The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×

Abstract

Objective:

The infant temperament behavioral inhibition is a potent risk factor for development of an anxiety disorder. It is difficult to predict risk for behavioral inhibition at birth, however, and the neural underpinnings are poorly understood. The authors hypothesized that neonatal functional connectivity of the ventral attention network is related to behavioral inhibition at age 2 years beyond sociodemographic and familial factors. This hypothesis is supported by the ventral attention network’s role in attention to novelty, a key feature of behavioral inhibition.

Method:

Using a longitudinal design (N=45), the authors measured functional connectivity using MRI in neonates and behavioral inhibition at age 2 using the Infant-Toddler Social and Emotional Assessment. Whole-brain connectivity maps were computed for regions from the ventral attention, default mode, and salience networks. Regression analyses related these maps to behavioral inhibition at age 2, covarying for sex, social risk, and motion during scanning.

Results:

Decreased neonatal functional connectivity of three connections was associated with increased behavioral inhibition at age 2. One connection (between the right ventrolateral prefrontal cortex and the right temporal-parietal junction) included the ventral attention network seed, and two connections (between the medial prefrontal cortex and both the right superior parietal lobule and the left lateral occipital cortex) included the default mode network seed.

Conclusions:

Neonatal functional connectivity of the ventral attention and default mode networks is associated with behavioral inhibition at age 2. These results inform the developmental neurobiology of behavioral inhibition and anxiety disorders and may aid in early risk assessment and intervention.

Anxiety disorders are the most common class of psychiatric disorders and are often associated with significant functional impairment (1). Symptoms of anxiety disorders can begin in infancy or toddlerhood (2), and the median age at onset of anxiety disorders has been reported to range from 6 to 11 years (1, 3). Most adults with anxiety disorders experienced symptoms during childhood (4). The infant temperament behavioral inhibition, characterized by increased attention and distress to novel stimuli (5, 6), is one of the most potent known risk factors for developing an anxiety disorder later in life (7). This continuity between infant temperament, symptoms in early childhood, and disorders in adulthood suggests that behavioral inhibition and anxiety disorders represent an early departure from typical brain development (8). Unfortunately, the timing and nature of this atypical brain development are poorly understood. Moreover, it is difficult to predict which neonates will exhibit high behavioral inhibition during infancy (6), and behaviors in the first year of life are only moderately predictive of behavioral inhibition in boys and not predictive of behavioral inhibition in girls at age 2 (9). Consequently, at-risk children may not be identified until many years after the onset of atypical brain development. The present study addresses these issues by identifying brain network variation at birth that is related to levels of behavioral inhibition at age 2 years.

Indirect evidence supports the hypothesis that the physiology of behavioral inhibition during infancy includes an overactive ventral attention network. The ventral attention network is a cortical functional brain network involved in directing attention to novel, unexpected stimuli (e.g., a stranger or a new toy) and includes portions of the right ventrolateral prefrontal cortex and right temporal-parietal junction (10). Infants with high behavioral inhibition exhibit indirect behavioral and physiological signs of an overactive ventral attention network, including increased attention and increased right lateralized brain activity (as measured by EEG) after presentation of novel stimuli such as toys or a stranger (11, 12). Reports of ventral attention network structural and functional abnormalities in older children and adults with anxiety disorders provide further support for this hypothesis (13, 14).

While the ventral attention network may play a role in behavioral inhibition during infancy, previous work indicates that many other brain regions and systems are associated with behavioral inhibition in older children and adults. Adolescents and adults who had high behavioral inhibition during early childhood, for example, have increased amygdala reactivity to novelty (15, 16) and altered striatal activity in response to reward (17, 18) relative to peers. Similarly, older children and adults who had high behavioral inhibition during early childhood have altered functional connectivity within and between the default mode, salience, and other brain networks (19), as well as altered connectivity between the amygdala and cortical regions that may be within these same networks (20, 21). While amygdala functional connectivity alterations associated with behavioral inhibition are apparent at the time of birth (22), it is unknown whether altered connectivity within and between cortical brain networks is also already evident in the neonatal period. Given the central role of functional brain networks in brain organization and information processing (23), uncovering disruptions that are already present at birth is likely to have fundamental implications for the developmental neurobiology of behavioral inhibition and anxiety disorders.

The goal of this study was to identify variation in cortical functional brain networks evident soon after birth that is related to behavioral inhibition at age 2, in order to elucidate the neural substrate of behavioral inhibition and identify early risk factors. To achieve this goal, we used functional MRI (fMRI) to relate variation in resting-state functional connectivity of brain regions putatively within the ventral attention, default mode, and salience networks in 45 neonates at term-equivalent age to measures of behavioral inhibition at age 2. This prospective, longitudinal approach provides an unprecedented window into the initial stages of the developmental neurobiology of behavioral inhibition and anxiety disorders. By identifying variation in brain network connectivity shortly after birth and before temperamental characteristics of behavioral inhibition emerge, the results from this study most likely reflect variation in fetal brain development. Results cannot be attributed to early childhood experiences that are a consequence of having a behavioral inhibited temperament. Based on the indirect evidence cited above relating behavioral inhibition to the ventral attention network during infancy, our primary hypothesis was that functional connectivity of the ventral attention network at term-equivalent age would be associated with behavioral inhibition at age 2. We also tested whether functional connectivity of the default mode and salience networks was associated with behavioral inhibition, because of the role of these networks in older children and adults with behavioral inhibition. We previously (22) related functional connectivity of the amygdala during the neonatal period to internalizing symptoms (including behavioral inhibition) at age 2 in a subset of this cohort, demonstrating the feasibility of our approach. To ensure that our primary analysis was sensitive to the strongest brain-behavior relations, we performed two follow-up analyses. In a confirmatory analysis, we related functional connectivity among 18 regions (comprising 153 region-region pairs) at term-equivalent age to behavioral inhibition at age 2. These regions are frequently implicated in psychiatric illnesses and are distributed across the brain (13, 14, 19, 20, 2426). In a final exploratory analysis, we examined similar relations among 216 regions, covering most of the cerebral cortex and much of the subcortex and comprising 23,220 region pairs. The primary, confirmatory, and exploratory analyses converged on a consistent set of results, suggesting that our primary, hypothesis-driven analysis detected the strongest relations between neonatal functional connectivity and behavioral inhibition at age 2.

Method

This study was approved by the Washington University Human Studies Committee. Parental written informed consent was obtained for all subjects recruited from Washington University School of Medicine hospitals. A group of preterm infants (N=27; born at gestational age ≤30 weeks) were recruited from St. Louis Children’s Hospital Neonatal Intensive Care Unit, and a group of term infants (N=18; born at gestational age ≥36 weeks) were recruited from the adjoining mother-baby unit at Barnes-Jewish Hospital. Full-term infants underwent MRI scanning within 4 days of birth and were scanned at a mean postmenstrual age of 39.7 weeks SD=1.0). Preterm infants underwent MRI scanning at term-equivalent postmenstrual age 37.5 weeks (SD=1.3). Full inclusion and exclusion criteria, a comparison of included and excluded subjects, and MRI acquisition parameters are provided in the data supplement that accompanies the online edition of this article.

Follow-Up at Age 2

Age 2 assessments included the Infant-Toddler Social and Emotional Assessment (ITSEA), a 166-item parent-report measure with good psychometric properties (27). Based on study hypotheses, this analysis focused on the behavioral inhibition subscale of the ITSEA, which has acceptable internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha=0.77) and is within the internalizing domain. Previous work indicates that ITSEA ratings, including behavioral inhibition, correlate significantly with direct observations of infant behaviors by trained evaluators (27, 28). ITSEA behavioral inhibition scores are also highly correlated with the inhibition to novelty score of the Infant Behavior Questionnaire, another commonly used measure of infant temperament (28). We also examined several factors that could be associated with infant behavioral inhibition, including social disadvantage (29) and a maternal history of affective symptoms (30). Details of these measures are provided in the Supplementary Methods section in the online data supplement. Zero-order relationships among all variables of interest in this study are described in the Supplementary Results section and Table S1 in the data supplement.

MRI Preprocessing

Resting-state fMRI data were preprocessed with in-house software (ftp://imaging.wustl.edu/pub/raichlab/4dfp_tools/); the FMRIB Software Library was also used for field map correction. Magnetization inhomogeneity–related distortions were corrected using a mean field map technique (31). Atlas transformation was computed using infant templates. Volumetric time series in adult Talairach atlas space (3×3×3-mm voxels) were generated, combining field map correction and atlas transformation in a single resampling step. Additional preprocessing included regression of nuisance waveforms derived from rigid body motion correction, CSF and white matter regions, plus whole-brain global signal. The data were low-pass filtered (<0.08 Hz) and spatially smoothed. Frames affected by movement (volume-to-volume head displacement ≥0.25 mm) were excluded from the resting-state fMRI computations (32). A minimum of 5 minutes of fMRI data, excluding censored frames, was required for inclusion in the analysis. Forty-five subjects survived this stringent volume-censoring threshold.

Regions of Interest

Regions of interest were selected from a commonly used set of regions (the “Power 264 set”) in which the functional network identities in adults are well established (23) and which have been used in previous studies of neonates (33). The specific regions are listed in Table S2 in the online data supplement. For the primary, hypothesis-driven analysis, we selected a region in the right ventrolateral prefrontal cortex for the ventral attention network, the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex for the salience network, and the medial prefrontal cortex for the default mode network. We chose these three particular seeds because they have been consistently implicated in studies of behavioral inhibition and anxiety disorders (13, 14, 19, 20, 2426). Mean connectivity maps for these seed regions are presented in Figure S1 in the data supplement and support the functional network assignments. For the confirmatory region-to-region based analyses, we examined relations among 18 regions from the Power 264 set that are distributed across the brain and are representative of five functional brain networks frequently implicated in neuropsychiatric diseases. These 18 regions define 153 region pairs (each of the 18 regions of interest can be correlated with the 17 others, and the total divided by two to avoid double counting). For a final exploratory analysis (see the data supplement), we examined relations among all 214 regions in the Power 264 set that can be accurately mapped to the infant brain, plus individualized left and right amygdala regions that were the focus of a previous study using the present data set (22). These 216 regions define 23,220 region-to-region pairs.

Statistical Analysis

Whole-brain voxel-wise analyses were performed using in-house software (www.nil.wustl.edu/labs/fidl/). All other analyses were conducted with SPSS, version 22 (IBM, Armonk, N.Y.). For the primary, hypothesis-driven analysis, the functional connectivity of each of the three seed regions to every other voxel in the brain was computed in each subject. Correlation coefficients were Fisher z-transformed, generating z(r) correlation maps; average maps are depicted in Figure S1. These voxelwise whole-brain functional connectivity maps were the dependent variables in a series of regression analyses that included behavioral inhibition, sex, social risk (see the data supplement for details on the social risk index), behavioral inhibition-by-sex interaction, behavioral inhibition-by-social risk interaction, and postprocessing residual average framewise displacement. Sex and social risk were included because they were related to behavioral inhibition. The three resulting voxel-wise maps corresponding to behavioral inhibition in the regression model (one for each seed region) were masked to include only voxels in cortical gray matter. Each map was corrected for multiple comparisons using AFNI tools as currently recommended (34, 35). Further details are provided in the Supplementary Methods section in the data supplement.

To test whether our primary seed-based analysis was sensitive to the strongest brain-behavior relationships, we performed a confirmatory region-to-region analysis. This analysis was done to ensure that our selection of three seeds for the primary analysis captured the functional connections at term most strongly associated with behavioral inhibition at age 2. For this confirmatory analysis, we first computed functional connectivity for each of the 153 region pairs for each subject. Next, we performed the identical regression analysis described above. Standardized betas were computed for the association between behavioral inhibition at age 2 and functional connectivity of each region pair at term. Betas that were significant at p<0.05, uncorrected, were examined to determine whether the hypothesis-driven seed-based analysis above captured the most relevant relationships between behavioral inhibition and functional connectivity. To further contextualize the findings, we performed an exploratory analysis examining relationships among 216 regions (comprising 23,220 region pairs); these exploratory results are described in the data supplement.

Results

The demographic characteristics of the sample are summarized in Table 1. Behavioral inhibition at age 2 was significantly negatively associated with social risk score at age 2 (r=−0.34, p=0.02), and girls had nonsignificantly greater behavioral inhibition relative to boys (mean difference, 0.3 points; t=2.0, df=43, p=0.06). History of preterm birth and history of maternal affective disorders were unrelated to behavioral inhibition at age 2. Motion during neonatal scanning after frame censoring was also unrelated to behavioral inhibition at age 2. Based on these results, all imaging analyses below include sex, social risk, the interaction between sex and behavioral inhibition, the interaction between social risk and behavioral inhibition, and postprocessing motion (framewise displacement) as covariates.

TABLE 1. Demographic Characteristics of 45 Children in a Study of Neonatal Functional Connectivity and Behavioral Inhibition at Age 2

Characteristic
N%
Male2146.7
Race
 Black2146.7
 White2044.4
 Asian36.7
 Biracial12.2
Hispanic12.2
Preterm birth2760.0
History of maternal affective disorder2248.9
Social risk scorea
 01942.2
 1511.1
 2715.6
 31022.2
 448.9
MeanSD
Behavioral inhibition subscale of the Infant-Toddler Social and Emotional Assessment0.950.59
Age at 2-year assessment (months)28.43.4

aSee the online data supplement for a description of the social risk index.

TABLE 1. Demographic Characteristics of 45 Children in a Study of Neonatal Functional Connectivity and Behavioral Inhibition at Age 2

Enlarge table

Hypothesis-Driven Seed-Based Functional Connectivity Analyses

As depicted in Figure 1A, functional connectivity between the right ventrolateral prefrontal cortex seed region (located in the ventral attention network in adults) and a cluster near the right temporal-parietal junction was negatively associated with behavioral inhibition at age 2 (peak z-score=−4.3, cluster size=1,323 mm3, with all voxels z<−3.0, centered at 51, −60, 27 in Talairach coordinates). As depicted in Figure 2A, neonatal resting-state functional connectivity between the medial prefrontal cortex (located in the default mode network in adults) and two distinct clusters was negatively associated with behavioral inhibition at age 2. These clusters were located within the right superior parietal lobule (peak z-score=−4.1, cluster size=1,188 mm3, centered at 26, −59, 52) and in the left lateral occipital cortex (peak z-score=−4.0, cluster size=1,053 mm3, centered at −42, −81, −2). The whole-brain analysis with the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex seed (from the salience network in adults) did not detect any relationships surviving correction for multiple comparisons.

FIGURE 1.

FIGURE 1. Hypothesis-Driven and Confirmatory Analyses Indicating That Ventral Attention Network Resting-State Functional Connectivity Evident Soon After Birth Is Related to Behavioral Inhibition at Age 2a

a Panel A depicts a whole-brain regression relating functional connectivity soon after birth between a right ventrolateral prefrontal cortex region of interest and the rest of the brain to behavioral inhibition at age 2. The regression controls for sex, social risk, postprocessing residual motion, the interaction between sex and behavioral inhibition, and the interaction between social risk and behavioral inhibition, and it includes stringent whole-brain correction for multiple comparisons. Panel B depicts the two regions from the confirmatory analysis in which functional connectivity at term-equivalent age was most strongly related to behavioral inhibition at age 2 (as determined by the highest standardized beta weight), controlling for the same factors as in the hypothesis-driven analysis. The two analyses converge on a similar result, namely, that functional connectivity between the right ventrolateral prefrontal cortex and a region near the right temporal-parietal junction and right lateral parietal cortex is related to behavioral inhibition at age 2. Note that this right temporal-parietal junction location overlaps with the seed map of the right ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (see Figure S1A in the online data supplement), suggesting that this location is within the ventral attention network. TPJ=temporal-parietal junction; R LP=right lateral parietal; R VLPFC=right ventrolateral prefrontal cortex.

FIGURE 2.

FIGURE 2. Hypothesis-Driven and Confirmatory Analyses Indicating That Neonatal Resting State Functional Connectivity of a Default Mode Network Seed Is Related to Behavioral Inhibition at Age 2a

a Panel A depicts a whole-brain regression relating functional connectivity soon after birth between a medial prefrontal cortex region of interest and the rest of the brain to behavioral inhibition at age 2, controlling for the same covariates and including the same multiple comparison correction as in Figure 1. Panel B depicts the region pair from the confirmatory analysis that had the second highest effect size relating functional connectivity around the time of birth to behavioral inhibition at age 2, with the same covariates. Both the hypothesis-driven and the confirmatory analysis identified a connection between the medial prefrontal cortex and the right superior parietal lobule that is related to behavioral inhibition at age 2. The hypothesis-driven analysis additionally identified a significant relationship between behavioral inhibition and resting-state connectivity between the medial prefrontal cortex and the left lateral occipital cortex. mPFC=medial prefrontal cortex; R SPL=right superior parietal lobule.

Confirmatory Region-of-Interest Functional Connectivity Analyses

We performed an additional confirmatory analysis to verify that the three seeds we selected for the primary analysis captured the strongest relationships between functional connectivity at term-equivalent age and behavioral inhibition at age 2. For this analysis, the goal was to determine whether we would have obtained stronger results if we had chosen to use other seed regions that are commonly used in studies of neuropsychiatric disorders. In this confirmatory analysis, we examined functional connectivity among 18 regions of interest (yielding 153 region-region pairs) from five functional brain networks commonly implicated in neuropsychiatric illnesses; three of these 18 regions were used as seeds in the hypothesis-driven analysis described above. Figure 3 depicts standardized beta weights for the regression analyses relating each of these region-region pair functional connectivity values during the neonatal period to behavioral inhibition at age 2. Only standardized beta weights in which p<0.05 are presented. Of the 153 region pairs examined, the region pair with largest effect size included the right ventrolateral prefrontal cortex region that was used as a seed for the ventral attention network and a region in the right lateral parietal cortex, near the temporal-parietal junction, that maps to the default mode network in adults (standardized beta=−0.14, indicating that for every one-standard-deviation increase in behavioral inhibition, the connectivity value decreases by 0.14). The region pair with the second largest effect size included the medial prefrontal cortex region that was used as a seed for the default mode network and a region in the right superior parietal lobule that maps to the dorsal attention network (standardized beta=−0.11). Figures 1B and 2B depict these two connections with results from the whole-brain analyses side by side to illustrate that both the primary and confirmatory analyses converge on similar results. In the Supplementary Results section and Figure S2 in the data supplement, results from the whole-brain, hypothesis-driven analyses are further contextualized in an exploratory analysis examining relationships among 216 regions of interest that cover most of the brain (yielding 23,220 region pairs).

FIGURE 3.

FIGURE 3. Confirmatory Analysis Relating Functional Connectivity Among 18 Regions of Interest at Term-Equivalent Age to Behavioral Inhibition at Age 2a

a The color of each square in the 18×18 matrix represents the standardized beta weight of the relationship between functional connectivity and behavioral inhibition, controlling for sex, social risk, residual motion, sex-by-behavioral inhibition interaction, and social risk-by-behavioral inhibition interaction. Only beta weights in which p<0.05, uncorrected, are depicted. White numerals indicate the two connections with the largest effect sizes relating functional connectivity to behavioral inhibition; similar connections were identified in the hypothesis-driven analysis. dACC=dorsal anterior cingulate cortex; DAN=dorsal attention network; DLPFC=dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; DMN=default mode network; FEF=frontal eye field; FPN=fronto-parietal network; IPL=inferior parietal lobule; LP=lateral parietal; mPFC=medial prefrontal cortex; Precun=precuneus; pSTG=posterior superior temporal gyrus; SN=salience network; SPL=superior parietal lobule; VAN=ventral attention network; VLPFC=ventrolateral prefrontal cortex; vSMG=ventral supramarginal gyrus.

Discussion

The goal of this study was to examine associations between functional connectivity of cortical functional brain networks evident soon after birth and behavioral inhibition at age 2. Hypothesis-driven and confirmatory analyses converged on a common set of results: the functional connectivity of a right ventrolateral prefrontal cortex seed (which maps to the ventral attention network in adults) and a medial prefrontal cortex seed (which maps to the default mode network in adults) in the neonatal period was associated with behavioral inhibition at age 2. Notably, neonatal connectivity was related to behavioral inhibition at age 2 beyond information provided by sociodemographic factors, as all analyses controlled for these other factors.

Our results provide the first direct evidence for the previously hypothesized link between the ventral attention network and behavioral inhibition. Previous authors have hypothesized that behavioral inhibition is associated with an overactive ventral attention network because behavioral inhibition includes increased attention to novel stimuli, the putative function of the ventral attention network (13, 36). In addition, infants with behavioral inhibition have increased right lateralized brain activity, as measured by EEG, in response to novel stimuli relative to infants without behavioral inhibition (37); the ventral attention network is a plausible contributor to this increased activity given that it is also right lateralized. Our results also indicate that previously identified functional connectivity alterations associated with behavioral inhibition in the default mode network in childhood and adulthood (1921) are already evident in early infancy. Given the role of the default mode network in self-focused processes, previous authors have suggested that default mode network connectivity alterations in behavioral inhibition may reflect an increased internal focus (19); our results suggest that the precursors of this process are evident at birth.

The continuity of the infant temperament behavioral inhibition, symptoms of anxiety in childhood, and increased risk for anxiety disorders throughout the lifespan suggest that behavioral inhibition and anxiety disorders represent an early departure from typical brain development (38). The present results suggest that this departure from typical brain development may have its origin in fetal brain development, since alterations in functional connectivity are already evident soon after birth. We previously demonstrated, in this same cohort, that functional connectivity of the amygdala during the neonatal period is associated with internalizing symptoms, including behavioral inhibition at age 2 (22). It appears, therefore, that disruptions in the ventral attention network, the default mode network, and amygdala functional connectivity may represent the initial stage in the cascade that leads to behavioral inhibition and potentially to anxiety disorders. Other problems may emerge as functional connectivity continues to develop, and other forces may turn early risk into clinically symptomatic anxiety disorders. Longitudinal studies are required to test this hypothesis.

Identifying the first stages in the pathway to an anxiety disorder provides an opportunity for early identification and intervention of high-risk individuals. Previously identified risk factors are only moderately predictive at birth of behavioral inhibition and anxiety disorders in later childhood (6, 29, 30), so most children with anxiety disorders are not identified until many years after the initial departure from typical brain development. Our results suggest that functional connectivity soon after birth could serve as a biomarker for guiding early interventions such as parent training and early socialization of at-risk infants. Given that the present study suggests that the origins of behavioral inhibition may lie in fetal brain development, functional connectivity shortly after birth could also serve as an outcome measure for perinatal interventions targeting risk for anxiety disorders (39).

This study’s results should be interpreted in light of several limitations. The borders of functional brain networks in infants are the subject of ongoing work, so the functional network identities of regions are not fully defined. We label regions based on adult network identities, but there are almost certainly differences in functional network organization between infants and adults. The results should therefore be interpreted as reflecting properties of regions that become parts of particular functional networks in later childhood and adulthood rather than definitively residing in those networks at birth. Current work generally supports the hypothesis that adult network identities can be applied to data on newborns, although local connections and bilateral connections between homologous regions are much stronger than longer-range anterior-posterior connections (40). Whole-brain fMRI analyses have historically been contaminated by unacceptably high false positive rates; in this study we utilized a conservative correction criterion defined by our own data set, as currently recommended by experts (34, 35). Contamination of functional connectivity by submillimeter frame-to-frame motion is also a major source of artifacts (32); we used strict motion-correction procedures in this study. Finally, behavioral inhibition was assessed by maternal report, and future studies could extend these results by combining maternal report with laboratory observational measures.

In summary, decreased functional connectivity of regions in the ventral attention and default mode networks at term-equivalent age was associated with behavioral inhibition at age 2. Neonatal functional connectivity was related to behavioral inhibition beyond sociodemographic factors, including sex and social risk. These data potentially identify the earliest stage of the neurodevelopmental cascade leading to behavioral inhibition and anxiety disorders. These results have fundamental implications for the neurobiology of anxiety disorders and could aid in early risk stratification and intervention.

From the Departments of Psychiatry, Neurology, Pediatrics, Radiology, Psychology, and Neuroscience, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, Mo.
Address correspondence to Dr. Sylvester ().

The authors report no financial relationships with commercial interests.

Supported by NIH (grants K23MH109983, K23MH105179, R01HD057098, R01HD061619, KL2TR000250, UL1TR000448, K02NS089852, R01NS06424, and R01NS32970), the Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities Research Center at Washington University (grant P30HD062171), the McDonnell Center for Systems Neuroscience, the Taylor Family Institute, the Parker Fund for Young Investigators in Psychiatry, the McDonnell Foundation Collaborative Activity Award, the Child Neurology Foundation, the Cerebral Palsy International Research Foundation, the Dana Foundation, and the Doris Duke Foundation.

References

1 Kessler RC, Berglund P, Demler O, et al.: Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2005; 62:593–602Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

2 Overgaard KR, Aase H, Torgersen S, et al.: Continuity in features of anxiety and attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder in young preschool children. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2014; 23:743–752Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

3 Merikangas KR, He J-P, Burstein M, et al.: Lifetime prevalence of mental disorders in US adolescents: results from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication–Adolescent Supplement (NCS-A). J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2010; 49:980–989Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

4 Mian ND, Wainwright L, Briggs-Gowan MJ, et al.: An ecological risk model for early childhood anxiety: the importance of early child symptoms and temperament. J Abnorm Child Psychol 2011; 39:501–512Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

5 Kagan J, Reznick JS, Snidman N, et al.: Childhood derivatives of inhibition and lack of inhibition to the unfamiliar. Child Dev 1988; 59:1580–1589Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

6 Fox NA, Henderson HA, Marshall PJ, et al.: Behavioral inhibition: linking biology and behavior within a developmental framework. Annu Rev Psychol 2005; 56:235–262Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

7 Clauss JA, Blackford JU: Behavioral inhibition and risk for developing social anxiety disorder: a meta-analytic study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2012 ; 51:1066–1075.e1Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

8 Pine DS: Research review: a neuroscience framework for pediatric anxiety disorders. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2007; 48:631–648Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

9 Fox NA, Snidman N, Haas SA, et al.: The relation between reactivity at 4 months and behavioral inhibition in the second year: replication across three independent samples. Infancy 2015; 20:98–114Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

10 Corbetta M, Patel G, Shulman GL: The reorienting system of the human brain: from environment to theory of mind. Neuron 2008; 58:306–324Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

11 Fox NA, Rubin KH, Calkins SD, et al.: Frontal activation asymmetry and social competence at four years of age. Child Dev 1995; 66:1770–1784Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

12 Fox NA, Henderson HA, Rubin KH, et al.: Continuity and discontinuity of behavioral inhibition and exuberance: psychophysiological and behavioral influences across the first four years of life. Child Dev 2001; 72:1–21Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

13 Sylvester CM, Barch DM, Corbetta M, et al.: Resting state functional connectivity of the ventral attention network in children with a history of depression or anxiety. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2013; 52:1326–1336.e5Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

14 Sylvester CM, Corbetta M, Raichle ME, et al.: Functional network dysfunction in anxiety and anxiety disorders. Trends Neurosci 2012; 35:527–535Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

15 Schwartz CE, Wright CI, Shin LM, et al.: Inhibited and uninhibited infants “grown up”: adult amygdalar response to novelty. Science 2003; 300:1952–1953Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

16 Pérez-Edgar K, Roberson-Nay R, Hardin MG, et al.: Attention alters neural responses to evocative faces in behaviorally inhibited adolescents. Neuroimage 2007; 35:1538–1546Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

17 Guyer AE, Benson B, Choate VR, et al.: Lasting associations between early-childhood temperament and late-adolescent reward-circuitry response to peer feedback. Dev Psychopathol 2014; 26:229–243Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

18 Guyer AE, Nelson EE, Perez-Edgar K, et al.: Striatal functional alteration in adolescents characterized by early childhood behavioral inhibition. J Neurosci 2006; 26:6399–6405Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

19 Taber-Thomas BC, Morales S, Hillary FG, et al.: Altered topography of intrinsic functional connectivity in childhood risk for social anxiety. Depress Anxiety 2016; 33:995–1004CrossrefGoogle Scholar

20 Roy AK, Benson BE, Degnan KA, et al.: Alterations in amygdala functional connectivity reflect early temperament. Biol Psychol 2014; 103:248–254Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

21 Clauss JA, Seay AL, VanDerKlok RM, et al.: Structural and functional bases of inhibited temperament. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2014; 9:2049–2058Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

22 Rogers CE, Sylvester CM, Mintz C, et al.: Neonatal amygdala functional connectivity at rest in healthy and preterm infants and early internalizing symptoms. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2017; 56:157–166Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

23 Power JD, Cohen AL, Nelson SM, et al.: Functional network organization of the human brain. Neuron 2011; 72:665–678Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

24 Sylvester CM, Barch DM, Harms MP, et al.: Early childhood behavioral inhibition predicts cortical thickness in adulthood. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2016; 55:122–129.e1Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

25 McClure EB, Monk CS, Nelson EE, et al.: Abnormal attention modulation of fear circuit function in pediatric generalized anxiety disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2007; 64:97–106Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

26 Monk CS, Nelson EE, McClure EB, et al.: Ventrolateral prefrontal cortex activation and attentional bias in response to angry faces in adolescents with generalized anxiety disorder. Am J Psychiatry 2006; 163:1091–1097LinkGoogle Scholar

27 Carter AS, Briggs-Gowan MJ, Jones SM, et al.: The Infant-Toddler Social and Emotional Assessment (ITSEA): factor structure, reliability, and validity. J Abnorm Child Psychol 2003; 31:495–514Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

28 Carter AS, Little C, Briggs-Gowan MJ, et al.: The Infant-Toddler Social and Emotional Assessment (ITSEA): comparing parent ratings to laboratory observations of task mastery, emotion regulation, coping behaviors, and attachment status. Infant Ment Health J 1999; 20:375–392CrossrefGoogle Scholar

29 Slopen N, Fitzmaurice G, Williams DR, et al.: Poverty, food insecurity, and the behavior for childhood internalizing and externalizing disorders. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2010; 49:444–452MedlineGoogle Scholar

30 Côté SM, Boivin M, Liu X, et al.: Depression and anxiety symptoms: onset, developmental course, and risk factors during early childhood. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2009; 50:1201–1208Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

31 Gholipour A, Kehtarnavaz N, Gopinath K, et al: Average field map image template for Echo-Planar image analysis. Conference Proceedings: Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society, 2008, pp 94–97Google Scholar

32 Power JD, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE: Recent progress and outstanding issues in motion correction in resting state fMRI. Neuroimage 2015; 105:536–551Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

33 Smyser CD, Dosenbach NU, Smyser TA, et al.: Prediction of brain maturity in infants using machine-learning algorithms. Neuroimage 2016; 136:1–9Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

34 Cox RW, Chen G, Glen DR, et al.: fMRI clustering in AFNI: false-positive rates redux. Brain Connect 2017; 7:152–171Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

35 Eklund A, Nichols TE, Knutsson H: Cluster failure: why fMRI inferences for spatial extent have inflated false-positive rates. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2016; 113:7900–7905Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

36 Henderson HA, Pine DS, Fox NA: Behavioral inhibition and developmental risk: a dual-processing perspective. Neuropsychopharmacology 2015; 40:207–224Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

37 Calkins SD, Fox NA, Marshall TR: Behavioral and physiological antecedents of inhibited and uninhibited behavior. Child Dev 1996; 67:523–540Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

38 Pine DS, Fox NA: Childhood antecedents and risk for adult mental disorders. Annu Rev Psychol 2015; 66:459–485Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

39 Hunter SK, Mendoza JH, D’Anna K, et al.: Antidepressants may mitigate the effects of prenatal maternal anxiety on infant auditory sensory gating. Am J Psychiatry 2012; 169:616–624LinkGoogle Scholar

40 Smyser CD, Snyder AZ, Shimony JS, et al.: Resting-state network complexity and magnitude are reduced in prematurely born infants. Cereb Cortex 2016; 26:322–333Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar