The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×
Brief ReportFull Access

Enhanced Early Morning Salivary Cortisol in Neuroticism

Abstract

OBJECTIVE: Neuroticism is a predisposing factor for major depression. The increase in salivary cortisol that follows waking provides a reliable measure of adrenocortical activity, and this response is increased in recovered depressed patients. This study compared waking cortisol levels in healthy subjects with high and low levels of neuroticism without a previous history of depression. METHOD: Salivary cortisol levels were measured upon waking and at 15-minute intervals for the next hour in volunteers selected to have high (>19/23) or low (<4/23) neuroticism based on the Eysenck Personality Inventory. RESULTS: Subjects with high neuroticism showed significantly greater levels of salivary cortisol 30 minutes after waking, which were maintained for the next half hour. CONCLUSIONS: Abnormalities in waking cortisol are associated with neuroticism in a way similar to those seen in major depression. Elevated waking cortisol may represent a vulnerability marker for mood disorder.

Neuroticism is a dimensional measure of an individual’s tendency to experience negative emotions that are manifested at one extreme as anxiety, depression, and moodiness and at the other as emotional stability. The heritability of neuroticism is well established (1), and some studies have suggested that there is a substantial overlap between the genetic risk factors for neuroticism and major depression (2). However, it is not known which biological correlates of neuroticism might give rise to the increased risk of clinical depression.

Major depression is often accompanied by hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis dysfunction (3). Studies have suggested that the increase in salivary cortisol that follows waking provides a reliable dynamic measure of adrenocortical activity (4) and that this response is increased in euthymic unmedicated patients with a history of depression (5). However, it is unknown whether elevated waking cortisol is a risk factor for depression or a result of having been depressed or treated for depression in the past. The aim of this study was to examine waking cortisol in people with high or low levels of neuroticism without a history of mood disorder to test the hypothesis that high neuroticism itself is associated with altered adrenocortical regulation.

Method

Thirty healthy volunteers (ages 21–57 years) were selected by their neuroticism score on the Eysenck Personality Inventory. Individuals were chosen from a cohort of 20,427 families collected as part of an investigation into the genetic basis of personality (6). We contacted unrelated individuals from selected extremes: 15 subjects (seven men, age: mean=39.5, SD=14.6) with extremely high scores for neuroticism (mean=21.3, SD=1.3, range=19–23) and 15 subjects (seven men, age: mean=46.9, SD=9.0) with extremely low scores (mean=0.53, SD=1.13) were included in the analysis.

On the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV, subjects were determined to be free of past or current history of axis I disorder. They had no current physical illness and had been free of medication for at least 1 month. The group with high neuroticism scores had a mean score on the Beck Depression Inventory of 9.14 (SD=4.73), and the group with low neuroticism scores had a mean score of 3.07 (SD=2.34). The study was approved by the local ethics committee. All subjects gave written informed consent.

Fasting saliva samples were collected in salivette tubes with the first sample taken immediately upon waking and continuing at 15-minute intervals for the next hour (7). After this, the subjects resumed normal activity. Subsequently, saliva samples were taken at noon, 6:00 p.m., and 10:00 p.m., with subjects avoiding food for 1 hour previously. The premenstrual week was avoided for women. Salivary cortisol was measured with an in-house double-antibody radioimmunoassay.

Salivary levels were analyzed with a two-way repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA), with group and gender asbetween-subject factors and time (sampling time) as the main within-subject factor and Huynh-Feldt correction (uncorrected dfs reported). Significant interactions were analyzed with post hoc t tests. The area under the curve for the first 60 minutes after waking was measured with the trapezoid method, with subtraction of baseline cortisol secretion.

Results

There was no difference in age between the groups (t=–1.67, df=28, p=0.10). The mean time of awakening did not differ between subjects with high neuroticism scores (7:03 a.m. ±41 minutes) and the subjects with low neuroticism scores (6:43 a.m. ±49 minutes) (t=1.15, df=28, p=0.26).

The ANOVA showed a significant group-by-time interaction (F=4.18, df=4, 104, p=0.005). Post hoc testing (Figure 1) showed a greater increase in salivary cortisol 30 minutes after waking in the group with high neuroticism scores, and this difference from subjects with low neuroticism scores was maintained for the next 30 minutes. After that, levels of cortisol were similar for both groups. There was no effect of gender (F=2.47, df=1, 26, p=0.13) or gender by time (F=0.88, df=4, 104, p=0.48).

The area under the curve for cortisol secretion was substantially greater in the group with high neuroticism scores (mean=14.92 nmolhour/liter, SD=11.38, versus mean=5.79 nmolhour/liter, SD=9.46) (t=2.39, df=28, p=0.02). In the subjects with high neuroticism scores, there was a correlation between the area under the curve and neuroticism score (r=0.53, p=0.04); however, Beck Depression Inventory score did not correlate with cortisol area under the curve in the volunteers with high neuroticism scores (r=0.26, p=0.37).

Discussion

The subjects with high neuroticism scores showed significantly greater levels of salivary cortisol between 30 and 60 minutes after waking. It is well established that waking in the morning is followed by an adrenocortical response with brief ACTH and cortisol pulses in the majority of the subjects. The greater waking increase in free cortisol found in this study is similar to that reported in recovered depressed patients (5). Also, we have recently identified a similar abnormality in unmedicated patients with acute major depression (Bhagwagar et al., unpublished). The salivary cortisol response to waking represents only a single aspect of HPA axis function; thus, we cannot conclude that neuroticism is associated with generally increased HPA axis activity. However, our observations are consistent with a greater ACTH response to waking or increased sensitivity of the adrenal gland to ACTH stimulation in subjects with high neuroticism scores.

Our group had no past or current history of depression, suggesting that increased waking cortisol is a risk factor for, rather than a consequence of, depression. It is possible that this abnormal response is inherited as part of the trait of neuroticism and, like high neuroticism scores, waking salivary cortisol levels show significant heritability (8). Another possibility, however, is that the elevated waking cortisol levels might be a consequence of experiencing more subjective stress. Nonetheless, our data suggest that elevated morning cortisol levels can exist in the absence of major depression. In this respect, our findings could resemble the abnormal HPA axis activity seen in the first-degree relatives of depressed patients who have not experienced depression (9). Longitudinal prospective studies initiated before the onset of depression are needed to untangle the relationship between neuroticism, cortisol hypersecretion, and depression.

Received Jan. 15, 2004; revisions received March 18 and April 20, 2004; accepted May 14, 2004. From the Department of Psychiatry, Warneford Hospital, University of Oxford. Address correspondence and reprint requests to Dr. Harmer, Department of Psychiatry, Warneford Hospital, Neurosciences Bldg., Oxford OX3 7JX, U.K.; (e-mail). Funded by the Medical Research Council and Wellcome Trust. Ms. Portella was supported by the Departament d’Universitats, Recerca i Societat de la Informació-DURSI (Generalitat de Catalunya), through grant 2002FI 00756. The authors thank Alison Reed for technical assistance.

Figure 1.

Figure 1. Mean Diurnal Variation in Salivary Cortisol Levels in Subjects With High or Low Neuroticism Scores

aSignificant difference between groups (t=2.33, df=28, p=0.03).

bSignificant difference between groups (t=2.59, df=28, p=0.01).

cSignificant difference between groups (t=2.74, df=28, p=0.01).

References

1. Kirk KM, Birley AJ, Statham DJ, Haddon B, Lake RI, Andrews JG, Martin NG: Anxiety and depression in twin and sib pairs extremely discordant and concordant for neuroticism: prodromus to a linkage study. Twin Res 2000; 3:299–309Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

2. Lake RI, Eaves LJ, Maes HH, Heath AC, Martin NG: Further evidence against the environmental transmission of individual differences in neuroticism from a collaborative study of 48,850 twins and relatives from two continents. Behav Genet 2000; 30:223–233Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

3. Pariante CM, Miller AH: Glucocorticoid receptors in major depression: relevance to pathophysiology and treatment. Biol Psychiatry 2001; 49:391–404Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

4. Pruessner JC, Wolf OT, Hellhammer DH, Buske-Kirschbaum A, von Auer K, Jobst S, Kaspers F, Kirschbaum C: Free cortisol levels after awakening: a reliable biological marker for the assessment of adrenocortical activity. Life Sci 1997; 61:2539–2549Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

5. Bhagwagar Z, Hafizi S, Cowen PJ: Increase in concentration of waking salivary cortisol in recovered patients with depression. Am J Psychiatry 2003; 160:1890–1891LinkGoogle Scholar

6. Fullerton J, Cubin M, Tiwari H, Wang C, Bomhra A, Davidson S, Miller S, Fairburn C, Goodwin G, Neale MC, Fiddy S, Mott R, Allison DB, Flint J: Linkage analysis of extremely discordant and concordant sibling pairs identifies quantitative-trait loci that influence variation in the human personality trait neuroticism. Am J Hum Genet 2003; 72:879–890Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

7. Wüst S, Federenko I, Hellhammer DH, Kirschbaum C: Genetic factors, perceived chronic stress, and the free cortisol response to awakening. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2000; 25:707–720Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

8. Bartels M, Van den Berg M, Sluyter F, Boomsma DI, de Geus EJ: Heritability of cortisol levels: review and simultaneous analysis of twin studies. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2003; 28:121–137Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

9. Holsboer F, Lauer CJ, Schreiber W, Krieg JC: Altered hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical regulation in healthy subjects at high familial risk for affective disorders. Neuroendocrinology 1995; 62:340–347Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar