The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×
New ResearchFull Access

Prevalence, Comorbidity, and Correlates of DSM-5 Proposed Disruptive Mood Dysregulation Disorder

Abstract

Objective

No empirical studies on the DSM-5 proposed disruptive mood dysregulation disorder have yet been published. This study estimated prevalence, comorbidity, and correlates of this proposed disorder in the community.

Method

Prevalence rates were estimated using data from three community studies involving 7,881 observations of 3,258 participants from 2 to 17 years old. Disruptive mood dysregulation disorder was diagnosed using structured psychiatric interviews.

Results

Three-month prevalence rates for meeting criteria for disruptive mood dysregulation disorder ranged from 0.8% to 3.3%, with the highest rate in preschoolers. Rates dropped slightly with the strict application of the exclusion criterion, but they were largely unaffected by the application of onset and duration criteria. Disruptive mood dysregulation co-occurred with all common psychiatric disorders. The highest levels of co-occurrence were with depressive disorders (odds ratios between 9.9 and 23.5) and oppositional defiant disorder (odds ratios between 52.9 and 103.0). Disruptive mood dysregulation occurred with another disorder 62%–92% of the time, and it occurred with both an emotional and a behavioral disorder 32%–68% of the time. Affected children displayed elevated rates of social impairments, school suspension, service use, and poverty.

Conclusions

Disruptive mood dysregulation disorder is relatively uncommon after early childhood, frequently co-occurs with other psychiatric disorders, and meets common standards for psychiatric “caseness.” This disorder identifies children with severe levels of both emotional and behavioral dysregulation.

Disruptive mood dysregulation disorder (briefly called temper dysregulation disorder with dysphoria) has been proposed by the DSM-5 work groups for childhood and adolescent disorders and mood disorders to account for children with severe emotional and behavioral problems, of which a prominent feature is nonepisodic (or chronic) irritability (1). Such a phenotype had been conceptualized as pediatric bipolar disorder (1, 2), but evidence from both community and clinical longitudinal studies suggests that such irritability is associated with later unipolar, but not bipolar, mood disorders (35). The work groups adapted the “severe mood dysregulation” category proposed by Leibenluft et al. (6) by opting for a more descriptive name and eliminating hyperarousal as a criterial symptom. Thus, the criteria for the proposed disorder include frequent (three or more times per week) severe temper outbursts combined with persistently negative mood between outbursts. These symptoms must be present for at least 12 months in multiple settings, have an onset before age 10, and the child must be at least 6 years old. The disorder has proven to be one of the more controversial proposals for DSM-5 (710).

Concerns related to this proposed diagnosis fall into two groups: 1) the potential negative consequences of adding a new childhood diagnostic category (e.g., the possibility that it might result in increased medication use in young children or a popular backlash against pathologizing “normal” behavior) and 2) the lack of any empirical basis for the definition (710). The justification for disruptive mood dysregulation disorder itself states, “It can certainly be argued that it is premature to suggest the addition of the disruptive mood dysregulation disorder diagnosis to DSM-5, since the work has been done predominately by one research group in a select research setting, and many questions remain unanswered” (1). But this understates the problem. All research to date has focused on severe mood dysregulation, not the proposed disruptive mood dysregulation disorder criteria. As noted above, the latter omitted the hyperarousal criterion and also differ in terms of criteria related to the onset of symptoms (age 10 years for disruptive mood dysregulation disorder and age 12 for severe mood dysregulation). There are in fact no published empirical studies that have focused on the newly proposed criteria for disruptive mood dysregulation disorder. Our goal in this study was to review the relative utility of the proposed criteria in community samples of children and to determine whether the children who meet these criteria display a pattern of functioning indicative of psychopathology.

Method

Participants

Table 1 summarizes the characteristics of the three community-based samples used in our study.

TABLE 1. Characteristics of Three Community-Based Samples
CharacteristicDuke Preschool Anxiety StudyGreat Smoky Mountains StudyCaring for Children in the Community
Total subjects9181,420920
Total observations9185,3361,627
InformantParent onlyParent and self-reportParent and self-report
Age (years)mean=3.9 (SD=1.3, range=2–6)mean=13.7 (SD=2.0, range=9–17)mean=14.2 (SD=3.4, range=9–17)
% Female51.849.250.0
Race/Ethnicity
 White62.1%89.8%41.0%
 African American37.5%6.4%53.8%
 Native American3.8%
 Other0.5%5.2%
InterviewPreschool Age Psychiatric AssessmentChild and Adolescent Psychiatric AssessmentChild and Adolescent Psychiatric Assessment
TABLE 1. Characteristics of Three Community-Based Samples
Enlarge table

The Duke Preschool Anxiety Study.

This is a cross-sectional study of a representative sample of preschoolers (ages 2–5) attending a large primary care pediatric clinic in central North Carolina. In this study, 3,424 clinic attendees were screened using the parent report of the anxious/depressed subscale of the Child Behavior Checklist. The parents of all children who scored 4 or more on the screening in addition to a 7.3% random sample of the remaining children received a diagnostic interview. Of 1,125 participants selected, the parents of 918 children agreed to complete interviews (81.6%). Of these children, 49% were girls, 42% were African American, and 43% were white. Data from an earlier sample recruited from this clinical setting indicated that the mean and standard deviation of scores on the total symptom scale of the Child Behavior Checklist (for ages 1.5–5 years) were almost identical to those reported in the national norms (11). The majority (58%) were attending the pediatric clinic for well visits.

The Great Smoky Mountains Study.

This is a longitudinal, representative study of children in 11 predominantly rural counties of North Carolina (12). Three cohorts of children ages 9, 11, and 13 years were recruited from a pool of some 20,000 children using a two-stage sampling design. Of the 1,777 children selected to participate in interviews, 1,420 completed interviews (79.9%; see also Costello et al. [12]). American Indians were oversampled to constitute 25% of the sample, and 7% of the participants were African American. Annual assessments were completed on the 1,420 children until age 16, for a total of 6,634 assessments. The youngest cohort was not interviewed at age 13, and only half of the youngest cohort was interviewed at age 14 because of funding limitations.

Caring for Children in the Community.

This is a longitudinal, representative study of 920 children ages 9–17 years from four rural counties in North Carolina (13). A random sample of 17,117 individuals in the public school database generated a screening sample of 4,500 youths. Of these, 3,613 agreed to complete screenings, which were done with the externalizing scale of the Child Behavior Checklist, and 1,302 were selected for interviews. Interviews were completed for 920 participants (70.7%). Fifty-four percent of the participants were African American, and 50% were girls. Two additional assessments were completed at 9-month intervals for participants who had not yet reached age 18.

For all studies, the interviews were completed by a parent figure and by the subject if he or she was at least 9 years old. Before the interviews in each study began, the parent and youth signed informed consent or assent forms that were approved by the Duke University Medical Center Institutional Review Board. All interviewers had at least a bachelor’s level degree, and they received 1 month of training and constant quality control.

Measures

Disruptive mood dysregulation disorder.

None of the studies were designed to assess disruptive mood dysregulation disorder. However, it was possible to define disruptive mood dysregulation because its criteria overlap entirely with those of other common disorders. The psychiatric status of participants in all samples was assessed with the Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Assessment (14, 15) or its down-aged congener, the Preschool Age Psychiatric Assessment (11). A symptom was counted as present if the parent, child, or both endorsed it. To minimize recall bias, the time frame for determining the presence of psychiatric symptoms was the preceding 3 months. However, because onset dates were collected for all items, the duration criterion was determinable.

Definitions for disruptive mood dysregulation disorder criteria were identical across studies (the codebook is available at http://devepi.duhs.duke.edu/codebooks.html). Criterion A was defined using items that assessed temper outbursts and tantrums as part of the oppositional/conduct problems section. If these behaviors were reported, the informant was then queried about the frequencies of these behaviors at home, school, and elsewhere. We could therefore calculate whether these behaviors occurred three or more times per week (criterion B) and whether they occurred across multiple settings (criterion E). The frequency of temper outbursts in different contexts was not assessed for the first wave of the Great Smoky Mountains Study, so this wave was not included in our analyses. Criterion C was assessed using items that addressed the frequency of depressed, sad, irritable, or angry mood or low frustration threshold. These moods had to be displayed on more days than not. Onsets for temper outbursts and negative mood were used to establish a duration of more than 12 months (criterion D) and onset before age 10 (criterion G). Criterion F requires a chronological age of at least 6 years for the diagnosis to be made. Of course, nearly all participants in the preschool sample would be excluded if this criterion were applied to them. In order to compare the potential presentation of disruptive mood dysregulation disorder in young children with that in older children, we ignored criterion F in the preschool sample. Criteria H and I are exclusions based on other psychiatric disorders or conditions. Criterion H refers to a manic episode in the past year. Across all samples, this was vanishingly rare and had no impact on the diagnosis of disruptive mood dysregulation disorder. Criterion I, however, would affect prevalence rates because it involves exclusion for common mood disorders. However, in a study aimed at exploring the utility of the proposed criteria, it would make little sense to exclude one of the most likely “competing” categories a priori. We examined the effects of hierarchical rules empirically.

Psychiatric comorbidities.

We included the diagnostic groups of depressive disorders, anxiety disorders (generalized anxiety disorder, social phobia, separation anxiety disorder, and specific phobia), conduct disorder, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), and oppositional defiant disorder. Children who met criteria for conduct disorder could also meet criteria for oppositional defiant disorder. For specific analyses, disorders were categorized as either emotional, which included anxiety and depressive disorders, or behavioral, which included conduct disorder, oppositional defiant disorder, and ADHD. Two-week test-retest reliabilities of interview-derived diagnoses were comparable to those of other structured interviews, with kappa values ranging from 0.36 to 1.0 (11, 14).

Impairments.

Psychosocial impairment secondary to psychiatric symptoms was assessed in areas of functioning related to life at home, at school, and elsewhere (see Angold et al. [14] for a full description of the concept of impairment implemented in the Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Assessment). For this analysis, impairment variables were constructed to describe social functioning between the subject and his or her parents, sibling, and teachers. In addition, recent suspension from school was noted.

Sociodemographic correlates.

Poverty status was coded using thresholds issued by the U.S. Census Bureau based on income and family size (16). We coded parental school dropout if the participant’s parents had not graduated from high school, and we coded single parent if the parent reported only one parental figure in the house.

Service use.

Service utilization for mental health problems was identified using the Child and Adolescent Services Assessment (17, 18), an interview for the parent and child that provides details of mental health service use during the 3 months preceding the interview. For this study, we categorized services into five domains: specialty mental health, general medical, school, child welfare, and juvenile justice.

Analytic Strategy

Participants from all samples were assigned a weight that was inversely proportional to their probability of selection to account for screen stratification, so that the results from our analyses represent unbiased estimates for the original populations from which the samples were drawn. Sandwich-type variance corrections (19) were applied to adjust for the parameter and variance effects induced by sampling stratification and repeated observations. Odds ratios for comorbidity analyses or associations with other variables were conducted using weighted logistic regression in the SAS procedure GENMOD (SAS Institute, Cary, N.C.).

Results

Prevalence Rates

Three-month prevalence rates are provided for each criterion in Table 2. Estimates for those meeting all inclusion criteria ranged from 0.8% to 3.3%. The rates for the full diagnosis and each criterion were highest in the preschool sample. Temper outbursts and negative mood (criteria A and C) were common across all samples. The application of frequency, duration, and cross-context criteria (criteria B, D, and E) limited the rates of those meeting full criteria. Having an onset before age 10 had little impact on the final prevalence rates in the two older samples. The only sex difference in the rates of individual criteria in any of the three samples was for criterion E (multiple settings) in the Great Smoky Mountains Study (3.6% in boys and 1.9% in girls; p=0.007). No differences were observed in the rates of the full diagnosis by sex in any of the three samples. In the Great Smoky Mountains Study, which included multiple observations across childhood and adolescence, the cumulative prevalence by age 16 was 4.4% (SE=0.9).

TABLE 2. Prevalence Rates of Disruptive Mood Dysregulation Disorder and Individual Criteria in Three Community Samplesa
Duke Preschool Anxiety Study (N=918)
Great Smoky Mountains Study (N=5,336)
Caring for Children in the Community (N=1,627)
CriterionN%N%N%
A. Severe tantrums76980.82,46545.787149.0
B. Frequency18217.75147.11406.3
C. Negative mood26821.179812.81798.2
D. Duration925.92212.8421.5
E. Multiple settings11610.12292.81013.8
Excluding onset criterion991.2330.9
Excluding duration criterion1431.6471.2
Full criteria583.3891.1310.8

a Percentages are weighted and N values are unweighted. The criterion that a child must be at least 6 years old to be diagnosed with disruptive mood dysregulation disorder was not applied. The exclusion criteria were not applied, and rates with application of the exclusion rate are provided in Table 3.

TABLE 2. Prevalence Rates of Disruptive Mood Dysregulation Disorder and Individual Criteria in Three Community Samplesa
Enlarge table

The higher rates of severe tantrums and negative mood in the younger sample are consistent with findings from developmental psychopathology (e.g., Tremblay et al. [20]). If the frequency thresholds for tantrums and negative mood were tightened to require that preschoolers display these problems every day, then 10.3% (SE=1.8) of children would meet criterion B, 16.9% (SE=2.1) would meet criterion C, and 1.7% (SE=2.1) would meet full diagnostic criteria. This rate is still higher than in the older samples, but these thresholds also indicate a high level of emotional and behavioral dysregulation.

Comorbidity

One of the primary questions for this proposed disorder has been the degree to which it overlaps with other psychiatric disorders. Table 3 lists the rates of co-occurrence between disruptive mood dysregulation disorder and other common psychiatric disorders. Disruptive mood dysregulation significantly co-occurred with all common psychiatric disorders with the exception of anxiety disorders and ADHD in one data set, although there was evidence of overlap even in these cases. The highest levels of co-occurrence were with depressive disorders (odds ratios between 9.9 and 23.5) and oppositional defiant disorder (odds ratios between 52.9 and 103.0). Rates of disruptive mood dysregulation disorder in those without a depressive disorder provide the prevalence rate if the exclusion criterion were strictly applied. Application of this criterion reduces the rates to 2.9% (SE=0.8) in the Duke Preschool Anxiety sample, 0.8% (SE=0.2) in the Great Smoky Mountains sample, and 0.8% (SE=0.2) in the Caring for Children in the Community sample.

TABLE 3. Comorbidity Rates Between Disruptive Mood Dysregulation Disorder and Other Common Psychiatric Disordersa
Rate of DMDD (%)
Rate of Diagnosis (%)
ComorbidityDiagnosisNo DiagnosisDMDDNo DMDDOdds Ratio95% CIp
Duke Preschool Anxiety Study
 Depressive23.02.912.41.49.94.1–23.7<0.0001
 Anxiety7.51.372.930.76.11.9–19.00.002
 ODD37.71.167.73.852.917.1–163.8<0.0001
 Conduct disorder9.72.822.17.03.81.5–9.30.004
 ADHD23.52.430.83.412.64.0–39.6<0.0001
Great Smoky Mountains Study
 Depressive15.60.832.72.123.59.9–56.1<0.0001
 Anxiety5.51.09.31.85.21.2–22.90.03
 ODD23.30.557.42.261.027.7–134.4<0.0001
 Conduct disorder10.90.923.12.111.93.4–41.0<0.0001
 ADHD9.21.16.30.77.62.9–19.7<0.0001
Caring for Children in the Community
 Depressive11.80.835.83.216.36.3–42.1<0.0001
 Anxiety2.51.17.73.62.20.6–8.20.25
 ODD27.00.470.62.3103.040.1–264.2<0.0001
 Conduct disorder5.31.018.84.04.41.2–15.30.02
 ADHD4.31.19.41.82.90.4–23.30.32

a DMDD=disruptive mood dysregulation disorder; ODD=oppositional defiant disorder; ADHD=attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder.

TABLE 3. Comorbidity Rates Between Disruptive Mood Dysregulation Disorder and Other Common Psychiatric Disordersa
Enlarge table

Seven manic episodes were reported across 6,963 observations in the older samples. Only one of these cases overlapped with disruptive mood dysregulation disorder. Levels of comorbidity were similar when applying the exclusion criterion for those with mania. We also looked at the overlap with Leibenluft’s severe mood dysregulation, which was evaluated in the Great Smoky Mountains Study (5). The levels of co-occurrence were high (odds ratio=44.5, 95% CI=18.5–107.1; p<0.001), with 38.9% of severe mood dysregulation cases meeting criteria for disruptive mood dysregulation disorder.

Figure 1 depicts how often disruptive mood dysregulation disorder overlapped with emotional disorders (anxiety or depressive disorders), behavioral disorders (conduct disorder, oppositional defiant disorder, or ADHD), or both. Although there were variations across samples, the most common presentation in each sample was comorbidity with another disorder. The likelihood of disruptive mood dysregulation disorder alone ranged from 8% in the Duke Preschool Anxiety sample to 38% in the Great Smoky Mountains sample. (For comparison, conduct disorder occurred alone 64% of the time and depression occurred alone 36.6% of the time in the Great Smoky Mountains study.) The likelihood of disruptive mood dysregulation disorder occurring with both an emotional and a behavioral disorder ranged from 68% in the Duke Preschool Anxiety sample to 32% in the Great Smoky Mountains sample.

FIGURE 1. Co-Occurrence Rates of Disruptive Mood Dysregulation Disorder in Three Community Samplesa

a These charts indicate the rates of individuals with disruptive mood dysregulation disorder (DMDD) only, disruptive mood dysregulation plus emotional disorders (DMDD+INT), disruptive mood dysregulation plus behavioral disorders (DMDD+EXT), or disruptive mood dysregulation plus both emotional and behavioral disorders (DMDD+Both) in the Duke Preschool Anxiety Study, the Great Smoky Mountains Study, and the Caring for Children in the Community study.

Impairment, Service Use, and Sociodemographic Correlates

Table 4 summarizes the relationships between disruptive mood dysregulation disorder and impairments, service use, and sociodemographic correlates in the studies of older children. Youths with disruptive mood dysregulation disorder experienced higher levels of all social impairments and also had elevated rates of recent suspension. Rates of service use were universally elevated in affected subjects compared with those without a diagnosis. These individuals were more likely to come from impoverished families, although this was not necessarily accounted for by family structure or parental educational level.

TABLE 4. Associations Between Disruptive Mood Dysregulation Disorder and Impairments, Service Use, and Sociodemographic Variablesa
Great Smoky Mountains Study
Caring for Children in the Community
CriterionDMDD (%)No DMDD (%)Odds Ratio95% CIpDMDD (%)No DMDD (%)Odds Ratio95% CIp
Impairment
 Parental relations57.37.017.28.2–36.1<0.000164.411.112.35.2–29.2<0.0001
 Sibling relations25.43.87.72.6–22.70.000266.26.824.310.7–55.3<0.0001
 Teacher relations16.52.08.52.7–26.80.000355.14.520.910.2–42.7<0.0001
 School suspension35.14.211.44.4–30.0<0.000138.38.66.02.8–12.9<0.0001
Service use
 Mental health31.56.06.52.6–16.3<0.000139.84.713.84.7–40.5<0.0001
 General medical8.03.32.61.3–5.10.0060.02.4
 School system26.07.54.11.7–9.70.00237.58.76.82.2–20.50.0008
 Child welfare15.41.313.13.6–47.90.00015.80.610.21.4–75.30.02
 Juvenile justice8.81.84.10.6–27.70.1528.41.016.14.6–56.5<0.0001
 Any54.218.04.52.1–9.70.00160.814.09.63.2–28.2<0.0001
Sociodemographic
 Impoverished42.919.03.01.3–6.90.0166.231.94.11.6–10.40.003
 Single-parent family40.023.72.21.0–4.90.0526.114.72.00.8–48.00.12
 Low parent education19.417.31.30.5–3.20.6161.842.72.10.9–4.90.09

a DMDD=disruptive mood dysregulation disorder.

TABLE 4. Associations Between Disruptive Mood Dysregulation Disorder and Impairments, Service Use, and Sociodemographic Variablesa
Enlarge table

Given the high degree of overlap between disruptive mood dysregulation and other disorders, it is reasonable to suggest that these associations might be accounted for by such comorbidity. Analyses from Table 3 were rerun in the Great Smoky Mountains Study, excluding those who met criteria for any other DSM disorders. All significant findings continued to be significant except those for impairment in relations with teacher and any service use.

Discussion

To our knowledge, this is the first study to apply the proposed criteria for disruptive mood dysregulation disorder in community samples. The studies evaluated children and adolescents from 2 to 18 years old in rural and urban communities and included large groups of European Americans, African Americans, and American Indians. Disruptive mood dysregulation occurred at relatively low rates in the community, and it most often occurred in combination with other psychiatric disorders. This propensity toward comorbidity extended to all common psychiatric disorders but was strongest for oppositional defiant disorder and depressive disorders. Overall, disruptive mood dysregulation met the standards of psychiatric caseness tested: it was comorbid with psychiatric disorders and was associated with high levels of social impairment, school suspension, all types of service use, and family poverty. This does not clarify its distinctiveness from existing disorders, however.

A few limitations of this study should be kept in mind. First, the investigation of disruptive mood dysregulation disorder in the three studies relied exclusively on psychiatric interviews designed to assess other disorders. Test-retest reliability data are not available for disruptive mood dysregulation disorder. None of the samples were collected to approximate a nationally representative sample of children, although all studies employed sampling and weighting strategies to minimize selection bias. Furthermore, results from these representative community samples will likely differ from clinical samples of disruptive mood dysregulation disorder. All studies focused on a 3-month primary period to minimize recall bias and forgetting.

In these samples, disruptive mood dysregulation was relatively uncommon in childhood and adolescence. By comparison, 3-month rates were between 2% and 3% for depressive disorders and between 2% and 5% for conduct disorder in the older samples (12, 13). These rates are consistent with the proposed justification for disruptive mood dysregulation as a “severe mood disorder” (1). It was not the case, however, that the primary symptoms were uncommon. It was only when frequency, duration, and cross-context criteria were applied that a relatively uncommon phenotype was identified.

The prevalence of disruptive mood dysregulation disorder in the preschool sample was 2–3 times that observed in the older samples. This is not surprising given the literature identifying early childhood as a peak period for temper tantrums and irritability (e.g., see Table 2 in Egger and Angold [21]). Developmental differences are often used to justify amended criteria, and we present one example of alternative frequency thresholds for criteria B and C that could attenuate prevalence differences across development.

In the two older samples, application of the onset criterion had only a minimal effect on the prevalence rates. Furthermore, no justification was provided by the DSM-5 work groups for why this disorder cannot be diagnosed before age 6 (1). This age criterion precludes the diagnosis for preschoolers, yet our results suggest that disruptive mood dysregulation disorder can be diagnosed in this population and that its comorbidity patterns were generally similar to those observed in older children. The DSM-5 Task Force states that “only by having consistent (reliable) diagnoses can researchers compare different treatments for similar patients, determine the risk factors and causes for specific disorders, and determine their incidence and prevalence rates” (22). If this is so, then it might be preferable to eliminate this criterion to facilitate the study of severe irritability across development.

A primary concern has been whether the proposed criteria identify a distinct diagnostic entity (8). Comorbidity is common in psychiatry. In a meta-analysis of childhood comorbidity patterns, median odds ratios for comorbidity ranged from 3.0 for ADHD with anxiety to 10.7 for ADHD with conduct disorder (23), and comorbidity rates from individual studies were often much higher. Yet these high levels of pairwise associations are not typically considered a threat to the validity of the diagnostic system. The observed comorbidity rates in the present study were generally within the range observed for other disorder pairs with the exception of oppositional defiant disorder (odds ratios ranged from 52.9 to 103.0). It was also the case that disruptive mood dysregulation disorder did sometimes occur alone, particularly in the older samples.

The high levels of co-occurrence with oppositional defiant disorder, however, require further attention and belie proposed attempts to categorize disruptive mood dysregulation as a mood disorder only. We believe that a provisional effort should be made to clarify the nature of this overlap before the publication of DSM-5. At the same time, cross-sectional comorbidity is only one consideration, and evidence from longitudinal studies (including the Great Smoky Mountains Study) has linked severe mood dysregulation with later mood disorders (35). This is exactly the same pattern that has been found for the putative behavioral disorder of oppositional defiant disorder (4, 24, 25), which so commonly co-occurs with disruptive mood dysregulation. Both oppositional defiant disorder and disruptive mood dysregulation disorder should be considered to have mixed behavioral and emotional features.

This diagnosis does not identify an area of unmet need in the traditional sense of marking children with lower levels of service utilization. For childhood disorders, most studies have found that between 35% and 45% of those with a disorder have received treatment (13, 2628). With disruptive mood dysregulation disorder, 3-month service use rates were between 45% and 61%. This does not mean, of course, that the treatment such children receive is either appropriate or even helpful. Concerns about inappropriate or untested interventions are no less a concern than the absence of treatment altogether.

Conclusions

This early look at disruptive mood dysregulation disorder suggests that it meets common standards for psychiatric “caseness” and that it identifies a group of children with severe emotional and behavioral dysregulation. Its relatively low prevalence and high levels of service utilization moderate worries about “pathologizing” normal behavior, although the core symptoms are common and its rarity comes from strict application of frequency, duration, and cross-context criteria. It is unclear which aspects of the pathophysiology are unique to disruptive mood dysregulation and which are shared with the individual emotional and behavioral disorders with which it so commonly occurs. This should be a priority area of research (see Stringaris et al. [29] for one example) so that these issues can be resolved well before the advent of DSM-6.

From the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Duke University Medical Center, Durham, N.C.
Address correspondence to Dr. Copeland ().

The authors report no financial relationships with commercial interests.

Supported by NIMH grants MH-080230, MH-63970, MH-63671, MH-48085, MH-075766; National Institute on Drug Abuse grants DA/MH-11301, DA-011301, DA-016977, DA-011301; a NARSAD Early Career Award to Dr. Copeland; and the William T. Grant Foundation.

References

1 DSM-5 Childhood and Adolescent Disorders Work Group: Justification for temper dysregulation disorder with dysphoria. Arlington, VA, American Psychiatric Association, 2010 (www.dsm5.org/Proposed Revision Attachments/Justification for Temper Dysregulation Disorder with Dysphoria.pdf)Google Scholar

2 Pogge DL, Wayland-Smith D, Zaccario M, Borgaro S, Stokes J, Harvey PD: Diagnosis of manic episodes in adolescent inpatients: structured diagnostic procedures compared to clinical chart diagnoses. Psychiatry Res 2001; 101:47–54Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

3 Leibenluft E, Cohen P, Gorrindo T, Brook JS, Pine DS: Chronic versus episodic irritability in youth: a community-based, longitudinal study of clinical and diagnostic associations. J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol 2006; 16:456–466Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

4 Stringaris A, Cohen P, Pine DS, Leibenluft E: Adult outcomes of youth irritability: a 20-year prospective community-based study. Am J Psychiatry 2009; 166:1048–1054LinkGoogle Scholar

5 Brotman MA, Schmajuk M, Rich BA, Dickstein DP, Guyer AE, Costello EJ, Egger HL, Angold A, Pine DS, Leibenluft E: Prevalence, clinical correlates, and longitudinal course of severe mood dysregulation in children. Biol Psychiatry 2006; 60:991–997Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

6 Leibenluft E, Charney DS, Towbin KE, Bhangoo RK, Pine DS: Defining clinical phenotypes of juvenile mania. Am J Psychiatry 2003; 160:430–437LinkGoogle Scholar

7 Parens E, Johnston J, Carlson GA: Pediatric mental health care dysfunction disorder? N Engl J Med 2010; 362:1853–1855Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

8 Axelson DA, Birmaher B, Findling RL, Fristad MA, Kowatch RA, Youngstrom EA, Arnold EL, Goldstein BI, Goldstein TR, Chang KD, Delbello MP, Ryan ND, Diler RS: Concerns regarding the inclusion of temper dysregulation disorder with dysphoria in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition. J Clin Psychiatry 2011; 72:1257–1262Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

9 Stringaris A: Irritability in children and adolescents: a challenge for DSM-5. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2011; 20:61–66Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

10 Taylor E: Diagnostic classification: current dilemmas and possible solutions, in Child Psychology and Psychiatry: Frameworks for Practice, 2nd ed. Edited by Skuse DBruce HDowdney LMrazek D. Chichester, UK, John Wiley & Sons, 2011, pp 223–228CrossrefGoogle Scholar

11 Egger HL, Erkanli A, Keeler G, Potts E, Walter BK, Angold A: Test-retest reliability of the Preschool Age Psychiatric Assessment (PAPA). J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2006; 45:538–549Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

12 Costello EJ, Mustillo S, Erkanli A, Keeler G, Angold A: Prevalence and development of psychiatric disorders in childhood and adolescence. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2003; 60:837–844Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

13 Angold A, Erkanli A, Farmer EM, Fairbank JA, Burns BJ, Keeler G, Costello EJ: Psychiatric disorder, impairment, and service use in rural African American and white youth. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2002; 59:893–901Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

14 Angold A, Prendergast M, Cox A, Harrington R, Simonoff E, Rutter M: The Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Assessment (CAPA). Psychol Med 1995; 25:739–753Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

15 Angold A, Costello EJ: The Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Assessment (CAPA). J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2000; 39:39–48Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

16 Dalaker J, Naifah M: Poverty in the United States: 1997, in US Bureau of the Census, Current Population Reports: Consumer Income, pp 60–201. www2.census.gov/prod2/popscan/p60-201.pdfGoogle Scholar

17 Ascher BH, Farmer EMZ, Burns BJ, Angold A: The Child and Adolescent Services Assessment (CASA): description and psychometrics. J Emot Behav Disord 1996; 4:12–20CrossrefGoogle Scholar

18 Farmer EMZ, Angold A, Burns BJ, Costello EJ: Reliability of self-reported service use: test-retest consistency of children’s responses to the Child and Adolescent Services Assessment (CASA). J Child Fam Stud 1994; 3:307–325CrossrefGoogle Scholar

19 Pickles A, Dunn G, Vázquez-Barquero JL: Screening for stratification in two-phase (“two-stage”) epidemiological surveys. Stat Methods Med Res 1995; 4:73–89Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

20 Tremblay RE, LeMarquand D, Vitaro F: The prevention of oppositional defiant disorder and conduct disorder, in Handbook of Disruptive Behavior Disorders. Edited by Quay HCHogan AE. New York, Plenum Publishers, 1999, pp 525–555CrossrefGoogle Scholar

21 Egger HL, Angold A: Common emotional and behavioral disorders in preschool children: presentation, nosology, and epidemiology. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2006; 47:313–337Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

22 DSM-5 Task Force: Frequently asked questions. Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Association, 2010 (http://www.dsm5.org/about/pages/faq.aspx)Google Scholar

23 Angold A, Costello EJ, Erkanli A: Comorbidity. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1999; 40:57–87Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

24 Burke JD, Hipwell AE, Loeber R: Dimensions of oppositional defiant disorder as predictors of depression and conduct disorder in preadolescent girls. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2010; 49:484–492Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

25 Copeland WE, Shanahan L, Costello EJ, Angold A: Childhood and adolescent psychiatric disorders as predictors of young adult disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2009; 66:764–772Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

26 Farmer EM, Burns BJ, Phillips SD, Angold A, Costello EJ: Pathways into and through mental health services for children and adolescents. Psychiatr Serv 2003; 54:60–66LinkGoogle Scholar

27 Costello EJ, Copeland W, Cowell A, Keeler G: Service costs of caring for adolescents with mental illness in a rural community, 1993-2000. Am J Psychiatry 2007; 164:36–42LinkGoogle Scholar

28 Leaf PJ, Alegria M, Cohen P, Goodman SH, Horwitz SM, Hoven CW, Narrow WE, Vaden-Kiernan M, Regier DA: Mental health service use in the community and schools: results from the four-community MECA Study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1996; 35:889–897Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

29 Stringaris A, Zavos H, Leibenluft E, Maughan B, Eley TC: Adolescent irritability: phenotypic associations and genetic links with depressed mood. Am J Psychiatry 2012; 169:47–54LinkGoogle Scholar