The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×
Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1176/ajp.156.7.1075

Abstract

OBJECTIVE: The goal of this study was to assess the occurrence of anniversary reactions in Gulf War veterans 6 years after the conclusion of the war. METHOD: Subjects were administered questionnaires and asked to identify specific months of best and worst functioning and months of least or most symptoms of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) for the 12 months before the study. Months of negative experiences were compared with previously documented dates of exposure to traumatic events during the war. Similar reports were also obtained from the veterans’ spouses in order to assess corroborative evidence for the occurrence of anniversary reactions. RESULTS: Anniversary reactions occurred with a frequency greater than chance and most often in individuals exposed to a greater number of traumatic events. Overall, spouse reports matched the veterans’ reports of anniversary reactions. In addition, spouses identified anniversary reactions that were not endorsed by their veterans. CONCLUSIONS: These data suggest that anniversary reactions occur in numbers greater than those expected by chance, are correlated to the occurrence of traumatic events, and may be a part of the syndrome of PTSD.

An anniversary reaction has been described as a specifically timed, emotionally invested episode during which an individual experiences significant psychiatric or medical symptoms (1). It has been theorized that individuals suffering from such reactions have been sensitized by exposure to a specific traumatic event and that the effects of this sensitization are experienced later under circumstances that are reminiscent of the event. A review of the literature suggests that authors have differed quite significantly in their opinions about which circumstances or triggers are more important in precipitating anniversary reactions. While some have placed emphasis on the patient’s age, others have focused on the date and nature of the stressful event (26). Regardless of emphasis, there is a consensus that the patient’s distress is meaningfully connected to a specific, previously experienced traumatic event (1, 2, 4, 710).

Despite limited empirical data on anniversary reactions, it has become routine for many war-related posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) treatment programs to ask veterans about combat traumas and corresponding annual exacerbations of psychiatric symptoms (personal communication from American Lakes Program, Tacoma, Wash.; PTSD Program, Veterans Administration [VA] Medical Center, West Haven, Conn.; PTSD Program, Menlo Park VA Medical Center, Palo Alto, Calif.). It is believed that by learning about and recognizing anniversary dates, veterans can better understand, and control, their psychological distress. Models of treatment meant to prevent relapse instruct patients to anticipate anniversaries so that PTSD symptoms can be dealt with more effectively.

To date, only two longitudinal studies have empirically examined the timing of, and the symptoms experienced during, anniversary reactions to traumatic events. In the first, Borstein and Clayton (11) interviewed 92 widows and widowers on the first anniversary of their spouse’s death and identified anniversary reactions in four subjects. Each subject suffered from clinically significant depression and identified the anniversary date as the nidus of his or her psychological distress. While the study is promising, it has been difficult to interpret the precise meaning or implication of these data, since the incidence of anniversary reactions in this study is well within the range of occurrences that would be expected by chance (N=7.6).

In the second study (12), we investigated the frequency of anniversary reactions in a group of Gulf War veterans 2 years after the war. Veterans were invited to report on their psychological symptoms during the year (12 months) before their participation in the study. Each was asked to select, if applicable, the month during the previous year when he or she felt the worst and the month, if applicable, when he or she experienced the most stress symptoms (i.e., the specific PTSD symptoms according to DSM-III-R). If either of these months corresponded with the date during which a veteran experienced his or her most traumatic event while in the Persian Gulf, then the veteran was identified as having an anniversary reaction. Of the 59 subjects who participated in the study, 18 had anniversary reactions, twice the number that would be expected by chance. Moreover, veterans with anniversary reactions were more symptomatic on measures of PTSD compared with veterans who did not have anniversary reactions. This study was limited by the absence of third-party reports and by the fact that the veterans were asked about functioning and dates of trauma in the same setting.

The present study is a follow-up to our previous investigation of anniversary reactions in Gulf War veterans. It is also part of a larger investigation that has focused on the evolution of trauma-related symptoms in Gulf War veterans (13, 14). Using questionnaires based on DSM-III-R nosology, the current study evaluated the relationship between traumas experienced during the Gulf War and the timing and nature of postwar psychiatric distress.

More specifically, the current follow-up investigation was designed to evaluate whether anniversary reactions would still be present in the individuals who had them at the 2-year evaluation, determine whether the occurrence of anniversary reactions at 6 years would exceed that expected by chance, assess whether anniversary reactions would be found in individuals with greater levels of PTSD symptoms, and understand the relationship between the reports provided by the veterans and those provided by the veterans’ spouses. The rationale for examining the relationship between anniversary reactions and the level of PTSD symptoms was based on the findings of a previous study indicating that individuals with anniversary reactions also exhibited more PTSD symptoms (12). Self-report data were compared with spouse report data in order to evaluate whether anniversary reactions were observable by others.

In order to minimize bias in their reporting, the veterans were not asked about, nor asked to provide a date of, trauma. Instead, this information was obtained from the initial questionnaires completed by the veterans in May 1991—1 month after their return from the Gulf War.

METHOD

The subjects of the study were a subgroup of 119 soldiers, who, during their first monthly training session after the Gulf War, were recruited from two Connecticut Army Reserve units (the 142nd Medical and the 143rd Military Police) to participate in a longitudinal study of war-related symptoms (14, 15). Fifty-nine soldiers continued to participate in the longitudinal study at 2 years and were the subjects of our previous investigation of anniversary reactions to war-related stressors (12). Thirty-two of the 59 were available at the 6-year time point. Each had been a subject in previous investigations at our site (1214). Subjects were contacted by telephone and agreed to participate in the current investigation. All subjects gave written informed consent.

Measures

As reported in the investigation of the 2-year time point (12) to assess emotional functioning within the past 12 months, subjects were asked to indicate during which month of the previous year they had felt the best and during which month they had felt the worst. Additionally, each subject was asked to indicate during which month, if any, he or she experienced the most stress-related symptoms—i.e., the 17 symptoms of DSM-III-R for PTSD. Subjects also rated the month during which they experienced the least stress-related symptoms.

The initial questionnaire was administered in 1991, 1 month after their return from the war. Subjects had been asked to give the date and description of the most traumatic event they had experienced during their tour in the Persian Gulf. In this report, unlike in the previous report, subjects were not asked about the date of their most traumatic event so as not to create bias. Instead, the date noted in their original 1991 surveys, which specified his or her most traumatic event, was used to determine the presence or absence of anniversary reactions.

As in the earlier investigation, an anniversary reaction was defined as either 1) a match between the month of the most severe war-related traumatic event and the month identified as worst during the previous year or 2) a match between the month of the most severe war-related traumatic event and the month during which PTSD specific symptoms were most prominent. All subjects completed the questionnaires as well as the interviews during August and September 1997. Neither of these months corresponded to the dates of military deployment or to exposure to military trauma.

Subjects were also administered the PTSD Symptom Scale (13, 14), a valid self-report inventory consisting of 35 items derived from DSM-III-R. It provided a measure of both frequency and intensity of PTSD symptoms within the 30 days before participation in the study. After completing the questionnaire, subjects participated in a direct interview with the principal investigator (C.A.M.). Each was asked about his or her responses to the questionnaire in order to gain a more complete understanding of the nature of the responses.

Finally, married subjects or those who lived with a significant other were given a questionnaire for his or her spouse or significant other to fill out without the veteran’s assistance. The questionnaire was identical to the one given the veterans, except that it asked the spouse to identify which months, if any, over the past year the veteran had felt the best and the worst and had experienced the least and most symptoms of stress.

Data Analysis

Since an anniversary reaction was defined as either a match between the date of most severe trauma and the month of feeling worst or a match between the date of trauma and the month of greatest PTSD-specific symptoms, there was a two in 12 (17%) probability that an anniversary reaction would exist by chance alone. The number of anniversary reactions within the group as a whole, and within each unit, was compared with this probability factor in order to determine whether these matches occurred at a greater frequency than that expected by chance.

Data from the 6-year time point were compared with data from the 2-year time point in order to determine whether identified cases were new or chronic in nature. Comparisons between the 2- and 6-year data also permitted a determination as to whether any of the identified anniversary reactions at 2 years no longer existed at 6 years. We performed t tests between the total PTSD Symptom Scale scores to determine whether individuals with anniversary reactions differed from unit members without anniversary reactions.

Finally, spouse reports were examined in order to determine whether there was agreement between the veteran and his or her spouse about the existence of anniversary reactions. The number of anniversary reactions identified by the spouse only—and not the veteran—was also calculated.

RESULTS

In the total group (N=32), the observed number of subjects who had an anniversary reaction at 2 years was 12. The observed number of subjects in the group who had anniversary reactions at 6 years was also 12 (38%). This was more than twice the number of occurrences (N=5.4, 17%) that would be expected by chance alone. Eight of the 12 cases identified at the 6-year time point represented chronic cases, whereas four were new.

Within the group of 20 subjects who were from a medical unit, one would expect 3.4 anniversary reactions by chance alone. By contrast, 10 subjects selected a month corresponding to his or her previously identified date of trauma. This occurrence was three times greater than that expected by chance. Similarly, in the group of subjects from a military police unit (N=12), one would expect two anniversary reactions because of chance alone. Within this group, only two subjects selected a month corresponding to their identified date of trauma. Thus, the occurrence of anniversary reactions was greater than that expected by chance in the group of veterans as a whole and in the group from the medical unit but not in the group from the military police unit.

Comparisons of the total PTSD scores between the group of individuals with anniversary reactions and the group without anniversary reactions (mean=12.7, SD=12.6, and mean=5.8, SD=1.3, respectively) revealed a statistically significant effect of group (t=–2.07, df=30, p<0.05). Thus, individuals with anniversary reactions had more symptoms of PTSD than those who did not have anniversary reactions. In the group as a whole, only four (12.5%) of the 32 subjects met the criteria for PTSD on the PTSD Symptom Scale. All four of these subjects with PTSD were identified as having anniversary reactions.

In 11 of 12 subjects with an anniversary reaction, the associated trauma involved witnessing the death or serious injury of another human being. Exposure to mines while in a war zone constituted the associated trauma for the remaining subjects who had anniversary reactions.

Thirteen of the 32 subjects were married. Of these, five reported anniversary reactions. Four of the five respective spouses matched their veteran’s report and identified their veteran as suffering from an anniversary reaction. The spouse whose report did not match that of her veteran identified her husband as suffering from an anniversary reaction during the month of February—not January, as the veteran had indicated. This appeared to be a negative finding. However, examination of the veteran’s narrative description of his worst trauma, which was logged in his January 1991 questionnaire, revealed that he had incorrectly paired his date of trauma (January 1991) with an incident that was documented by the military (and by other members of his unit) to have occurred during the month of February 1991. Thus, by his own report, he suffered from an anniversary reaction during the month in which he believed the incident had occurred, whereas by his wife’s report, he suffered an anniversary reaction during the month of February—the month during which the trauma had actually occurred.

Four of the married subjects who did not personally endorse one were identified as suffering from an anniversary reaction by his or her spouse. None of these veterans had previously endorsed anniversary reactions at the 2-year time point, and each was from the military police unit. When these veterans were asked about the apparent inconsistency between their data and those provided by their spouse, each suggested to the interviewer that the spouse’s report was a more accurate description of his or her behavior. If these third-party spouse reports are considered equal in validity to those given by the veterans where anniversary reactions are concerned, nine (69%) of the 13 married subjects would be identified as suffering from anniversary reactions. Such a percentage would represent a sizable (albeit speculative) increase from that calculated from self-report data alone (31%–38%).

The most frequently endorsed symptoms of PTSD that were identified by veterans as being associated with an anniversary reaction were irritability, sleep disturbance, intrusive memories, efforts to avoid thinking about the war, emotional numbness, and reactivity to reminders of war-related events. The spouses did not mention symptoms of memories, intrusive thoughts, or avoidance of thinking but instead endorsed (in descending frequency) irritability, sleep disturbance, emotional distance, emotional numbness, and reactivity to reminders of war-related events. Thus, the spouses reported what might be considered more objective symptoms of PTSD compared with subjective items such as intrusive memories. This supports the idea that the reports provided by the spouses are valid and not simply copies of the veterans’ reports.

Only three subjects (9%) selected a feeling-best month and one subject (3%) a least-symptoms-of-stress month that corresponded to their previously reported dates of most severe trauma. This frequency of endorsement is cumulatively less than that expected by chance alone (17%) and supports the idea that anniversary reactions are predominantly distressing experiences.

Of note, no significant differences were observed when comparing total PTSD Symptom Scale scores between the subjects who dropped out of the study (N=27) and those who remained in the study at the 6-year time point. Similarly, of the original 18 veterans identified as having anniversary reactions, six dropped out and 12 remained in the study. Thus, the occurrence of anniversary reactions in the dropout group also exceeded, albeit modestly, the number expected by chance alone (N=4.5).

DISCUSSION

This follow-up study provides empirical evidence for anniversary reactions in Gulf War combat veterans. As in our earlier report (12), anniversary reactions occurred in the group as a whole with a frequency that was nearly twice that expected by chance alone. Of note, all but two of the anniversary reactions occurred in members of the medical unit, where the number of such reactions (N=10) was approximately three times greater than that expected by chance (N=3.4). In contrast, only two members of the military police unit experienced anniversary reactions, the frequency expected by chance (N=2.0).

The higher frequency of anniversary reactions in the medical unit may be related to the types of traumatic events experienced by members of that unit. It appears that some events are more likely than others to be matched with anniversary reactions. The majority of anniversary reactions (N=11 of 12) were associated with events involving loss of life, suggesting that witnessing the loss of life may serve as a powerful stimulus for the development of anniversary reactions. Consistent with this notion is the observation that the four cases of anniversary reactions that were no longer present at the 6-year time point had developed in response to SCUD missile attacks or to being activated for the war. In addition, the four new cases that were identified at the 6-year time point were linked to events that included witnessing the loss of human life.

Individuals with anniversary reactions reported significantly greater symptoms of PTSD as measured by the PTSD Symptom Scale. This raises an important question about the relationship between anniversary reactions and PTSD. Of the four subjects in the study who met the criteria for PTSD, all four (100%) had anniversary reactions. Of the subjects who did not have PTSD, only 29% had anniversary reactions. Eight of the 12 anniversary reactions in this study had been identified at 2 years and continued to be present at 6 years. At 6 years, there were four new cases, suggesting that anniversary reactions, like PTSD symptoms, may be chronic or show a delayed onset. Taken together, these findings raise the possibility that anniversary reactions may be an important aspect of PTSD that has not been included in the formal DSM-IV criteria.

Four anniversary reactions that existed at 2 years were no longer reported at 6 years. It is not possible to know the number of cases that may have subsided in the dropout group; however, in the group that remained in the study, 22% (four of the original 18 cases) were no longer endorsed 6 years after exposure to trauma.

The veterans’ spouses provided information that both coincided and diverged from that given by the veterans. There was a high degree of agreement (four out of five) between the veterans who endorsed an anniversary reaction and their respective spouses. These third-party reports lend credibility to the idea that anniversary reactions may be pronounced enough that they can be observed by others. In fact, there is evidence that, at times, they may be more apparent to others than to the veterans themselves. This raises the question as to whether anniversary reactions are conscious or nonconscious processes. Evidence for a nonconscious process may be found in the fact that several of the spouses identified anniversary reactions in veterans who personally did not endorse such reactions. These women (and a man) each mentioned relatively objective symptoms (irritability, sleep disturbances, or emotional numbness) as evidence for their claims.

It is consistent with our clinical observations that men in these military units may downplay symptoms and emotional reactions. It is possible that some individuals did indeed have anniversary reactions but did not report them. Among the 13 married subjects, where one would expect the occurrence of anniversary reactions to be 2.2 by chance, they numbered nine—when the spouse’s report was used. This suggests that future investigations exploring the nature and existence of anniversary reactions should involve third-party reports.

The limitations of this study are clearly those of a small study group size. The small group size reflects the inherent difficulties encountered in a longitudinal study. Many subjects from the original group of veterans were unavailable for participation in the study at the 6-year time point. However, it is likely that the current data are representative of the group as a whole, since we have previously found that subjects who dropped out of the study did not differ in the degree of symptoms from those who continued to participate (14).

The empirical evidence for anniversary reactions is robust. However, the current data must be viewed with caution, since the study of anniversary reactions is nascent. The data from the present study do not permit a conclusion as to whether such reactions are the result of conditioned response, trauma sensitization, or an unconscious time-keeping process—all of which have been proposed in the literature. However, it is theoretically possible to design studies that might address this issue. For example, it may be fruitful to assess psychobiological responding (e.g., as sensory processing, conditioned responding, acoustic startle, yohimbine challenge studies) before, during, and after anniversary reactions in victims of trauma. It seems plausible that psychobiological responses, and perhaps responses to medications, may be significantly different depending on time of year (15).

The current data are relevant to psychotherapy as well. Psychotherapeutic issues may also be approached with the concept of anniversary reactions in mind. Clinicians and patients might reasonably expect that at least 30% of trauma-exposed individuals have a likelihood of experiencing or exhibiting significant psychiatric distress (including more symptoms of PTSD) on the anniversary of their trauma. Clinicians and patients might acknowledge the trauma-related source of the distress rather than assuming that the patient’s distress reflects a non-trauma-related contemporary issue. This understanding might result in a therapeutic clinical focus on the index trauma and the corresponding symptoms of reexperiencing (36, 17).

Received May 15, 1998; revision received Nov. 30, 1998; accepted Jan. 4, 1999. From the Department of Psychiatry, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, Conn.; the National Center for Post Traumatic Stress Disorder, VA Medical Center; and the State University of New York at Stony Brook, Long Island. Address reprint requests to Dr. Morgan, 116A, VA Medical Center, 950 Campbell Ave., West Haven, CT 06516; (e-mail)

References

1. Weiss E: The clinical significance of the anniversary reaction. General Practitioner 1958; 27:117–119Google Scholar

2. Beratis S, Gourzis P, Gabriel J: Anniversary reaction as seasonal mood disorder. Psychopathology 1994; 27:14–18Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

3. Marsh RW, Peery CJ: Anniversary reactions in military aviators. Aviat Space Environ Med 1977; 48:61–64MedlineGoogle Scholar

4. Miller SP: Amenorrhea and anniversary reaction in a woman presenting with chest pain. Am J Psychiatry 1978; 135:120–121LinkGoogle Scholar

5. Minz I: The anniversary reaction: a response to the unconscious sense of time. J Am Psychoanal Assoc 1971; 19:720–735Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

6. Musaph H: Anniversary reaction as a symptom of grief in traumatized persons. Isr J Psychiatry Relat Sci 1990; 27:175–179MedlineGoogle Scholar

7. Cavenar JO Jr, Nash JL, Maltbie AA: Anniversary reactions masquerading as manic-depressive illness. Am J Psychiatry 1977; 134:1273–1276Google Scholar

8. Green CM: Filicidal impulses as an anniversary reaction to childhood incest. Am J Psychother 1982; 36:264–271Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

9. Sanger MD: The psychosomatic allergy. Psychosomatics 1970; 11:473–476Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

10. Birtchnell J: In search of correspondences between age at psychiatric breakdown and parental age at death—anniversary reactions. Br J Med Psychol 1981; 54:111–120Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

11. Bornstein PE, Clayton PJ: The anniversary reaction. Dis Nerv Syst 1972; 33:470–472MedlineGoogle Scholar

12. Morgan CA, Kingham P, Nicolaou A, Southwick SM: Anniversary reactions in Gulf War veterans: a naturalistic inquiry 2 years after the Gulf War. J Trauma Stress 1998; 11:165–170Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

13. Southwick SM, Morgan A, Nagy LM, Bremner D, Nicolaou AL, Johnson DR, Rosenheck R, Charney DS: Trauma-related symptoms in veterans of Operation Desert Storm: a preliminary report. Am J Psychiatry 1993; 150:1524–1528Google Scholar

14. Southwick SM, Morgan CA III, Darnell A, Bremner D, Nicolaou AL, Nagy LM, Charney DS: Trauma-related symptoms in veterans of Operation Desert Storm: a 2-year follow-up. Am J Psychiatry 1995; 152:1150–1155Google Scholar

15. Wang S, Wilson JP, Mason JW: Stages of decomposition in combat-related posttraumatic stress disorder: a new conceptual model. Integr Physiol Behav Sci 1996; 31:237–253Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

16. Foa EB, Rothbaum BO, Rigg DS, Murdock TB: Treatment of posttraumatic stress disorder in rape victims: a comparison between cognitive-behavioral procedures and counseling. J Consult Clin Psychol 1991; 59:715–723Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

17. Keane TM, Fairbank JA, Caddell JM, Zimering RT: Implosive (flooding) therapy reduces symptoms of PTSD in Vietnam combat veterans. Behavioral Therapy 1989; 20:245–260CrossrefGoogle Scholar