoms over time in cognitively normal older individuals. Prior depression history was related to higher but not worsening symptom ratings. These results suggest a direct or indirect association of elevated amyloid beta levels with worsening anxious-depressive symptoms and support the hypothesis that emerging neuropsychiatric symptoms represent an early manifestation of preclinical Alzheimer’s disease.

Alzheimer’s disease begins with a long “preclinical” phase defined by the accumulation of brain deposits of fibrillar amyloid and pathological tau, a process spanning more than a decade before the onset of mild cognitive impairment (MCI) (13). Increasingly, observational studies have implicated depression and other neuropsychiatric symptoms as predictors of Alzheimer’s disease progression during this long preclinical period. Cognitively normal older people with neuropsychiatric symptoms, particularly depression-related symptoms and anxiety, have been found in multiple epidemiological cohorts (36) to be twice as likely to develop amnestic MCI, a prodromal phase of Alzheimer’s dementia, compared with those without these symptoms, over 3–6 years.

Alternative, and possibly complementary, models propose that depressive symptoms may be a causal factor accelerating Alzheimer’s disease progression, or they may be stigmata of the disease, even as early as “preclinical” stages (7). It is also possible that these roles differ by symptom or symptom cluster or across time and disease stage. Understanding these mechanisms is important for accurate classification and treatment of cognitively normal individuals at high risk for early Alzheimer’s progression and to differentiate these individuals from older adults with psychiatric symptoms distinct from Alzheimer’s-related processes.

A small number of studies have investigated the relationship of in vivo markers of amyloidosis to syndromal depression or continuous measures of depression-related symptoms using cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) Alzheimer’s biomarker analyses and positron emission tomography (PET) imaging modalities in samples of cognitively normal older people. In cross-sectional analyses, Pomara and colleagues (8) found that CSF amyloid beta 42 levels were reduced, as in Alzheimer’s disease, in cognitively intact elderly persons with late-life depression but not in those without depression, and that lower levels of amyloid beta 42 were inversely related to Hamilton Depression Rating Scale scores across the whole sample. CSF total tau and phosphorylated tau levels did not differ between the depressed and nondepressed groups. More recently, Babulal and colleagues (9) found no cross-sectional association of CSF Alzheimer’s biomarkers with continuous scores of mood disturbance as assessed by the Profile of Mood States–Short Form (POMS-SF) in a cognitively normal community-dwelling sample. Notably, higher baseline CSF tau–amyloid beta 42 ratio, but not other CSF markers, predicted 1-year increases in anxiety and total mood disturbance scores on the POMS-SF and in total score on the Neuropsychiatric Inventory Questionnaire, suggesting a dynamic relationship between Alzheimer’s-specific markers and changes in emotional tone. In related findings, mean cortical binding of fibrillar amyloid determined by Pittsburgh compound B (PiB) PET imaging was not associated with depression or other neuropsychiatric symptom measures at baseline but was positively associated with change scores on the 15-item Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS-15) over 1 year. Similarly, Harrington and colleagues (10), using PET radioligands to classify a large, nondepressed, cognitively normal sample into high and low amyloid beta groups, found that the high amyloid beta group was 4.5 times more likely than the low amyloid beta group to develop high categorical depression, defined by the GDS-15, after 54 months, in unadjusted analyses.

Together these recent findings suggest that cognitively normal older individuals with biomarker evidence of amyloidosis (principally high fibrillar brain amyloid beta levels) are more likely to experience rising depressive symptoms over time. At the same time, these preliminary observations raise more pointed questions as to the quality, severity, and time course of depressive symptoms that are most characteristic of preclinical Alzheimer’s disease and the specificity of their associations with Alzheimer’s molecular markers. To approach these questions, we investigated the relationship of brain amyloid beta burden, determined by PiB-PET, to longitudinal measures of depression in a cohort of cognitively normal older adults comprising individuals with a wide range of amyloid beta values, including a subset with a high amyloid beta burden consistent with preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. We hypothesized that higher amyloid beta levels would predict greater depression scores, even in a subclinical range, after adjustment for other potential confounders, including depression history. Building on previous work (11) that defined latent factors of subclinical depressive symptoms within the same cohort, we also examined associations of amyloid beta and depressive symptom clusters over time.

Method

Participants

Data were derived from the Harvard Aging Brain Study (HABS), an observational study of older adult volunteers, aimed at defining neurobiological and clinical changes in early Alzheimer’s disease. A total of 270 participants completed study visits over 5 years (mean number of visits, 3.8; range, 1–5). Participants were English-speaking, community-dwelling men and women, ages 62–90, who were cognitively normal and had no active major psychiatric disorders at the time of enrollment (11). A history of past or current depression adequately treated with standard antidepressant medication (selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors or dual serotonin-norepinephrine reuptake inhibitors, bupropion, mirtazapine, trazodone, or nortriptyline) was allowed. At screening, all participants scored below cutoff for mild depression, defined as a score ≥11 on the 30-item Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS) (12). Cognitively normal status was defined by a Clinical Dementia Rating (13) global score of 0 and education-adjusted normal performance on the logical memory subtest of the Wechsler Memory Scale (14) and the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) (15). The Partners Human Research Committee approved the study, and all participants provided written informed consent.

Clinical Measures

Baseline clinical assessments relevant to these analyses included the MMSE; the American National Reading Test intelligence quotient (AMNART) (16), a measure of premorbid intelligence; and the two-factor Hollingshead score, calculated according to primary occupation and educational attainment (range in the sample, 11–73; higher score indicates lower socioeconomic status) (17). Participants were classified by genotype as apolipoprotein E ε4 (APOEε4) allele carriers or noncarriers. Self-reported depression and treatment were elicited at baseline via history obtained by a study physician, followed by medical record review if necessary. Depression history was defined as a dichotomous variable in which participants with any self-reported depression, including past or current diagnoses, were classified together and compared with participants with no depression history.

Depression was quantified at baseline and annually using the GDS (item range, 0–1; total range, 0–30; higher score indicates greater depression) (12). In addition to calculating a total score for each time point, we also calculated an average score corresponding to each of three clusters of GDS items. These aggregate GDS items, the anxiety-concentration, apathy-anhedonia, and dysphoria clusters, were previously defined by principal component analysis using a HABS sample that was nearly identical to the baseline sample used in the present study (11) (see the Methods section in the data supplement that accompanies the online edition of this article).

All depression data were acquired in a blinded fashion with regard to other assessments and procedures.

PiB-PET Data

Fibrillar amyloid burden was measured using PiB-PET, according to established protocols, at the Massachusetts General Hospital PET facility (1821). PiB distribution volume ratio (DVR) was calculated according to methods established in previous studies that include a single region representing the aggregate cortical areas at risk for amyloid burden, across the frontal, lateral temporal, and lateral and medial parietal lobes (21, 22). Analyses used aggregate PiB DVR as a continuous measure. Participants and investigators were blind to all PiB data. PiB DVR data across the full range of values were analyzed.

Statistical Analysis

Unadjusted associations between baseline GDS total score, GDS cluster scores, and PiB measures were tested using Pearson correlations. Relations among categorical predictors were evaluated using chi-square tests, and differences in mean relations between categorical predictors and baseline numerical variables were assessed with Satterthwaite t tests.

Mixed random- and fixed-effects longitudinal analyses were run across time in the study for each of four dependent variables (GDS total score and average scores for the anxiety-concentration, dysphoria, and apathy-anhedonia clusters) in separate analyses, employing a backward elimination algorithm (p<0.05 cutoff) on an initial pool of fixed predictors and variances/covariances of random terms. During backward elimination, by convention, nonsignificant terms are retained in the model if higher-order terms subsuming them, e.g., interactions, are still in the model. The time predictor was the linear component of years in the study (preliminary graphs did not suggest curvilinearity). Fixed terms were baseline PiB DVR, age at baseline, history of depression (yes/no), AMNART score, Hollingshead score, sex, APOEε4 carrier status (yes/no), and the interaction of each of these predictors with time in study. Antidepressant use was not included as a predictor, as this substantially overlapped with depression history. Random terms were intercepts and linear slopes across time per participant, initially allowing for a correlation between them. Percent variance accounted for in the dependent variable by fixed and random predictors was computed.

SAS, version 9.4 (SAS, Cary, N.C.), SPSS, version 23 (IBM, Armonk, N.Y.), and R, version 3.3.2 were used.

Results

Demographic, clinical, and imaging data are summarized in Table 1 and in Figure S1 in the online data supplement. The mean GDS score at baseline was 2.8 for the whole sample and 3.7 for participants with a history of depression. Average scores for items corresponding to the apathy-anhedonia and the anxiety-concentration clusters were greater than for the dysphoria cluster (mean values, 0.161, 0.129, and 0.030, respectively, where these are equivalent to the mean proportion of cluster items that participants endorsed; see the Methods section in the data supplement). At baseline, anxiety-concentration scores were significantly but weakly correlated with the other cluster scores (for apathy-anhedonia, r=0.2, p<0.0001; for dysphoria, r=0.2, p=0.0003), whereas the other clusters were not significantly correlated with each other (for apathy-anhedonia and dysphoria, r=0.1, p=0.09). At baseline, PiB binding was not significantly correlated with GDS total score (r=0.08, p=0.2), anxiety-concentration score (r=0.03, p=0.6), dysphoria score (r=−0.01, p=0.9), or apathy-anhedonia score (r=0.1, p=0.06).

TABLE 1. Baseline Demographic, Clinical, and Imaging Data for Participants in a Longitudinal Study of Amyloid Beta and Anxious-Depressive Symptoms in Cognitively Normal Older Adultsa

Measure and Groupb
N%p
Female0.4
 Total sample15858.5
 No depression group13557.0
 Depression group2366.0
MeanSDRangep
Age (years)0.5
 Total sample73.66.163, 90
 No depression group73.7
 Depression group72.8
Hollingshead score0.9
 Total sample27.615.111,73
 No depression group27.4
 Depression group27.8
AMNART score0.07
 Total sample120.69.378, 132
 No depression group120.4
 Depression group123.7
MMSE score0.4
 Total sample29.01.125,30
 No depression group29.1
 Depression group29.0
N%p
APOEε4 carrier status (positive)0.2
 Total sample7529.3
 No depression group6228.0
 Depression group1339.0
MeanSDRangep
Amyloid-β (cortical PiB DVR)0.01
 Total sample1.170.1660.947, 1.820
 No depression group1.16
 Depression group1.24
GDS score (possible range 0–30)
 Total sample2.82.60,10
Anxiety-concentration cluster (possible range 0–1)
 Total sample0.1290.1560, 0.778
Apathy-anhedonia cluster (possible range 0–1)
 Total sample0.1610.1740, 0.857
Dysphoria cluster (possible range 0–1)
 Total sample0.0300.1010, 0.667
N%p
History of depression
 Total sample3513.0
Any antidepressant use<0.001
 Total sample3613.3
 No depression group198.1
 Depression group1748.6
SSRI/SNRI use<0.001
 Total sample269.6
 No depression group114.7
 Depression group1543.0

aAMNART=American National Adult Reading Test; APOEε4=apolipoprotein E ε4; GDS=Geriatric Depression Scale, 30 item; MMSE=Mini-Mental State Examination; PiB DVR=Pittsburgh compound B distribution volume ratio; SSRI/SNRI=selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor or serotonin-norepinephrine reuptake inhibitor. Mann-Whitney tests or chi-square tests were used to compute p values.

bThe group Ns were 270 for the total sample, 235 for the no depression group, and 35 for the depression group. Data were complete for all measures except AMNART score (missing for one participant) and APOEε4 carrier status (missing for 14 participants).

TABLE 1. Baseline Demographic, Clinical, and Imaging Data for Participants in a Longitudinal Study of Amyloid Beta and Anxious-Depressive Symptoms in Cognitively Normal Older Adultsa

Enlarge table

Thirteen percent (N=35) of the participants reported a history of depression: 7% of participants reported active depression within 2 years of enrollment (current depression) and 6% reported past depression but no active depression within 2 years of enrollment (past depression). Participants with and without a history of depression did not differ proportionally across sex or APOEε4 categories (Table 1). Participants with a history of depression, compared with those with no depression history, had a significantly higher mean baseline PiB binding (Table 1). More specifically, a significant difference in mean PiB binding was found between those with no depression history compared with those with current depression (1.16 and 1.28, respectively; p=0.006) but not compared with those with past depression (1.16 and 1.22, respectively; p=0.2).

Longitudinal Analyses

In the final model for GDS score, participants with a history of depression had a higher adjusted mean GDS score across time compared with those without self-reported depression (Table 2). The interaction of PiB binding with time was also a significant predictor in this model, such that higher baseline PiB binding associated with steeper increases in GDS score over time (Figure 1). Neither APOEε4 nor its interaction with time was associated with higher GDS score, and no other fixed terms were significant. Significant random terms were an uncorrelated intercept and linear slope across time. In a post hoc model, we repeated the final model, testing for possible effect modification based on APOEε4 status. Terms for the multiplicative interactions of PiB binding by APOEε4 status and PiB binding by APOEε4 status by time were added as predictors to the final model and were not significantly associated with GDS score.

TABLE 2. Longitudinal Mixed-Effects Model for GDS Total Scores, Measured Annually, Showing Fixed-Effects Predictors Retained in Final Model in a Sample of Cognitively Normal Older Adultsa

PredictorRegression Coefficient95% CISEp
PiB DVR-by-time interaction0.650.02, 1.280.320.04
Depression history1.360.35, 2.350.500.008
PiB DVR1.60–0.49, 3.741.080.13
Years in study (time)–0.56–1.31, 0.200.380.15

aGDS=Geriatric Depression Scale, 30 item; PiB DVR=Pittsburgh compound B distribution volume ratio. The regression coefficient is the unstandardized partial regression coefficient. For binary predictors, this is equivalent to the difference in adjusted means between the two groups. Model: R2=0.04 for fixed effects, p<0.0001; R2=0.82 including random terms, p<0.0001.

TABLE 2. Longitudinal Mixed-Effects Model for GDS Total Scores, Measured Annually, Showing Fixed-Effects Predictors Retained in Final Model in a Sample of Cognitively Normal Older Adultsa

Enlarge table
FIGURE 1.

FIGURE 1. Values Predicted From the Fixed Effects of the Best-Fitting Model for GDS Total Scores as a Function of PiB DVR and Depression History in a Sample of Cognitively Normal Older Adultsa

a GDS=Geriatric Depression Scale; PiB DVR=Pittsburgh compound B distribution volume ratio. GDS trajectories corresponding to PiB values one standard deviation above and one standard deviation below the mean value of PiB DVR (1.17) are shown with 95% confidence limits (shaded areas). Model predictions by history of depression are shown separately. Depression history is related to vertical elevation of lines across the whole span of the study, whereas PiB DVR is related to the slope of the lines; higher PiB DVR is associated with a steeper upward slope. (Corresponding spaghetti plots of raw longitudinal data for GDS are provided in Figure S2 in the online data supplement.)

As in the model for GDS total score, final predictors of anxiety-concentration scores included a main effect of depression history and the PiB binding-by-time interaction with effects in the same direction as before (Table 3). Lower AMNART score was also associated with steeper increases in anxiety-concentration score across time (Table 3). No other fixed terms were significant. Among random terms, only the intercept showed significant variance and was retained in the final model.

TABLE 3. Longitudinal Mixed-Effects Model for Anxiety-Concentration Scores, Measured Annually, Showing Fixed-Effect Predictors Retained in Final Model in a Sample of Cognitively Normal Older Adultsa

PredictorRegression Coefficient95% CISEp
PiB DVR-by-time interaction0.040.007, 0.0670.0150.015
Depression history0.0560.003, 0.1070.0260.04
PiB DVR0.056–0.059, 0.1720.0590.34
AMNART score-by-time interaction–0.0007–0.001, –0.000020.00030.045
Years in study (time)0.043–0.041, 0.1270.0420.31
AMNART score0.0002–0.002, 0.0020.0010.81

aAMNART=American National Adult Reading Test; PiB DVR=Pittsburgh compound B distribution volume ratio. The regression coefficient is the unstandardized partial regression coefficient. For binary predictors, this is equivalent to the difference in adjusted means between the two groups. Model: R2=0.03 for fixed effects, p<0.0001; R2=0.71 including random terms, p<0.0001.

TABLE 3. Longitudinal Mixed-Effects Model for Anxiety-Concentration Scores, Measured Annually, Showing Fixed-Effect Predictors Retained in Final Model in a Sample of Cognitively Normal Older Adultsa

Enlarge table

To examine the possibility that the relationship of PiB binding to anxiety-concentration scores was specifically attributable to two GDS items related to concentration (“Is your mind as clear as it used to be?” and “Do you have trouble concentrating?”), we calculated an average anxiety-only score by excluding these two items. In a secondary model analyzing anxiety-only scores, effects were virtually the same as before (for the PiB binding-by-time interaction, p=0.03; for depression history, p=0.02; for fixed effects, R2=0.02; with random terms included, R2=0.7, p<0.0001), except that the AMNART score-by-time interaction term was no longer predictive in this model.

In the model for dysphoria scores, history of depression was associated with a higher adjusted mean for dysphoria scores across time compared with no depression history. No other fixed terms were significant except for a positive linear effect of time indicating greater dysphoria scores over time (for depression history, p<0.0001; for time, p=0.003; for fixed effects, R2=0.04; with random terms included, R2=0.72, p<0.0001). Significant random terms were an uncorrelated intercept and linear slope across time.

For the final model for apathy-anhedonia scores, significant predictors included an interaction of age with time (older age raised the trajectory of apathy-anhedonia scores), an interaction of Hollingshead score with time (lower socioeconomic status lowered the trajectory of symptoms across time), and a main effect of AMNART score (higher cognitive reserve was associated with greater apathy-anhedonia scores across time) (Table 4). Among random terms, only the intercept showed significant variance and was retained in the final model.

TABLE 4. Longitudinal Mixed-Effects Model for Apathy-Anhedonia Scores, Measured Annually, Showing Fixed-Effect Predictors Retained in Final Model in a Sample of Cognitively Normal Older Adultsa

PredictorRegression Coefficient95% CISEp
Age-by-time interaction0.0010.0001, 0.0020.00050.03
Hollingshead score-by-time interaction–0.0005–0.0008, –0.000080.00020.02
AMNART score0.0030.0008, 0.0060.0010.009
Age0.002–0.002, 0.0050.0020.39
Hollingshead score0.0007–0.0008, 0.0020.00080.37
Years in study (time)–0.06–0.13, 0.0170.040.13

aAMNART=American National Adult Reading Test. The regression coefficient is the unstandardized partial regression coefficient. For binary predictors, this is equivalent to the difference in adjusted means between the two groups. Model: R2=0.05 for fixed effects, p<0.0001; R2=0.75 including random terms, p<0.0001.

TABLE 4. Longitudinal Mixed-Effects Model for Apathy-Anhedonia Scores, Measured Annually, Showing Fixed-Effect Predictors Retained in Final Model in a Sample of Cognitively Normal Older Adultsa

Enlarge table

Residuals from predictions of the random and fixed terms for all final models reasonably conformed to assumptions of normality and homogeneity of variance. For the dysphoria model, however, residuals from the fixed-term predictions alone were somewhat positively skewed because of the floor effect of zero values in the distribution of dysphoria scores.

Discussion

We examined the relationship of brain amyloid beta burden to longitudinal measures of depression in a community-based sample of cognitively normal older people and found that higher baseline amyloidosis was associated with worsening depressive symptoms over time. Higher brain amyloid beta levels were associated with increasing anxious-depressive symptom ratings rather than with ratings on symptoms related to dysphoria or apathy-anhedonia in this sample, suggesting that this particular dimension of depressive symptoms may be most useful as an early, dynamic marker in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease.

Our results are consistent with recent findings from two other clinical imaging cohort studies. Investigators at the Washington University Alzheimer’s Disease Research Center reported (9) that cognitively normal participants with a high baseline amyloid beta burden had greater change scores on the GDS-15 over 1 year compared with those with a low amyloid beta burden, adjusting for age, sex, and education. Similarly, the Australian Imaging, Biomarkers, and Lifestyle research group found (10) that cognitively normal participants with high amyloid beta levels were over four times more likely to develop categorical depression (defined by the standard GDS-15 cutoff) at 54 months compared with those with low amyloid beta levels, in unadjusted analyses. As in the HABS cohort, these samples included some participants with a depression history and/or antidepressant medication use and GDS scores at baseline were generally low (9) or in a subclinical range (10). No significant differences in GDS score or other mood disturbance measures were found between high and low amyloid beta groups at baseline in either study.

While significant cross-sectional associations of brain amyloid beta and depression were not found in these cognitively normal cohorts using amyloid beta–specific PET ligands, Yasuno and colleagues reported (23) an age- and education-adjusted association of PiB-PET-derived amyloid beta measures and low-range GDS-15 scores among cognitively normal older individuals with elevated PiB retention (only values within the highest two tertiles for the sample were analyzed). This sample excluded individuals with a depression history or antidepressant use, reducing the likelihood of reverse causation and providing indirect support for high amyloid beta levels as a factor that precedes subclinical depressive symptoms.

We provide further biomarker evidence for depression-related symptoms as outcomes of Alzheimer’s pathological changes at the preclinical stage and, in this case, independent of previously diagnosed depression. Regardless of depression history, higher amyloid beta burden predicted de novo or rising depression-spectrum symptoms in the near term that may lead to clinical depression over the longer term. Notably, 7% of the HABS sample reported a current diagnosis of depression, and the unadjusted mean PiB value was significantly higher in these participants compared with those with no depression history. This points to amyloid beta–related brain changes as a possible etiological basis for clinical depression in a subset of these participants. Early expansion of amyloid beta, interacting with tau, within medial temporal lobe structures such as the entorhinal cortex, may affect activity in functionally coupled limbic and neocortical regions, resulting in changes in emotional regulation (24). The emergence of these symptoms in individuals with high amyloid beta levels may also coincide with progressive amyloid beta deposition and local neurodegeneration within subcortical structures and circuits involved in emotional responses, such as anxiety (25, 26).

Theoretical constructs of depression and other neuropsychiatric symptoms in neurodegenerative disorders, along with instruments for their measurement, continue to evolve (2729). In line with recent consensus criteria, anxiety may be a symptom of emotional dysregulation in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease that could anticipate syndromal depression or other changes in emotion, temperament, and behavior, as encompassed in the newly defined mild behavioral impairment construct (29).

Neuropsychiatric symptoms may be most useful as clinical or prognostic markers in cognitively normal older individuals with evidence of other biological risk factors or as sentinels of decline (30). Holmes and colleagues (31) found higher anxiety subscale scores, assessed by the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale, in APOEε4 carriers compared with noncarriers, specifically within the subgroup of cognitively normal older people with high amyloid beta levels. No main effect of amyloid beta group with anxiety was reported. In a subset of the HABS cohort, we previously reported (32) that greater amyloid beta burden was associated with higher self-reported loneliness, a novel neuropsychiatric symptom measured by the three-item UCLA Loneliness Scale, after adjustment for anxiety scores on the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale and other demographic and psychosocial factors. This effect was also found to be stronger in APOEε4 carriers compared with noncarriers.

We found no main effect of APOEε4 in models for GDS total score or anxiety-concentration scores, and in a post hoc model, no interaction effect of APOEε4 status and amyloid beta level on longitudinal GDS scores. Similarly, Locke and colleagues (33) found no direct effect of APOEε4 carrier status on longitudinal depression scores in more than 600 cognitively normal adults, ages 21–86, followed for nearly 8 years. Given the low endorsement of depressive symptoms in our cohort and the confounded relationship of amyloid beta level and APOEε4 status, this post hoc analysis may have lacked sufficient power to detect a true relationship between the amyloid beta-by-APOEε4 interaction and the low-range GDS scores in these analyses.

Low educational attainment and income are established risk factors for greater depressive symptom burden in community-dwelling older people (34, 35), whereas in our models cognitive reserve and socioeconomic status did not predict total GDS scores or anxiety-only scores. This lack of effect may be attributable to the relatively high education and socioeconomic status of the sample, which diminished the influence of these factors. In other results, we found associations of higher cognitive reserve and socioeconomic status with greater apathy-anhedonia scores, controlling for age and time, in the opposite direction to expectations (35, 36). These results most likely reflect a survivor effect specific to this cognitively normal sample.

It is important to acknowledge that the fixed effects in these models, such as the PiB binding-by-time interaction and depression history, account for a small proportion of the variance for depression scores over time and that the magnitude of this effect also appears to be small. At the same time, it is plausible that the strength of these relationships was attenuated by antidepressant medication use. While these findings suggest that amyloid beta may play a role in the pathogenesis of certain forms of late-life depression, this observed relationship may be limited or indirect. Individuals with the highest levels of amyloid beta are also likely to have tau accumulation and neurodegenerative brain changes that may mediate the reported relationship between higher amyloid beta levels and rising depression scores (37). If not directly biologically determined, these symptoms may also be a psychological reaction to other subtle cognitive or somatic changes and stresses occurring in late-stage preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Finally, as multiple and heterogeneous factors may have an impact on depression within an individual and across a sample, psychosocial factors and pathogenic processes such as vascular disease and stress-related mechanisms may prove to be stronger predictors of late-life depressive symptoms than amyloid beta level. Additional studies in both clinically depressed and nonclinical samples are needed to determine whether depressive-spectrum symptoms have sufficient specificity and power to meaningfully inform preclinical Alzheimer’s disease assessment in routine screening or in selected groups.

There are limitations to this study. The relationships observed in these analyses were likely affected by study exclusion criteria, including restricted GDS scores at screening. For example, dysphoric symptoms were infrequently endorsed in this sample, limiting our ability to detect associations with amyloid beta. Individuals with major psychiatric disorders other than mild remitted depression or with active medical and neurological conditions were excluded from the HABS cohort, thereby focusing and limiting the external validity of these findings to older persons with relatively good mental and brain health. Sample size limited our ability to analyze potential moderating effects of antidepressant medications, and, among participants with a depression history, relevant characteristics such as recency of depressive episode and age at onset were not analyzed. Finally, the backward elimination method is sometimes criticized for being too liberal given that multiple runs produce multiple p values. For each dependent variable, however, significant effects found for PiB binding and other predictors of interest in the final model were also significant, or marginally so, in the initial full predictor set. Removal of extraneous terms mostly served to produce parsimonious models.

Conclusions

Higher brain amyloid beta burden was associated with increasing anxious-depressive symptoms over time in cognitively normal older adults. Prior depression history was related to higher but not worsening symptom ratings. These results suggest a direct or indirect association of elevated amyloid beta levels with worsening anxious-depressive symptoms and provide support for the hypothesis that emerging neuropsychiatric symptoms represent an early manifestation of preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Further longitudinal follow-up is necessary to determine whether these escalating depressive symptoms give rise to clinical depression and/or MCI and dementia stages of Alzheimer’s disease over an extended period.

From the Center for Alzheimer Research and Treatment, Department of Neurology, Brigham and Women’s Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston; the Department of Psychiatry, Brigham and Women’s Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston; the Division of Geriatric Psychiatry, McLean Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Belmont, Mass.; the Department of Neurology, Cliniques Universitaires Saint-Luc, Institute of Neurosciences, Université Catholique de Louvain, Brussels; the Department of Psychiatry, the Department of Neurology, and the Department of Radiology, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston.
Address correspondence to Dr. Donovan ().

Supported by the National Institute on Aging (grants R03 AG045080, R01 AG027435, K24 AG035007), the Harvard Medical School Department of Psychiatry Dupont-Warren Fellowship and Livingston Award, the Rogers Family Foundation, the Massachusetts Alzheimer’s Disease Research Center (grant P50 AG005134), and the Harvard Aging Brain Study (grants P01 AGO36694, R01 AG037497).

Dr. Donovan has received salary support from Eisai and Eli Lilly; her spouse is employed by Alkermes. Dr. Marshall has received salary support from Eisai and Eli Lilly and has served as a consultant for Halloran and GliaCure. Dr. Hanseeuw has received grants from the Belgian National Science Foundation (grant SPD28094292) and the Belgian Foundation for Alzheimer Research (grant P16008) and has served on an advisory board as a paid consultant for GE Healthcare. Dr. Rentz has served as a consultant for Eli Lilly, Janssen Alzheimer Immunotherapy, Biogen Idek, and Lundbeck Pharmaceuticals, and she has served on the scientific advisory board for Neurotrack. Dr. Johnson has served as a consultant for AbbVie, AZtherapies, Biogen, Bristol-Myers Squibb, GE Healthcare, Genentech, Genzyme, Isis Pharmaceuticals, Janssen Alzheimer’s Immunotherapy, Lundbeck, Merck, Novartis, Piramal, Roche, and Siemens Medical Solutions; he is a site principal investigator or co-investigator for Biogen, Eisai, Eli Lilly/Avid, Janssen Alzheimer Immunotherapy, Merck, and Navidea clinical trials; and he has spoken at symposia sponsored by GE Healthcare, Janssen Alzheimer’s Immunotherapy, Lundbeck, and Pfizer. Dr. Sperling has served as a consultant for AbbVie, Biogen, Bracket, GE, Genentech, Lundbeck, Merck, Otsuka, Pfizer, Roche, and Sanofi; she has served as a co-investigator for Avid, Eli Lilly, and Janssen Alzheimer Immunotherapy clinical trials; she has spoken at symposia sponsored by Biogen, Eli Lilly, and Janssen Alzheimer Immunotherapy; and her spouse has received payments from Lundbeck, Novartis, Piramal Healthcare, and Siemens. The other authors report no financial relationships with commercial interests.

References

1 Jack CR Jr, Knopman DS, Jagust WJ, et al.: Tracking pathophysiological processes in Alzheimer’s disease: an updated hypothetical model of dynamic biomarkers. Lancet Neurol 2013; 12:207–216Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

2 Sperling RA, Aisen PS, Beckett LA, et al.: Toward defining the preclinical stages of Alzheimer’s disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging–Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement 2011; 7:280–292Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

3 Geda YE, Knopman DS, Mrazek DA, et al.: Depression, apolipoprotein E genotype, and the incidence of mild cognitive impairment: a prospective cohort study. Arch Neurol 2006; 63:435–440Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

4 Steenland K, Karnes C, Seals R, et al.: Late-life depression as a risk factor for mild cognitive impairment or Alzheimer’s disease in 30 US Alzheimer’s disease centers. J Alzheimers Dis 2012; 31:265–275Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

5 Geda YE, Roberts RO, Mielke MM, et al.: Baseline neuropsychiatric symptoms and the risk of incident mild cognitive impairment: a population-based study. Am J Psychiatry 2014; 171:572–581LinkGoogle Scholar

6 Donovan NJ, Amariglio RE, Zoller AS, et al.: Subjective cognitive concerns and neuropsychiatric predictors of progression to the early clinical stages of Alzheimer disease. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2014; 22:1642–1651Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

7 Peters ME, Lyketsos CG: Beyond memory: a focus on the other neuropsychiatric symptoms of dementia. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2015; 23:115–118Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

8 Pomara N, Bruno D, Sarreal AS, et al.: Lower CSF amyloid beta peptides and higher F2-isoprostanes in cognitively intact elderly individuals with major depressive disorder. Am J Psychiatry 2012; 169:523–530LinkGoogle Scholar

9 Babulal GM, Ghoshal N, Head D, et al.: Mood changes in cognitively normal older adults are linked to Alzheimer disease biomarker levels. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2016; 24:1095–1104Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

10 Harrington KD, Gould E, Lim YY, et al.: Amyloid burden and incident depressive symptoms in cognitively normal older adults. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2017; 32:455–446Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

11 Donovan NJ, Hsu DC, Dagley AS, et al.: Depressive symptoms and biomarkers of Alzheimer’s disease in cognitively normal older adults. J Alzheimers Dis 2015; 46:63–73Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

12 Yesavage JA, Brink TL, Rose TL, et al.: Development and validation of a geriatric depression screening scale: a preliminary report. J Psychiatr Res 1982-1983; 17:37–49Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

13 Morris JC: The Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR): current version and scoring rules. Neurology 1993; 43:2412–2414Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

14 Weschler D: WMS-R Weschler Memory Scale Revised Manual. New York, Psychological Corporation, Harcourt Brace Jovanovich, 1987Google Scholar

15 Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR: “Mini-mental state”: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 1975; 12:189–198Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

16 Paolo AM, Ryan JJ: Generalizability of two methods of estimating premorbid intelligence in the elderly. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 1992; 7:135–143Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

17 Hollingshead AB: Two Factor Index of Social Position. New Haven, Conn, Yale University, 1957Google Scholar

18 Becker JA, Hedden T, Carmasin J, et al.: Amyloid-β associated cortical thinning in clinically normal elderly. Ann Neurol 2011; 69:1032–1042Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

19 Klunk WE, Engler H, Nordberg A, et al.: Imaging brain amyloid in Alzheimer’s disease with Pittsburgh compound-B. Ann Neurol 2004; 55:306–319Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

20 Hedden T, Mormino EC, Amariglio RE, et al.: Cognitive profile of amyloid burden and white matter hyperintensities in cognitively normal older adults. J Neurosci 2012; 32:16233–16242Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

21 Johnson KA, Gregas M, Becker JA, et al.: Imaging of amyloid burden and distribution in cerebral amyloid angiopathy. Ann Neurol 2007; 62:229–234Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

22 Raji CA, Becker JT, Tsopelas ND, et al.: Characterizing regional correlation, laterality, and symmetry of amyloid deposition in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease with Pittsburgh compound B. J Neurosci Methods 2008; 172:277–282Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

23 Yasuno F, Kazui H, Morita N, et al.: High amyloid-β deposition related to depressive symptoms in older individuals with normal cognition: a pilot study. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2016; 31:920–928Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

24 Leal SL, Noche JA, Murray EA, et al.: Disruption of amygdala-entorhinal-hippocampal network in late-life depression. Hippocampus 2017; 27:464–476Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

25 Kim MJ, Loucks RA, Palmer AL, et al.: The structural and functional connectivity of the amygdala: from normal emotion to pathological anxiety. Behav Brain Res 2011; 223:403–410Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

26 Loonen AJ, Ivanova SA: Circuits regulating pleasure and happiness in major depression. Med Hypotheses 2016; 87:14–21Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

27 Cummings JL, Mega M, Gray K, et al.: The Neuropsychiatric Inventory: comprehensive assessment of psychopathology in dementia. Neurology 1994; 44:2308–2314Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

28 Olin JT, Schneider LS, Katz IR, et al.: Provisional diagnostic criteria for depression of Alzheimer disease. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2002; 10:125–128Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

29 Ismail Z, Smith EE, Geda Y, et al.: Neuropsychiatric symptoms as early manifestations of emergent dementia: provisional diagnostic criteria for mild behavioral impairment. Alzheimers Dement 2016; 12:195–202Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

30 Dubois B, Hampel H, Feldman HH, et al.: Preclinical Alzheimer’s disease: definition, natural history, and diagnostic criteria. Alzheimers Dement 2016; 12:292–323Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

31 Holmes SE, Esterlis I, Mazure CM, et al.: β-Amyloid, APOE and BDNF genotype, and depressive and anxiety symptoms in cognitively normal older women and men. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2016; 24:1191–1195Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

32 Donovan NJ, Okereke OI, Vannini P, et al.: Association of higher cortical amyloid burden with loneliness in cognitively normal older adults. JAMA Psychiatry 2016; 73:1230–1237Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

33 Locke DE, Dueck AC, Stonnington CM, et al.: Depressive symptoms in healthy apolipoprotein E ε4 carriers and noncarriers: a longitudinal study. J Clin Psychiatry 2013; 74:1256–1261Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

34 Blazer D, Hughes DC, George LK: The epidemiology of depression in an elderly community population. Gerontologist 1987; 27:281–287Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

35 Yang Y: Is old age depressing? Growth trajectories and cohort variations in late-life depression. J Health Soc Behav 2007; 48:16–32Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

36 Tampubolon G, Maharani A: When did old age stop being depressing? depression trajectories of older Americans and Britons, 2002–2012. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2017; 25:1187–1195Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

37 Johnson KA, Schultz A, Betensky RA, et al.: Tau positron emission tomographic imaging in aging and early Alzheimer disease. Ann Neurol 2016; 79:110–119Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar