The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×
Reviews and OverviewsFull Access

The Phenomenology of Major Depression and the Representativeness and Nature of DSM Criteria

Abstract

How should DSM criteria relate to the disorders they are designed to assess? To address this question empirically, the author examines how well DSM-5 symptomatic criteria for major depression capture the descriptions of clinical depression in the post-Kraepelin Western psychiatric tradition as described in textbooks published between 1900 and 1960. Eighteen symptoms and signs of depression were described, 10 of which are covered by the DSM criteria for major depression or melancholia. For two symptoms (mood and cognitive content), DSM criteria are considerably narrower than those described in the textbooks. Five symptoms and signs (changes in volition/motivation, slowing of speech, anxiety, other physical symptoms, and depersonalization/derealization) are not present in the DSM criteria. Compared with the DSM criteria, these authors gave greater emphasis to cognitive, physical, and psychomotor changes, and less to neurovegetative symptoms. These results suggest that important features of major depression are not captured by DSM criteria. This is unproblematic as long as DSM criteria are understood to index rather than constitute psychiatric disorders. However, since DSM-III, our field has moved toward a reification of DSM that implicitly assumes that psychiatric disorders are actually just the DSM criteria. That is, we have taken an index of something for the thing itself. For example, good diagnostic criteria should be succinct and require minimal inference, but some critical clinical phenomena are subtle, difficult to assess, and experienced in widely varying ways. This conceptual error has contributed to the impoverishment of psychopathology and has affected our research, clinical work, and teaching in some undesirable ways.

The tower of Babel never yielded such confusion of tongues, as the chaos of melancholy doth variety of symptoms.

—Robert Burton, Anatomy of Melancholia, 6th edition, 1651 (1, p. 456)

The introduction of operationalized diagnostic criteria in DSM-III has had many benefits for psychiatric practice and research. However, its wide use has produced an unanticipated side effect—the reification of the DSM criteria (2). In our training, clinical work, and research, we typically evaluate only DSM criteria, as if they constituted all anyone would want to know about the disorder in question (3). Focusing solely on the symptoms and signs in DSM risks producing an impoverished view of psychopathology (4) and has encouraged the rise of diagnostic literalism (5).

In this article, I first evaluate these claims empirically for one of the most common and important of psychiatric disorders: major depression. In this historical inquiry, I ask: How well do the A criteria for major depression used in DSM-III through DSM-5 capture the descriptive approach taken to clinical depression in the post-Kraepelin Western psychiatric tradition circa 1900–1960? (I chose this period because the clinical features associated with what we now call depression were considerably more diverse in earlier epochs [6]). I focus initially on textbooks as the best place to obtain expert opinion about the important symptoms and signs of depression. I next review the phenomenology of depression described by Aubrey Lewis in what is the most detailed survey of the phenomenology of depression written in the 20th century (7). I then evaluate how well our clinical descriptive tradition for depressive illness is represented in the DSM criteria.

Then, in light of this historical analysis, I review the question: How should DSM criteria relate to the disorders they are designed to assess? I suggest that U.S. psychiatry, as a field, has tended to reify DSM criteria. This approach reflects a conceptual error—a category mistake. That mistake is taking an index of a thing for the thing itself. The criteria proposed in DSM-III and subsequent DSM editions are practical means to identify disorders with what are hoped to be good reliability, sensitivity, and specificity. They do not constitute the disorders they seek to identify. This conceptual error is central to the problem of psychopathologic impoverishment. Its correction has important implications for the ways in which the DSM criteria are used in research, clinical care, and especially teaching.

Method

I identified textbooks of psychiatry or psychological medicine published from ∼1900 to 1960, and written or translated into English, from three major sources: Amazon.com, the National Library of Medicine, and Forgotten Books (forgottenbooks.com). Textbooks were rejected if they did not adopt the Kraepelinian diagnostic perspective on affective illness or if the information they contained on the symptoms and signs of major depression was too sparse to be useful. When multiple editions were available, I reviewed the earliest edition. In total, 19 textbooks, published from 1899 to 1956 from five countries (nine from the United States, seven from the United Kingdom, and one each from Germany, France, and Switzerland), met inclusion criteria.

I reviewed the relevant sections of text in historical order starting with the 6th edition of Kraepelin’s Psychiatry textbook (8), creating categories for signs and symptoms as needed as I progressed through the textbooks. After going through all the texts, developing and testing the categories, I went back over them a second time to ensure consistent application of the categories. In the key table, I included short quotations or paraphrases to give a sense of the authors’ “turn of phrase” in their descriptions.

Nine issues arose during this process. First, I focused on the general descriptions of what the author termed either “depression” or (for many of the earlier authors who did not use this category) “melancholia” (terms included depression, melancholia, depressive states, state(s) of depression, depressed type, types of depression, depressive phase, depressive psychosis, and depressed reaction). For the purposes of this essay, I refer to this syndrome as major depression. Second, I never accepted symptoms or signs only described in case reports. Third, if the author described depressive subtypes, I generally took representative symptoms from those of mild to moderate severity. I did not include symptoms or signs only noted for severe or stuporous depression. Fourth, I did not accept symptoms or signs that were described only for “involutional melancholy,” as most authors considered it distinct from “depression” or more typical “melancholia.” Fifth, I did not include signs or symptoms that were present in “mixed states.”

Sixth, I did not describe the delusions or hallucinations associated with major depression, as these are not part of the DSM A criteria. Seventh, none of these authors separately described the symptoms and signs of depression occurring in the course of unipolar versus bipolar illness, so I could not make that distinction. Eighth, many authors described a brief set of symptoms or signs that they judged to be central or primary to the depressive syndrome: I noted these by an asterisk in the table. Ninth, several authors provided rather long lists of associated somatic symptoms. In these situations, I summarized those that seemed most prominent.

Results

Textbooks’ Descriptions of Symptoms and Signs

Mood disturbances in the course of major depression were described by all 19 textbook authors (Table 1). However, a number of them were broader than the “feels sad, empty, hopeless” described in DSM-5 criterion A1. Common additional terms included “gloomy,” “painful,” “misery,” “worried,” “wretched,” “blue”, and “lonesome.” Twelve of the authors commented on loss of interest using language broadly similar to DSM-5 criterion A2, often using terms indicating apathy or indifference.

TABLE 1. Symptoms and Signs of Depression as Described by Authors of 19 Textbooks of Psychiatry Published Circa 1900–1960a

Author, Year (Reference)MoodInterestCognitive ContentCognitive FunctionVolition/MotivationDecision MakingAnhedoniaPsychomotor ChangesEnergySpeechSleepAnxietyAppetiteWeightOther Physical SymptomsSuicidal BehaviorCircadian EffectsDepersonalization/Derealization
Kraepelin, 1899 (8)Gloomy, utterly miserableNothing can arouse interest, indifferentHopeless, future gloomy, without purposeThinking difficult, psychic inhibition*Lack of initiative,* work very difficultVery difficultNothing gives pleasurePsychomotor inhibitionTired, worn outQuiet and hesitantGreatly impairedObsessional ideas commonMuch reducedUsually drops significantlyPalpitations, heaviness; limp facial expression and postureDoes not want to live Deeply perplexed
De Fursac, 1905 (9)Painful,* sad, gloomyIndifferent to everything*PessimisticPsychic inhibition*Abulia*Constant indecisionExperiencing no affectionSlow Indistinct, few wordsDiminished, unrefreshing, disturbedSpellsAnorexiaLoss of weightHeadache, vague pains, constipation  Everything appears strange
Paton, 1905 (10)Emotional depression Self-insufficiency and unworthinessRetardation in thought, difficulty thinkingInability to think or act  Slow, with difficulty Slow or unable to speak Apprehension to marked anxiety      
Dana, 1907 (11)Mental depression*Apathy, loss of interestHopelessSlowness, difficulty in thinking* and attentionWeakness of will, attempts at work are futileHopeless indecision Psychomotor retardation,* but agitation also seen  InsomniaOften anxietyLoss of appetiteEmaciationDyspeptic troubles; precordial distress, constipation; menstrual irregularities; loss of libidoSuicidal ideas and impulses Stupefied by gloom
Craig, 1912 (12)Feeling of depression*Loss of interestSelf-accusationSlowing of thought and action*Inability to work    Slow Vague fears   “Potential suicides”Depression more acute in morning 
White, 1913 (13)Emotional depression* Self-accusatory, hypochondriacalDifficulty thinking*Incapable of effort  Psychomotor retardation* Speaks slowly, monosyllabic    Limp posture and facial expression  Disaggregation of the personality
Cole, 1913 (14)Mental pain, abject misery, wretchednessLack of interest in family or others, apatheticFuture impossible, feeling of impending evil, pervaded by painful emotionsDifficulty in association   Gait is slow but can pace, be restless  Only snatches of sleep, nights are wretched and sleeplessMorbid apprehensionsLoses all appetiteLoses weightDyspepsia, constipation, lost sexual drive, scanty menstruation, dry skin, specific facial expressionsSuicidal intention frequently expressed  
Buckley, 1920 (15)Depression of emotional tone,* sadnessNo interest in anythingHopelessness, personal inadequacyRetardation of thought,* sluggishness, poor attentionReduced; efforts made with difficulty Lost all feeling of affectionRetardation,* all action slowedFatigue, exhaustionCommonly absent Not infrequent state of fear  Weakness  Unlike themselves/surroundings have changed
Jelliffe, 1923 (16)Emotional depression* Self-accusatory, inadequacy, hypochondriacalDifficulty thinking*Incapable of effort, interference when trying to exert will  Psychomotor retardation*Unable to make any effort, lack of energyMonosyllabic, sometimes inaudibleDisturbedAnxiety attacks, apprehension, nameless dreadPoorLoss of weightConstipation, cold extremities, flexion of body   
Bleuler, 1924 (17)Depressive mood,* painful All experiences colored painfully; focus on imagined misfortuneRetardation of mental stream*Reduced resolution of acting*  Retardation of motility,* movements slow and weak, but can be restless, wailing Quiet, slow Often, compulsive fears  Specific facial expression, weakness, headacheSuicidal impulses Loss of emotion, everything strange, colorless; depersonalization
Yellowlees, 1932 (18)Unhappiness, emotional dullnessNo interest in anything, general indifferenceFocus on their moral failings, ideas of unworthiness From lack of initiative to grave impairment in volition Complete emotional unresponsiveness*Lowering of activity,* general slowness, but sometimes pacing restlessly Slow, low-pitched, dullOften insomnia Refusal of food Gastrointestinal disturbanceSuicidal tendency  
Sadler, 1936 (19)Emotional depression,* painful, gloomyLost all interestUnworthiness, guilt, hopeless, self-accusatory, discouragedDifficulty thinking,* slowed downDevoid of ability to initiate action  Psychomotor retardation,* all acting slowed downFatigue, exhaustionLow tone, monosyllabicDisturbed, un- refreshingIrritability  Headaches, indigestion, constipation; careworn expressionDanger of suicideAt worst in morningComplain of unreality of the environment
Noyes, 1936 (20)Despondency, “blue” Hopelessness, unworthiness, gloomyDifficulty thinkingUnable to carry out activitiesIndecision Movements slow, inhibitedFatigueSlow and in low toneInsomnia   Headache, hypochondriacal complaints; typical posture, stooped   
Gordon, 1936 (21)Depression*Lose all usual interestsHopelessness,* unworthiness*Difficulty concentratingEverything a burden Lacks the natural enjoyment of lifeSlowness of thought and action, but sometimes agitation, weeping, hand wringing Delay in responsesInsomniaWorry over trifles  Intestinal upset, constipation, face is very agedCommonOften worst in early morningIn a fog
Muncie, 1939 (22)Depressed, blue, sad, lonesome, worried, homesick* Ideas of self-derogation, unworthiness, guilt, sin*Thinking difficultyInhibition, lack of initiative  General slowing of motility*FatigueSlowedPrincipally early morning awakening* Poor*Weight loss*Menstrual irregularity, reduction in erections, reduced libido; stooped posture, constipation*Suicidal preoccupations*Early morning worst*Depersonalization, puzzle, perplexity*
Henderson, 1944 (23)Depression*Unable to take interestA failure, hopeless, a disgraceDifficulty in thinking* and concentrating  Loss of feelingPsychomotor retardation* but can be agitatedFatigue, exhaustionTypically, reduced speech, but can be increasedFrequently disturbed, always unrefreshingNot infrequently presentReduced Headache, dyspepsia, constipation, careworn expressionDanger of suicideWorst in morningEnvironment seems unreal
Curran, 1945 (24)Sadness,* listlessnessApatheticGuilt, self-reproachRetarded thinking* Indecision Motor retardation*  Insomnia, especially early awakeningFrequent LossLoss of sexual desire,* constipation, amenorrhea, sallow complexion*Suicidal ideas*Worse in morningDepersonalization, feels changed, lifeless, derealization
Mayer-Gross, 1954 (25)Depression, indifference, miseryLoss of interestFeeling of insufficiency, future dark and gloomy, hopelessDiminished quickness of thoughtLoss of willpower, incapable of initiating actionsInability to reach decisionsLoss of enjoymentRetardationFatigue, heaviness of limbsRetardationSleeplessness, unrefreshing sleepExaggerated fears and anxieties, anxiety attacksLoss of appetiteLoss of weightConstipation, reduced sexual desire, amenorrhea; distinct posture, facial expressionCommonWorse in morningDepersonalization; world through a mist
Ulett, 1956 (26)Emotional depression,* melancholy Guilt, self-accusation, self-deprecationSlowness and difficulty in thinking* Painful uncertainty Psychomotor retardation* Slowly and in low toneInsomnia, early rising Failure to eatWeight lossLibido is lost, constipation, menstrual disorder, depressed countenanceSuicidal ruminations  
Number of times noted1912191815771881714141091713712
Number of times prominent (of 15)130212301110010112211

aAn asterisk (*) indicates that the symptom or sign was judged by the author to be prominent. “Depression” is described variously by these authors as depressive states, state(s) of depression, types of depression, depressed type, melancholia, depressive phase, depressive psychosis, and depressed reaction.

TABLE 1. Symptoms and Signs of Depression as Described by Authors of 19 Textbooks of Psychiatry Published Circa 1900–1960a

Enlarge table

All authors described specific cognitive content as characteristic of depressed patients. While guilt and worthlessness were frequently noted, other common descriptors included hopelessness, pessimism, self-accusation, self-derogation, feelings of inadequacy and of being a failure, and a specific focus on prior moral shortcomings, often of a sexual nature. The depressive cognitive content was therefore considerably broader than that incorporated in the relevant DSM criterion A7: “Feelings of worthlessness or excessive or inappropriate guilt.”

All but one author described changes in cognitive functioning, most typically of slowness of thought and difficulty with concentration, consistent with major depression criterion A8, “diminished ability to think or concentrate.”

Depression-related changes in volition and motivation were described by 15 authors and were typically distinguished both from the more physical feelings of fatigue and the difficulty in decision making. Most commonly, this was described as lack of initiative, loss of will, or inability to work. This symptomatic domain is not included in the DSM-5 A criteria for major depression.

Difficulties with decision making were noted by seven authors and are well captured by part of criterion A8, “indecisiveness.” Anhedonia was described by seven authors, most commonly as a loss of feeling of affection for loved ones and natural enjoyment. This symptom is relatively well represented by the relevant part of criterion A2: “markedly diminished pleasure.”

Psychomotor changes were reported by all but one author, 12 of whom described only aspects of psychomotor retardation and six of whom also commented on agitation. These signs are well presented in DSM-5 criterion A5. Fatigue, exhaustion, and feelings of being tired out, symptoms well captured by DSM criterion A6, were described by only eight authors. Speech that was slow, hesitant, indistinct, and/or monosyllabic was described by all but two authors. This symptom is not included in DSM-5.

Disturbed sleep was noted by 14 authors, most typically described as insomnia or disturbed or unrefreshing sleep. Early morning awakening was described by three authors. This symptom is well covered by “insomnia” in criterion A4. However, “hypersomnia,” also noted in this criterion, was not described as a symptom of depression by any of the authors.

Various aspects of anxiety, including panic attacks and obsessive fears, were noted by 14 authors, but anxiety is not included in the DSM criteria for major depression. Poor appetite and weight loss were described by, respectively, 10 and nine textbook authors, consistent with DSM criterion A3. However, none of the authors noted increased appetite and weight gain, which are also described in this DSM criterion.

Other physical symptoms and signs, noted by 17 authors, was the most difficult symptom category to summarize. Most commonly described were gastrointestinal complaints, but diminished libido, amenorrhea, and headaches were also often noted. Also frequently described were changes in facial expression and posture. These phenomena are not captured by any part of the A criterion in DSM-5 major depression.

Suicidal thoughts and ruminations were described by 13 of the authors, and this symptom area is well covered by DSM criterion A9, which includes “recurrent thoughts of death,” “recurrent suicidal ideation,” or suicide attempt or plan. Seven authors described circadian shifts in depressive symptoms, all noting that the symptoms were more severe in the morning. This is not in the DSM A criteria for major depression but is reflected in specifier criterion B2 for “melancholic features.”

Depression-associated depersonalization and derealization were noted in 12 of the textbooks. A number of descriptions were provided, including everything feeling changed or unreal, seeing the world through a mist, and perplexity. No DSM-5 criteria for major depression describe this kind of depressive symptomatology.

Prominent Symptoms and Signs

Fifteen authors noted a subset of symptoms and signs of major depression they regarded as of particular importance or centrality to the syndrome. As seen in Table 1 (bottom row), this analysis indicated that these authors, in aggregate, strongly endorsed three symptoms/signs of depression as being of special diagnostic importance: lowered mood, impairment in cognitive function, and psychomotor changes. All three of these are well reflected in the DSM criteria for major depression. The next most common central symptom, volitional changes, was noted by only 20% of these authors and is not described in DSM-5 criteria. Of note, only one author described neurovegetative changes in sleep, appetite, or weight as a central symptom of depression.

Criteria for Depression Proposed by Muncie

One of the authors—Muncie (22), in a 1939 textbook—proposed a relatively complete set of diagnostic criteria for depression, which for historical interest is presented in Table 2. Compared with the DSM-5 criteria, Muncie’s criteria included symptoms of depersonalization and derealization and gave greater emphasis to physiological symptoms and changes in facial expression and posture.

TABLE 2. Criteria for Depression Proposed by Muncie in 1939a

CriterionNameDescription
1MoodDepressed, melancholy, blue, sad, lonesome, worried, homesick, and noted in the facial expression
1.aMood equivalentDepersonalization, feelings of unrealityb
1.bMood equivalentPuzzle and perplexity
2Cognitive content: appropriate to moodSelf-derogatory, self-depreciatory ideas, ideas of unworthiness, guilt, sin, the source of trouble for others
3Suicidal preoccupationsNatural consequence of mood and content
4General slowing of motilityOf musculature, speech, thinking, including feelings of inhibition, lack of initiative, fatigue
5Diurnal variationEarly morning after waking worst, the evening better
6Physiological alterations
6.aInsomniaPrincipally early morning awakening
6.bPoor appetite
6.cWeight loss
6.dReduction in sexual functionsIn women, menstrual irregularity. In men, reduction in frequency of erections. In both sexes, reduction or loss of sexual desire.
6.eReduction in muscular toneStooped posture
6.fSlowed motilityConstipation, lowered blood pressure, pulse
6.gReduction in basal metabolism

aFrom Muncie’s Psychobiology and Psychiatry (22).

b“In some cases at least, the expression of depersonalization … [may result from] … the fact that the misery is of such a degree as to beggar description in positive terms.”

TABLE 2. Criteria for Depression Proposed by Muncie in 1939a

Enlarge table

Lewis’s Monograph

In 1934 Aubrey Lewis (7) described the “clinical features” of 61 cases of “depressive state,” all examined and treated by Lewis in 1928–1929 in the Maudsley Hospital, London. This 102-page monograph provides considerably greater details of depressive symptoms and signs than any of the textbooks consulted. I can only hope to capture some relevant main themes of this rich paper. First, Lewis comments on all but two of the symptoms and signs that I developed from the textbook review, so there is a high degree of consilience between these two sources of information (Table 3). Second, as with the texts, his descriptions of the mood changes are much richer than those provided by DSM-5. Third, he strongly emphasizes the importance and diversity of changes in cognitive content, demonstrating even more clearly than the textbooks the relative narrowness of the “worthlessness and guilt” description provided in DSM criterion A7. Fourth, he comments on but does not emphasize the depression-related changes in volition. Fifth, he highlights the close relationship between depressive and anxious symptoms, and he specifically notes the relatively high frequency of episode-related obsessions and compulsions. Sixth, he comments prominently on the depression-related signs of change in posture and facial expression. Finally, in accord with the textbook writers, he notes in considerable detail the relatively frequent symptoms of depersonalization and derealization.

TABLE 3. Symptoms and Signs of Depression as Described by Lewis in 1934a

Symptom AreaDescription
MoodDepressed, miserable, dreadful, brokenhearted, worried, frightened, low, unhappy, despondent, awful, in agony, desperate, and reflects “the complex quality of experienced totality”
InterestLittle or no interest in their surroundings
Cognitive contentHopeless, nothing to live for, useless, inadequate, degradation, attribution to self of evil motives, self-reproach and self-accusation. These are “among the most striking of melancholic symptoms”
Cognitive functionDifficulties with concentration and focus. Muddled. But in some patients, due to a constant press of thoughts. “Difficulty in thinking, not slowness of thought, is the essential feature.” Feelings of inadequacy more important than performance.
Volition/motivationNo will power
Decision making
AnhedoniaRarely noted compared with loss of interest. One patient stated, “I can’t enjoy anything.”
Psychomotor changesGeneral slowing of action. But agitation seen in one-quarter of patients. Wringing of hands, picking. Also descriptions of “inner restlessness”
EnergyTired, worn out, exhausted
SpeechAround a third of patients “talked a great deal” and another third said “little”
Sleep
Anxiety“The relation of anxiety to depression is intimate.” Apprehension very common, fear of something unpleasant happening. Seen both “in attacks or as a lasting state.” Thirteen patients demonstrated compulsive symptoms. Obscene thoughts, repeating picking (“I feel I must do that; I know it’s wrong”), washing, “I can’t keep my thoughts off my private parts. It’s dreadful.”
Appetite“Refusal of food is one of the prominent features of any depressive state.”
WeightLoss is common
Other physical symptomsHeadaches, other “aches and pains” common. Drooping slack posture, facial expression “more easily recognized than described”
Suicidal behavior26 patients attempted suicide; 12 others often spoke of suicide
Circadian effectsInfrequently seen in this sample
Depersonalization/derealizationFeel dazed, queer, different, no feeling at all, don’t feel myself; 19 cases with depersonalization—“I have no feeling at all in me,” “I feel all dead”; 21 cases with derealization—“everything changed and unreal,” “a veil or mist interposed between them” and reality

aFrom Lewis’s monograph “Melancholia: A Clinical Survey of Depressive States” (7).

TABLE 3. Symptoms and Signs of Depression as Described by Lewis in 1934a

Enlarge table

Quantitative Analysis

Finally, I took each of the 18 symptoms and signs developed in the textbook review and divided them into three categories: well covered by DSM criteria, partly covered by DSM criteria, and not at all covered by DSM criteria. As seen in Table 4, ten of the 18 symptoms and signs were well described by the DSM A criteria for major depression and one by a criterion for melancholia. Two symptoms and signs (mood changes and changes in cognitive content) were partly covered by DSM criteria. Five symptoms and signs were not reflected in the DSM criteria for major depression.

TABLE 4. The Degree of Coverage by DSM-5 Criteria for Major Depression of the 18 Symptoms and Signs of Depression Assessed by Textbook Authors

Symptom AreaDegree of Coverage by DSM Criteria
MoodPartly covered, criterion A1
InterestWell covered, criterion A2
Cognitive contentPartly covered, criterion A7
Cognitive functionWell covered, criterion A8
Volition/motivationNot covered
Decision makingWell covered, criterion A8
AnhedoniaWell covered, criterion A2
Psychomotor changesWell covered, criterion A5
EnergyWell covered, criterion A6
SpeechNot covered
SleepWell covered, criterion A4
AnxietyNot covered
AppetiteWell covered, criterion A3
WeightWell covered, criterion A3
Other physical symptomsNot covered
Suicidal behaviorWell covered, criterion A9
Circadian effectsWell covered, criterion B2 for melancholia
Depersonalization/derealizationNot covered

TABLE 4. The Degree of Coverage by DSM-5 Criteria for Major Depression of the 18 Symptoms and Signs of Depression Assessed by Textbook Authors

Enlarge table

Discussion

Empirical Conclusions

The goal of the historical portion of this essay was to determine how well the phenomenology of depressive illness described in the post-Kraepelinian Western psychiatric tradition is captured by the current DSM symptomatic criteria for major depression. I begin by reviewing the nine major conclusions of this investigation.

First, the description of the core mood symptom for major depression in DSM-5, “depressed … sad, empty, hopeless,” was narrower than that provided by the textbook authors and by Lewis. Terms like “painful,” “miserable,” “wretched,” “dull,” “broken-hearted,” and “in agony” were used by various authors to reflect the diverse subjective manifestations of the dysphoric mood that is widely agreed to be central to the depressive syndrome.

Second, the descriptive literature placed greater relative emphasis on the cognitive and attitudinal changes associated with depression than do the DSM criteria, where these features constitute only one criterion, characterized solely by worthlessness and guilt. In agreement with Beck (27), the classical authors consistently emphasized the importance and diversity of cognitive changes in depression. In addition to worthlessness and guilt, other cognitive content was emphasized, including hopelessness, gloom, and a range of diverse self-accusatory and self-derogatory themes.

Third, second to mood changes, the most frequently noted key symptom of major depression was changes in cognitive functioning, typically characterized by difficulties in thinking and concentrating. These symptoms, while noted in the clinical and neuropsychological literature (28), have rarely, in the post-DSM-III world, been regarded as central to the clinical presentation of major depression.

Fourth, these authors describe a range of somatic symptoms and signs in depressed patients that receive little attention in DSM. Gastrointestinal changes, especially constipation, was commonly observed, as were menstrual and libidinal changes. Observations of dried skin and characteristic changes in facial expressions and posture were also frequently noted. Somatic symptoms of major depression may be more common in non-Western cultures, especially China, and it has been claimed that this is due to a “mentalization” of depression in Western culture (29). The widespread use of DSM criteria for major depression, which exclude consideration of somatic symptoms, may have contributed to this trend.

Fifth, the textbook authors gave less importance to the neurovegetative features of depression—changes in sleep, appetite, and weight—than the DSM criteria do. No author noted them as key depressive symptoms.

Sixth, consistent with recent work (30), these authors gave greater prominence to changes in psychomotor functioning, especially psychomotor retardation, than is our current practice. This was noted as a key depressive sign by over two-thirds of authors who listed such symptoms. However, only five of the 19 textbook authors described psychomotor agitation as a common symptom of depression or melancholia. In addition, several others referred to sections on “mixed” states where they described such cases.

Seventh, nearly two-thirds of the textbook authors described a domain of symptomatology in depression rarely noted in the post-DSM-III world: derealization and depersonalization. These are poignantly described by Curran:

The typical affect is that of sadness, but has been described as being qualitatively … different to that normally experienced … the typical depressed mood thus shades into depersonalization in which the patient feels changes—strange, lifeless, detached, automatic. Sometimes, instead of feeling that they themselves have changed, the patients lay emphasis on a change in the outer world which seems dead or macabre (24, p. 157).

This point is well captured by a recent first-person report:

It was as if the whatness of each thing … the essence of each thing in the sense of the tableness of the table or the chairness of the chair … was gone. There was a mute and indifferent object in that place. Its availability to human living … in the world was drained out of it. Its identity as a familiar object that we live with each day was gone … the world had lost its welcoming quality (31, pp. 212–213).

Some pathological experiences in depression may not be best understood as “symptoms that a person has” (e.g., sore throat, insomnia) but rather a fundamental change in a person’s “being in the world” (32). That is, hopelessness or guilt can become an “existential state” rather than a “symptom.”

Eighth, nearly all authors commented on changes in the speech of depressed patients, most typically describing it as slow, low in volume, with long pauses. This sign is not in DSM-5.

Finally, the symptomatology of atypical depression—particularly the “reverse” vegetative features of increased eating/weight and hypersomnia—was not described in the classical literature. One of these symptoms (hypersomnia) was introduced into the Feighner criteria (although it was not in the two earlier sets of diagnostic criteria for depression that influenced the Feighner criteria [33, 34]). Increased eating and weight gain were introduced into the Research Diagnostic Criteria (35) for major depression and carried over into all subsequent editions of DSM.

Conceptual Issues With Our Current Approach to DSM Criteria

This historical inquiry provides a useful framework in which to reflect on the way to answer the following question: How should DSM criteria relate to the disorders they are designed to assess? I wish to argue that many in our field have made a serious category mistake in the ways in which we have understood and used the DSM criteria. Put succinctly, we have confused an index of a thing with the thing itself.

Let me explain. As in all diagnostic systems, DSM criteria were designed to index (i.e., measure or assess) syndromes—to describe signs and symptoms that permit the clinician to classify individuals as being affected or unaffected, with good efficiency, reliability, sensitivity, and specificity. To use major depression as an example, if the criteria work well, then individuals who meet the criteria for major depression have a high likelihood of really having depression and being neither psychiatrically well nor having another syndrome, such as panic disorder. But meeting the DSM criteria for major depression is not the same thing as having major depression. The DSM criteria do not constitute clinically significant depression.

To illustrate this critical but abstract concept, let me give three examples. First, early World Health Organization (WHO) criteria for myocardial infarction (MI) were simple (36). Definite MI required these three criteria:

1.

Clinical history of ischemic type chest pain lasting for more than 20 minutes

2.

Changes in serial ECG tracings

3.

Rise and fall of serum cardiac biomarkers such as creatine kinase-MB fraction and troponin

These criteria were well validated, and they predicted definitive diagnostic tests for MI, especially angiography, which directly identifies arterial stenosis or blockage. Thus, we would use these criteria because they index having an MI—they provided a pragmatic and well-validated way to identify this syndrome. However, few would argue that these criteria constitute “having an MI”—that is, that having an MI is simply meeting those three criteria. Rather, an MI occurs when heart muscle dies from ischemia. This is often but not always accurately indexed by these criteria.

Second, the Apgar score (37, 38) was proposed in 1953 by Virginia Apgar as a rapid way to assess immediate postpartum neonatal health. It contains five items (heart rate, respiratory effort, reflex irritability, muscle tone, and color) that can be evaluated quickly in the delivery room (38). The Apgar score is practical, reliable, and valid in that it robustly predicts neonatal survival and risk of future cognitive impairment (38). But would anyone wish to argue that a healthy baby is just having a baby with a high Apgar score, or, to phrase it differently, that the construct of infant health can be reduced to an Apgar score?

Third, the intelligence quotient (IQ) originated with Alfred Binet in 1905. Its major function was to predict school performance, and it is still widely used (for example, as the Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scale) as a broad measure of intellectual functioning. Despite its wide popularity and entry into the general vocabulary, it would be a confusion to assert that being intelligent is just having a high IQ measured by constructs such as fluid reasoning, knowledge, quantitative reasoning, visual-spatial processing, and working memory.

There is, of course, a common core to these three examples. We have a latent concept or process that we cannot easily directly assess: MI, general health of a newborn, and intelligence. We develop a method to assess that construct which is practical to apply and is a good predictor of that latent concept. But then we are at risk of a misstep. As pointed out by John Locke, the great English philosopher (and physician), “Another great abuse of words is taking them for things.… How much names taken for things are apt to mislead the understanding” (39, pp. 442–443). Once we have a name—MI, Apgar, IQ, or DSM major depression—we are at risk of confusing our index with the thing itself, that is, what it was developed to measure.

Following from Locke’s observation, we can reframe our concept of a category mistake into a more concrete and semantic form: the meaning of the sentence “Mary suffers from depression” is reduced to “Mary meets the DSM-V criteria for major depression.” This would be the same error as claiming that “Roger has had a heart attack” is reducible to “Roger meets the WHO criteria for MI” and claiming that “April is a healthy baby” is just the same as saying “April had a high Apgar score.”

Illustrations of the Problems Associated With Our Category Mistake

Why is it a bad idea to conflate our DSM criteria with the disorders themselves? Our historical review of symptoms of major depression presents three illustrative problems. First, for most diagnostic criteria, rapidity and reliability of assessment is critical. The developers of DSM-III explicitly preferred criteria that required low levels of inference, because these are typically more reliable (40). Many of the symptomatic criteria for major depression—such as changes in appetite, weight, psychomotor performance, and sleep—can be quickly and reliably assessed.

But some important features of psychiatric disorders may not be like that. They may be subtle, and time-consuming to evaluate. Does this mean that such symptoms should be disregarded? If DSM criteria for major depression constitute depression, that would be a logical conclusion.

But many of our classical textbook authors believed that derealization was an important clinical feature of major depression. We can understand why this did not make it into DSM: It is a subtle concept, time-consuming to evaluate, and perhaps of limited reliability. Yet, senior clinicians of earlier generations thought it was a critical feature of the depressive syndrome that reflected their patients’ lived experiences. Should we not evaluate it or teach it to our students because it is not in DSM?

Second, developers of diagnostic criteria are also appropriately concerned about specificity. A symptom could be quite clinically important, but if it is shared by many other syndromes, it would likely not make a good diagnostic criterion. Most of our textbook writers considered anxiety to be a prominent and clinically important part of the presentation of major depression. If we conflate our criteria with our disorders, we are then in the awkward position of suggesting that anxiety is not important in major depression because it is not in our criteria.

Third, to be practical, diagnostic criteria need to be succinct. For the key major depression criteria A1 and A7, DSM-5 lists three and two descriptors, respectively. As illustrated by our textbook writers, these lists are too short to capture adequately the range of human experience of the mood state of depression and the range of self-derogatory/pessimistic depressive cognitions.

Consequences of Our Category Mistake

Reification has many meanings, but in this context it reflects the process whereby we have taken DSM criteria as too sacrosanct. Hyman trenchantly writes, “Unfortunately, the disorders within these classifications [DSM and ICD] are not generally treated as heuristic, but to a great degree have become reified” (2, p. 156). Many of us have become diagnostic literalists with respect to DSM, stymied by an excessive respect for own creation. DSM criteria are imperfect approximations created by a very human process that, although scientifically informed, could not be claimed by any knowledgeable individual to be infallible. These criteria have been enshrined in our diagnostic algorithms and structured interviews, and are so often required by journal and grant reviewers that they approach the tokens of an orthodox faith. By losing sight of the indexical function of our diagnostic criteria and confounding the criteria with the disorders themselves, we open the door to reification.

Furthermore, if meeting DSM criteria constitutes a psychiatric disorder, why should we evaluate anything but the DSM criteria? This view is deeply problematic. Psychiatry is the inheritor of the richest tradition of description in all of medicine because the features of the disordered mind/brain system that is the subject of our discipline are so diverse, so innately fascinating and profound in the degree to which they illuminate the human condition. Part of the process of good clinical care is to explore the experiences of our patients. This helps us better understand their experiences, and this sense of shared understanding can be directly therapeutic. This cannot be done without knowledge of the world of psychopathology outside of DSM. DSM might provide a guide to but can hardly be a replacement for our rich psychopathological tradition.

From a historical perspective, psychiatry has been appropriately proud of the “DSM revolution.” However, we still lack gold-standard validators like coronary angiography. The presence of such validators helps illustrate the difference between indexing and constituting a disorder. Given our pride in our diagnosis and our lack of definitive validators, it is understandable that our field has had undergone a “conceptual creep” in which our criteria have mistakenly become our disorder.

Potential Limitations

My historical review of symptoms and signs of major depression should be interpreted in the context of three potentially important methodological limitations. First, the nature of psychiatric practice changed during the 20th century, with outpatient care constituting a larger proportion of our work, especially after 1960. Many of the classical textbook writers were seeing severely depressed patients in hospitals. By the time DSM-III was published in 1980, the majority of depressed patients were being seen in ambulatory settings and were often more mildly ill. Second, none of these authors distinguished between depressions occurring in bipolar illness and those occurring in unipolar illness. It is possible, although unlikely given modest symptomatic differences seen for depression in major depression as compared with bipolar illness (37), that some of the differences between the symptoms described and those listed in the DSM-5 criteria result from admixture of patients with bipolar illness. Third, as noted above, the goals of those who develop diagnostic criteria are not the same as those of the textbook writers, with the former being much more concerned with brevity, reliability (and hence low inferential content), and specificity.

There are two potentially important concerns about my critique of this common approach to DSM criteria. First, readers might conclude that I am critical of the basic concept of operationalized criteria and the ensuing DSM-III revolution. Nothing could be further from the truth. The benefits of these changes to the clinical and research agendas of psychiatry have been immense. It is only against this background that I have attempted to address concerns for the negative consequences of the DSM-III revolution, which in my view arise in part from our misconstruing their true purpose as indices of disorders. Second, the most deflating critique of my view on the category mistake common in our field might be phrased as follows:

All you have done is pointed out the difference between diagnostic criteria and clinical evaluation. Since DSM-III, DSM has contained an “associated features” section that describes common symptoms and signs of the disorder not included as diagnostic criteria. We use diagnostic criteria for referral or treatment decisions but then put them aside and do our complete clinical evaluation.

While this argument has force, it falls short in three major ways. First, it fails to recognize the degree to which many mental health clinicians have conflated clinical evaluation and assessment of DSM criteria in our clinical work, research, and teaching. We often see a clinical evaluation as simply the evaluation of DSM diagnostic criteria. Second, it does not explain the diminution in psychopathological expertise and interest since the advent of DSM-III (4). Third and most importantly, the critique does not address the underlying conceptual problems of diagnostic literalism and reification that arise directly from our category mistake of confusing our fallible diagnostic criteria with the categories they are designed to assess.

Conclusions

The DSM symptomatic criteria for major depression do a reasonable but incomplete job of assessing the prominent clinical symptoms and signs of depressive illness as described in the Western post-Kraepelinian psychiatric tradition. In their use as diagnostic criteria, this is unproblematic because, across all of medicine, diagnostic criteria are designed to index rather than exhaustively describe a clinical syndrome. That is, criteria need only to identify true cases with sufficient sensitivity and specificity, and not to reflect complete catalogs of important symptoms and signs. But it is problematic when we focus our teaching, our clinical work, and our research solely around DSM criteria. In our teaching, our trainees need to understand that the DSM criteria for depression, while a good place to start a diagnostic evaluation, do not represent all the relevant symptoms and signs that merit evaluation. In our clinical work, we should make an effort to explore the diversity of the depressive experiences of our patients, some of which clearly lie beyond the bounds of the DSM criteria. For our research, if we focus only on DSM criteria for major depression, how can we further improve on our current criteria?

I suggest that, to many intents and purposes, we have been misusing the DSM diagnostic criteria because we have confused them with the diagnostic entities they are designed to assess. To be explicit, our DSM criteria for major depression are a good index of the clinical syndrome of depression. But, as my historical survey suggests, this depressive syndrome is not entirely constituted by the DSM criteria. Recognizing and correcting this approach to DSM should help us enjoy the many benefits our increasingly research-based criteria can afford to our field while diminishing its negative effects of reduced interest in our rich descriptive heritage and of excessive diagnostic literalism and reification.

From the Virginia Institute of Psychiatric and Behavioral Genetics and the Departments of Psychiatry and Human and Molecular Genetics, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond.
Address correspondence to Dr. Kendler ().

The author reports no financial relationships with commercial interests.

Denny Borsboom, Ph.D., and Paul Appelbaum, M.D., provided helpful comments on earlier versions of the manuscript.

References

1 Burton R: The Anatomy of Melancholia. London, Bell and Sons, 1893Google Scholar

2 Hyman SE: The diagnosis of mental disorders: the problem of reification. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 2010; 6:155–179Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

3 Kendler KS: DSM issues: incorporation of biological tests, avoidance of reification, and an approach to the “box canyon problem”. Am J Psychiatry 2014; 171:1248–1250LinkGoogle Scholar

4 Andreasen NC: DSM and the death of phenomenology in America: an example of unintended consequences. Schizophr Bull 2007; 33:108–112Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

5 Zachar P: A Metaphysics of Psychopathology. Cambridge, Mass, MIT Press, 2014CrossrefGoogle Scholar

6 Berrios GE: The History of Mental Symptoms: Descriptive Psychopathology Since the Nineteenth Century. New York, Cambridge University Press, 1996CrossrefGoogle Scholar

7 Lewis AJ: Melancholia: a clinical survey of depressive states. J Ment Sci 1934; 80:277–378CrossrefGoogle Scholar

8 Kraepelin E: Psychiatry: A Textbook for Students and Physicians, vol 2, Clinical Psychiatry. Translated by Sabine Ayed, edited by Jacques Quen. Canton, Mass, Science History Publications, 1990 (originally published 1899)Google Scholar

9 De Fursac R: Manual of Psychiatry. London, Forgotten Books, 2013 (originally published 1905)Google Scholar

10 Paton S: Psychiatry: A Text-Book for Students and Physicians. London, Forgotten Books, 2014 (originally published 1905)Google Scholar

11 Dana CL: Text-Book of Nervous Diseases and Psychiatry: For the Use of Students and Practitioners of Medicine, 6th rev and enlarged ed. New York, William Wood, 1904Google Scholar

12 Craig M: Psychological Medicine: A Manual on Mental Diseases for Practitioners and Students. London, Forgotten Books, 2013 (originally published 1912)Google Scholar

13 White WA: Outlines of Psychiatry: Nervous and Mental Disease Monograph Series No 1, 4th ed. New York, Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease Publishing Company, 1913Google Scholar

14 Cole RH: Mental Diseases: A Text-Book of Psychiatry for Medical Students and Practitioners. London, University of London Press, 1913Google Scholar

15 Buckley AC: The Basis of Psychiatry (Psychological Medicine): A Guide to the Study of Mental Disorders, for Students and Practitioners. London, Forgotten Books, 1920 (originally published 1920)Google Scholar

16 Jelliffe SE, White WA: Diseases of the Nervous System: A Text-Book of Neurology and Psychiatry, 4th ed, revised, rewritten, and enlarged ed. Philadelphia and New York, Lea & Febiger, 1923.Google Scholar

17 Bleuler E: Textbook of Psychiatry. New York, Arno Press, 1976Google Scholar

18 Yellowlees H: Clinical Lectures on Psychological Medicine. Baltimore, J & A Churchill, 1932Google Scholar

19 Sadler WS: Theory and Practice of Psychiatry. St Louis, Mosby, 1936CrossrefGoogle Scholar

20 Noyes AP: A Textbook of Psychiatry, 2nd ed. New York, Macmillan, 1936Google Scholar

21 Gordon RG, Harris NG, Rees JR: An Introduction to Psychological Medicine. London, Oxford University Press, Oxford Medical Publications, 1936Google Scholar

22 Muncie W: Psychobiology and Psychiatry: A Textbook of Normal and Abnormal Human Behavior. St Louis, Mosby, 1939Google Scholar

23 Henderson DK, Gillespie RD: A Text-Book of Psychiatry for Students and Practitioners, 6th ed. London, Oxford University Press, Oxford Medical Publications, 1944Google Scholar

24 Curran D, Guttmann E: Psychological Medicine: A Short Introduction to Psychiatry, 2nd ed. Edinburgh, UK, Livingstone, 1945Google Scholar

25 Mayer-Gross W, Slater E, Roth M: Clinical Psychiatry. London, Cassell, 1954Google Scholar

26 Ulett GA, Goodrich DW: A Synopsis of Contemporary Psychiatry. St Louis, Mosby, 1956CrossrefGoogle Scholar

27 Beck AT, Alford BA: Depression: Causes and Treatment, 2nd ed. Philadelphia, University of Pennsylvania Press, 2008Google Scholar

28 Trivedi MH, Greer TL: Cognitive dysfunction in unipolar depression: implications for treatment. J Affect Disord 2014; 152–154:19–27Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

29 Kleinman A, Kleinman J: Somatization: the interconnections in Chinese society among culture, depressive experiences, and the meanings of pain, in Culture and Depression: Studies in the Anthropology and Cross-Cultural Psychiatry of Affect and Disorder. Edited by Kleinman A, Good B. Berkeley, University of California Press, 1985, pp 429–490Google Scholar

30 Parker G, Hadzi-Pavlovic D, Austin MP, et al.: Sub-typing depression, I: is psychomotor disturbance necessary and sufficient to the definition of melancholia? Psychol Med 1995; 25:815–823Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

31 Hornstein G: Agnes’s Jacket: A Psychologist’s Search for the Meanings of Madness. New York, Rodale Books, 2009Google Scholar

32 Ratcliffe M: Experiences of Depression: A Study in Phenomenology. Oxford, UK, Oxford University Press, 2015Google Scholar

33 Cassidy WL, Flanagan NB, Spellman M, et al.: Clinical observations in manic-depressive disease: a quantitative study of one hundred manic-depressive patients and fifty medically sick controls. J Am Med Assoc 1957; 164:1535–1546Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

34 Stone TT, Burris BC: Melancholia; clinical study of 50 selected cases. JAMA 1950; 142:165–168Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

35 Spitzer RL, Endicott J, Robins E: Research Diagnostic Criteria for a Selected Group of Functional Disorders, 2nd ed. New York, New York Psychiatric Institute, 1975Google Scholar

36 Anonymous : Nomenclature and criteria for diagnosis of ischemic heart disease: Report of the Joint International Society and Federation of Cardiology/World Health Organization task force on standardization of clinical nomenclature. Circulation 1979; 59:607–609Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

37 Apgar V: A proposal for a new method of evaluation of the newborn infant. Curr Res Anesth Analg 1953; 32:260–267Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

38 Finster M, Wood M: The Apgar score has survived the test of time. Anesthesiology 2005; 102:855–857Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

39 Locke J: An Essay Concerning Human Understanding. Oxford, UK, Oxford University Press, 1979Google Scholar

40 Decker HS: The Making of DSM-III: A Diagnostic Manual’s Conquest of American Psychiatry. New York, Oxford University Press, 2013Google Scholar