The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×

Abstract

Clinically significant separation anxiety disorder in childhood leads to adult panic disorder and other anxiety disorders. The prevailing pathophysiological model of anxiety disorders, which emphasizes extinction deficits of fear-conditioned responses, does not fully consider the role of separation anxiety. Pathological early childhood attachments have far-reaching consequences for the later adult ability to experience and internalize positive relationships in order to develop mental capacities for self-soothing, anxiety tolerance, affect modulation, and individuation. Initially identified in attachment research, the phenomenon of separation anxiety is supported by animal model, neuroimaging, and genetic studies. A role of oxytocin is postulated. Adults, inured to their anxiety, often do not identify separation anxiety as problematic, but those who develop anxiety and mood disorders respond more poorly to both pharmacological and psychotherapeutic interventions. This poorer response may reflect patients’ difficulty in forming and maintaining attachments, including therapeutic relationships. Psychotherapies that focus on relationships and separation anxiety may benefit patients with separation anxiety by using the dyadic therapist-patient relationship to recapture and better understand important elements of earlier pathological parent-child relationships.

The fear extinction model of anxiety, a unifying construct in the Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) of the National Institute of Mental Health, focuses on biological underpinnings and plasticity of a critical behavioral response. Despite its many empirical strengths, it provides an incomplete model of anxiety (1, 2). Separation anxiety and attachment models hold promise for translational research, address aspects of anxiety complementary to fear conditioning, and may warrant consideration in working theoretical models of anxiety.

Anxiety induced by separation from close attachment figures is normal and adaptive in early childhood (35). Yet if this prominent emotional state persists into later childhood, adolescence, and adulthood, separation anxiety becomes linked to increasingly pathological self-perceptions and inadequate homeostatic regulation of internal object relations. An individual with separation anxiety feels unable to function in the absence of the mother or her surrogate (4, 5). Separation anxiety is often comorbid with mood, anxiety, and personality disorders (6). Its developmental role in panic disorder has long been considered formative (711). From the perspective of neural systems underlying fear and reward, separation anxiety may indicate excessive activation of fear circuits in response to separation and overactivation of reward circuits with reunion. However, it seems possible, even likely, that observed functional differences in fear and reward circuitry in individuals with separation anxiety do not cause but, rather, result from abnormalities or deficits in systems underlying social representation and cognition (12, 13). While the fear conditioning paradigm illuminates anxiety resulting from trauma, it sheds less light on the developmental pathway of chronic anxiety of more insidious onset. The following case illustrates this point.

“Lena,” a 25-year-old graduate student, had multiple daily severe, terrifying panic attacks wherein she felt she could not breathe and was dying. She described severe agoraphobia, inability to travel anywhere alone (including to her doctor’s office), terror of being any distance from her home, a new inability to drive because of panic, and frantic clinging to her girlfriend, “Jane,” toward whom she had mixed feelings. She sought treatment after being forced to take leave from her graduate program because she could not drive alone in her car to commute to school. Her DSM-IV diagnoses on the Anxiety Disorders Interview Schedule for DSM-IV (14) at intake were panic disorder (with a score of 7 out of a possible 8, indicating severe symptoms), agoraphobia (7 of 8, severe), and generalized anxiety disorder (3 of 8, indicating trait-level symptoms not meeting the full DSM criteria). Lena’s anxiety had increased to panic proportions several months before, when she began to contemplate breaking up with her previous girlfriend. Careful history revealed that anxiety had dominated Lena since earliest childhood.

Terrified and highly anxious throughout childhood, Lena experienced severe anxiety daily when her mother left for work. She reported crying throughout the day, even when her father or grandmother was present. Throughout childhood she could sleep only when sharing her mother’s bed, a situation that contributed to her parents’ divorce. At age 9, when her mother expelled her from her bed, Lena began sleeping with her younger sister. She remained frightened of being alone, especially at night. At college, she immediately began sharing her roommate’s bed. When she sought treatment at age 25, she had never spent a single night alone in a bed, having frantically juggled family, friends, and lovers to avoid this terrifying experience, which she described as “being alone in the void.”

Lena’s parents had sent her repeatedly to psychiatrists and therapists through childhood and adolescence. Although she had sometimes found it helpful to discuss aspects of her life, she never mentioned her terror of separation to any therapist, knowing it was “definitely unusual and really embarrassing.” She considered it an accepted, nearly imperceptible, if highly embarrassing backdrop to other aspects of her chaotic life.

Importance of Separation Anxiety Across Anxiety Disorders

Central attachment relationships form the core of human emotional development. The formation and qualities of the dyadic bond between the mother (primary caretaker) and infant create the nexus of an “internal working model” of the mind/brain (4, 5). Bowlby described this internal working model as the safe base from which the toddler can explore the surrounding world. This model in turn often affects patterns in future adult relationships. We describe this model as a separation-sensitive social schema.

The inherent dependency of the human infant makes anxiety normal for young children (like other mammals) when separated from caregivers. In contrast, separation anxiety disorder in childhood and adulthood describes a nonnormative, pervasive anxiety state accentuated by separations from close attachment figures at developmental junctures where the need for proximity to attachment figures is no longer adaptive. DSM-IV identified separation anxiety disorder solely as a childhood anxiety syndrome—indeed, as the only anxiety disorder listed under “Disorders Usually First Diagnosed in Infancy, Childhood, or Adolescence” (15). DSM-5 groups it more broadly among the anxiety disorders. The National Comorbidity Survey Replication epidemiological survey documented a 6.6% lifetime prevalence of separation anxiety disorder in adults across a large national general population sample (16). Prevalence ranges between 12% and 40% in adult psychiatric clinic settings (17, 18). (The studies by Silove et al. [17] and Pini et al. [18] had two of the largest clinical samples used to investigate adult separation anxiety disorder. Silove et al. [17] found a consistently higher degree of symptom severity and impairment associated with separation anxiety than with other adult anxiety disorders. Pini et al. [18] found delineation between the onset ages for patients with childhood separation anxiety only and those who had the disorder in both childhood and adulthood.)

Separation anxiety has both heritable (genetic) and social (experiential/epigenetic) origins. Some patients presumably have an inborn anxious propensity (i.e., lower thresholds); others may be phenocopies due to anxious or anxiety-provoking caretaking (anxious children have anxious mothers [19]); still others may represent an interaction of genes and environment. Anxiety about separations in 1–3-year-olds is a normative sign of healthy relationships (19), but its pathological persistence impairs children’s comfort in independent exploration and autonomy, and it complicates age-normative developmental tasks, such as sleeping without a parent or attending school, thus interfering with age and stage adaptation (10).

Patients with separation anxiety disorder have greater disability, more severe depression and anxiety symptoms, and larger stress responses than do other anxiety disorder patients routinely treated in anxiety disorder clinics (17). In cross-sectional findings, 75% of adults with anxiety disorders seeking treatment at anxiety disorders clinics report having had separation anxiety disorder in childhood (20). A recent longitudinal twin study suggests that a common genetic diathesis underlies childhood separation anxiety disorder and adult panic attacks (21). A meta-analysis of case-control, retrospective, and cohort studies associated childhood separation anxiety disorder with panic disorder and other anxiety disorders in adulthood (10). Nonetheless, the developmental perspective that informs such studies—specifically, links between separation anxiety and the course of other anxiety and mood disorders—has lacked sufficient articulation (20).

Table 1 contrasts summary findings on separation anxiety with findings on fear extinction in anxiety disorders.

TABLE 1. Comparison of Fear Extinction and Potential Separation/Attachment Models of Anxiety
DomainFear Extinction ModelSeparation/Attachment Model
Paradigmatic disorderPosttraumatic stress disorderPanic with or without agoraphobia
Psychosocial treatmentExposurePsychodynamic or interpersonal therapy
Animal modelFear conditioningDisrupted maternal care producing greater hypothalamic/pituitary responsivity to stress
Neuroimaging findingsHeightened activity in the amygdala and dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (fear expression), diminished activity in the ventromedial prefrontal cortexCircuitry underlying separation-sensitive social schemas and attachment: subcortical areas (amygdala, hippocampus, striatum) and cortical limbic areas (insula, cingulate); may imply predisposing endophenotypes from circuitry involved in attention, learning, and executive control (medial prefrontal cortex, superior temporal sulcus, and temporoparietal junction) (22)
GeneticsPotential systems: FK506-binding protein 5 (FKBP5, 6), brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF, 23), serotonin transporter (24)Potential systems: oxytocin receptor (25), vasopressin 1A receptor (26), D2 dopamine receptor (DRD2, 27)
TABLE 1. Comparison of Fear Extinction and Potential Separation/Attachment Models of Anxiety
Enlarge table

Genetic and Epigenetic Animal Models of Separation Anxiety

Separation anxiety has deep evolutionary roots. The larger literature on primate and subprimate mammals is selectively sampled below.

As juveniles, rats bred for high levels of infant calling responses upon separation from mothers showed major changes in autonomic responses when isolated. These animals engaged in significantly less social play behavior than control rats. As adults, they emerged significantly more slowly from familiar enclosed spaces into open areas and showed the distinctively passive, “helpless” behavioral pattern in a swim test, validated for detecting vulnerability to depression and anxiety in laboratory rats (28, 29).

Mother bonnet macaques showed rejection behavior toward their infants (30) when exposed to a variable feeding delivery schedule, a laboratory-induced environmental stress (31). The stressed mothers’ distant behavior toward their babies led to infants’ fearfulness and clinging to their mothers, difficulties in both leaving mothers and interacting socially, and lifelong “timidity” accompanied by high stress responses. The investigators (32) identified a specific genetic-environmental interaction constituting a risk for developing this abnormal mother-infant relationship that produced chronic anxiety later in life. High levels of maternal “anxiety” or stress appeared to mediate these changes, impairing the mothers’ ability to form normal attachments with their infants.

Diorio and Meaney (33) found that changes in rat postnatal maternal behavior in response to environmental stress during pregnancy yielded increased fear behaviors and adrenocortical responses in their adult offspring. The investigators traced these transgenerational effects to epigenetic changes in brain gene expression patterns throughout the offspring’s development. While possibly adaptive in the setting of acute stressors in adulthood, under normal conditions these changes placed the offspring at risk for multiple pathologies and lifelong heightened stress responses. These early nurture differences generated long-term changes in gene expression levels throughout life (28, 34).

Adult Separation Anxiety: Prevalence in Anxiety and Mood Disorder Patients

The traits of separation sensitivity, excessive dependence on close attachment figures, and anxiety surrounding separations (the standard threshold is a score of 35 or higher on the Panic-Agoraphobic Spectrum Self-Report [35]) have been linked to development of complicated grief (36) following loss. A study comparing 53 subjects with complicated grief to 50 healthy bereaved comparison subjects found levels of adult separation anxiety significantly higher (p<0.001) in the group with complicated grief than in the comparison subjects. In both groups a higher level of separation anxiety was associated with higher depressive and manic symptom levels on the self-report version of the Structured Clinical Interview for Mood Spectrum (MOODS-SR) (37), and in both groups mood variations appeared dimensionally related to separation anxiety (38). Greater depressive, bipolar, and anxiety disorder comorbidity in the group with complicated grief, however, makes it difficult to interpret these observations. A separate study of 283 subjects (36) significantly associated childhood separation anxiety with development of complicated grief (odds ratio: 3.2, 95% confidence interval [CI]: 1.2–8.9).

High levels of anxiety generally (39), high levels of separation anxiety, and high rates of panic spectrum symptoms, which include measures of separation anxiety, are common among patients with bipolar I disorder (50% prevalence of high separation anxiety level in bipolar I disorder [40, 41]), complicating course and treatment response. Patients with bipolar disorder and high levels of panic spectrum symptoms report worse depression after short-term treatment, higher rates of suicidal ideation (49% in patients with high levels of panic spectrum symptoms versus 19% in those with low panic symptom levels), and a 6-month delay in response to short-term treatment relative to bipolar I patients with low panic symptom levels (44 weeks versus 17 weeks) (40). Childhood and adult separation anxiety are associated with mood instability and development of bipolar II disorder or cyclothymia in adulthood (4244). Both childhood separation anxiety and adult separation anxiety disorder were common in patients with cluster B personality disorders (29% in patients with cluster B disorders versus 10% in those without cluster B disorders, p<0.01) and patients with cluster C disorders (55% in patients with cluster C disorders versus 26% in those without, p<0.01) in a study of 397 adult outpatients with primary anxiety disorders (45).

Negative Effect of Separation Anxiety on Treatment Outcome

Through still unknown mechanisms, separation anxiety is associated with poor response to treatments of adult anxiety and mood disorders, potentially through disruptions in the therapeutic relationship (40, 4649). In adults, co-occurring separation anxiety, as well as anxiety generally (49), negatively moderates treatment response in major depression, worsening symptom chronicity and quality of life (46, 49). Among 226 treated patients with major depression, separation anxiety accounted for 24% of the variance associated with impaired quality of life (46). High levels of panic spectrum symptoms inhibited the benefits of interpersonal psychotherapy (50), both alone and in combination with selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs). Panic-related symptoms decreased the rate of response to interpersonal therapy alone from 68.4% to 43.5% and prolonged the time to response after the addition of an SSRI to interpersonal therapy from 10.3 weeks to 18.1 weeks (47).

The only published study of which we are aware that investigated the moderating effect of adult separation anxiety on the response to cognitive-behavioral therapy (CBT) for patients with panic disorder similarly found that separation anxiety lessened CBT response. Among 256 patients with primary panic disorder with or without agoraphobia who were given 11 weeks of CBT, the overall response rate for the intent-to-treat group was 44.1% and the rate for completers was 65.6%; the odds ratio for nonresponse among patients with separation anxiety was 3.74 (95% CI, 1.8–7.8) (51). Separation anxiety predicted medication nonresponse in an open-label trial (SSRIs and tricyclic antidepressants, alone or combined, by algorithm) in 57 subjects with panic disorder with agoraphobia (p=0.001) (48).

Family Context

Childhood separation anxiety often arises in the context of anxious parenting (5257). A parental sense of incompetence in facing children’s anxiety (54, 58) can aggravate anxiety symptoms, even in the absence of parental anxiety disorders per se.

Because separation anxiety clusters in families, it may not emerge as a treatment focus in adults, who normalize living with profound, life-limiting restrictions that are consonant with family worries and accepted frameworks. Yet these predispositions may later erupt into overwhelming anxiety and mood disorders (7, 20). Children’s anxiety surrounding separations can echo the often imperceptible, background-noise quality of separation anxiety in adults, so parents may not notice the children’s separation anxiety; this situation offers a sense of typical patterns in such families (59, 60).

Bowlby’s work on attachment, and the literature his findings have engendered, elucidate the centrality of the infant-caretaker relationship to subsequent lifelong patterns of attachment quality, quality of relationships, and mental health. Bowlby highlighted the developmental premise that small children’s mothers buffer and externally modulate overwhelming external stimuli that the biologically immature infant cannot integrate. Bowlby contrasted normal development, in which anxiety levels do not limit the child’s capacity to explore age-appropriate developmental challenges, with the development of anxiety-laden, insecure attachments that underlie separation anxiety and limit exploration of the environment and the child’s sense of safety.

Normal, secure attachments arise from children’s maturational ability to use their mothers as “a secure base from which to confidently explore the environment” (4, p. 13). When mothers reassure and encourage exploratory behavior within the child’s mastery, children develop a secure sense of competence in their (social) environments. Anxious, ambivalent, depressed, withdrawn, or neglectful caretakers may foster insecure attachment, generating inhibition and anxious avoidance (5).

Toddlers manifest a range of attachment types as they develop the physical capacity for locomotion (61). Children’s security in exploring the environment beyond the mother’s or caretaker’s presence relates inversely to the degree of separation anxiety. Secure attachment describes Mahler’s separation-individuation paradigm: a toddler’s comfort in exploring the environment, briefly checking back with the mother or caretaker for security (“refueling”), then setting out on new adventures away from the mother or caretaker (61). The mother’s calm encouragement of the toddler’s exploration fosters development of secure attachments. In insecure attachments, however, some toddlers become anxious and inhibited, manifesting fear and various stress response patterns, including freezing, becoming mute, weeping, or crumpling when separated from their mothers (19).

Although anxious, inhibited early attachment styles have been linked to development of childhood anxiety disorders (54), a meta-analysis connecting inhibited attachment with internalizing disorders (62) found this link “small” (Cohen’s d=0.15). Nevertheless, many observations show that mother-child attachment relationships, which form the core of the separation-sensitive social schema, powerfully influence the development of anxiety disorders. Parenting style, the level of parental anxiety, especially surrounding separations, and parents’ ability to tolerate their child’s distress without urgently intervening all affect the onset of anxiety disorders in childhood, irrespective of whether the parents have anxiety disorders (54, 55, 63, 64). Humans, like other mammals (28, 3032), display a complex relationship of genetic predisposition, early experiences (the first 3 years of human life), and development of anxiety in later life (65). Whatever the environmental contributions to this anxiety may be, developing insecure central attachments does not require trauma-level criteria, such as DSM posttraumatic stress disorder criterion A (e.g., child abuse). Attachment style likely depends on far more subtle parent-child interactions, responses to children’s distress and anxiety, and the available capacity to moderate stimuli (52, 58).

Social Support

Secure attachment styles and supportive social relationships putatively buffer the negative emotional and physical impacts of acute, overwhelming stress, protecting against PTSD and other disorders (53, 66, 67). Thus, separation anxiety emerged as a specific risk factor for PTSD in burned children (68). Degree of maternal stress as well as physical separation from mothers correlated directly with children’s anxiety responses to Scud missile attacks (66). Anxious attachment style and high separation anxiety likely compromise the ability to modulate stress with social supports for several reasons: people with separation anxiety develop fewer social supports, they approach them more cautiously, and the social supports they have are more emotionally fraught, less “supportive” (66, 69).

Treatment Implications

Relative Ego-Syntonicity of Separation Anxiety

Patients with separation anxiety have profound sensitivity to transitions and losses, including those experienced in therapeutic relationships (7). Often normalized, so that patients and clinicians may hardly recognize its presence or potency, separation anxiety fuels chronic anxiety and a global sense of inadequacy and incompetence that can undermine psychiatric treatments of any modality (7). To enable change, the psychotherapist must consistently focus on separation anxiety and the distortions it evokes to facilitate its verbal articulation. It is interesting that contemporary communication devices (e.g., mobile phones) may cloak a pathological need for immediate contact, making careful clinical evaluation even more important.

Psychosocial Interventions

The dyadic nature of psychotherapy leads us to predict that attachment styles can affect psychotherapy effectiveness. Conversely, attachment styles can change: several studies have shown that psychotherapy can render attachment style more secure (7072). Indeed, attachment can differ among various dyads for the same individual, although formative early dyadic models strongly influence later central attachments.

Key active ingredients of psychotherapy include the capacities to trust, to share, and to feel soothed by the therapist (71). Psychotherapies differ in their degree of focus on attachment and separation-sensitive social schemata. Behavioral therapies for anxiety tend to focus on the fear extinction paradigm (73) rather than attachment per se. In contrast, psychodynamic and interpersonal psychotherapies for anxiety focus on relationships and associated affects. These therapies actively address improving patients’ capacity for reflection and helping them to recognize and tolerate emotional responses and perceived dangers surrounding attachment (69). Therapists attuned to patients’ separation fears may detect them in the transference or in outside relationships and can use dynamic or interpersonal approaches to articulate and help patients to better understand them, thereby decreasing their intensity. This work presumably increases patients’ reflective function (74). A putative mediator of affect-focused psychotherapies, reflective function measures emotional understanding of one’s formative relationships and one’s own and others’ attachments and emotions (75). Reflective function studies may be useful in delineating mechanisms of change occurring in psychiatric symptoms through modulation of attachment and reflection (7476).

Affect-Focused Psychotherapies Targeting Separation Anxiety

The negative impact of separation anxiety and panic spectrum symptoms on the outcomes of treatment for mood and anxiety disorders suggests that research should evaluate psychotherapy interventions targeting relationships, attachment, and associated affects. Indeed, the potency of separation anxiety argues for developing better-tailored treatments across disorders (49, 77). We highlight two small pilot psychotherapy trials in which some of us were involved.

Cyranowski et al. (77) treated 18 subjects with primary major depression and high levels of lifetime panic spectrum symptoms (35) in an open trial of interpersonal psychotherapy adapted to focus on depression, anxiety, and anxious avoidance. Fourteen (78%) subjects met remission criteria after 12 weeks, with improvements (p<0.0001) across all measured domains: depression, anxiety, and psychosocial functioning. A randomized trial comparing this treatment with supportive therapy is further evaluating this approach.

In another study, 49 adults with primary panic disorder with or without agoraphobia were randomly assigned to panic-focused psychodynamic psychotherapy (78) or to applied relaxation training (unpublished manual of J.A. Cerny et al., 1984), an efficacious non-separation-anxiety-focused intervention for panic disorder (79). The principles of panic-focused psychodynamic psychotherapy emphasize free association, centrality of the transference, and unconscious thoughts underlying physical sensations of panic and difficulty with separation and autonomy. The therapist focuses on these processes as they relate to panic symptoms. Common themes of difficulty with separations and unconscious rage inform interpretive interventions. Panic-focused psychodynamic psychotherapy, as an affect-focused psychotherapy, specifically targets separation anxiety as a core component of understanding panic; patients’ high separation anxiety levels constitute a central organizing element in their self-view as incompetent and unable to manage developmentally normative tasks without the presence of their central attachment figures. The inevitable repetition of this dyadic pattern with the therapist within a time-limited 24-session, 12-week format heightens the opportunity to work with separation anxiety and permits the reexperiencing and better understanding in verbal form of this affectively charged paradigm (7, 78).

Eleven of 23 patients receiving applied relaxation training (48%) and 15 of 26 in panic-focused psychodynamic psychotherapy (58%) had high baseline levels of current separation anxiety and panic spectrum symptoms, i.e., a score of 35 or higher on the Panic-Agoraphobic Spectrum Self-Report (37). A significant interaction between treatment and baseline score predicted panic symptom improvement at treatment end (b=−11.0, t=−3.68, df=44, p<0.001), indicating that baseline severity of separation anxiety moderated the effect of panic-focused psychodynamic psychotherapy on panic symptoms. Panic-focused therapy had significantly greater efficacy than relaxation training among patients with high levels of separation anxiety. Thus, patients with primary panic disorder with higher baseline separation anxiety levels responded particularly robustly to panic-focused psychodynamic psychotherapy, but not to applied relaxation training.

When Lena began panic-focused psychodynamic psychotherapy, she needed a friend to accompany her from a distant suburb because of her terror of traveling alone. The therapist first explored Lena’s worst panic attacks, which had occurred in cars when she was in the midst of deciding to break up with her last serious girlfriend. During panic attacks, she felt terrified and completely isolated, as if her car were a “tomb” and “as though I’ll never see anyone I love again.”

The therapist helped Lena to begin to trace an emotional line between her fury at her abusive ex-girlfriend and her plan to leave her, her subsequent physical sensations of overwhelming anxiety, her sense of loss of executive control as highlighted by her relatively new inability to drive, and her central fantasy of her car as a tomb, separating her forever from the people she loved, especially from her mother. In carefully delving into the complicated, ambivalent, yet intense and dependent relationships she tended to form, the therapist explored an emerging core fantasy Lena had about herself that fueled much of the intensity of her relationships: that she was incompetent and unable to manage situations that might arise (on the train to appointments with her therapist, for example).

Lena relinquished her travel companion and began traveling and attending sessions alone by session 5. She rapidly resumed driving and no longer felt so isolated in her car or as though she would panic. The therapist continued to pursue Lena’s core fantasy that she was incompetent like a small child and terrified to be apart from her mother, as she had been when she was very young, and that therefore she was unable to handle matters that might arise at night if she were to sleep alone without her new girlfriend. After session 11, Lena slept alone for the first time in her life.

The therapist helped Lena to verbally articulate how her strength and newfound independence were associated with her relationship with the therapist, something she would have to relinquish soon because of the (24-session) study time limit. Lena actively mourned the loss of her therapist, experiencing “jumpy nerves” on the train when coming to see her, resisting a “pull” to “pick up women to make it better,” and later expressing anger and sadness that the therapy could not continue. She said that she had never said goodbye as she was now in parting with the therapist, permitting herself to feel the sadness of the loss without becoming overwhelmingly anxious and frantic to replace the therapist with new “emergency” relationships. Despite tremendous anticipatory anxiety, Lena felt calmer and more comfortable: traveling, working, and attending school without anxiety at termination. She had ended her relationship with the new girlfriend after session 17 and despite feeling “lonely and unusual,” had very uncharacteristically not rushed into a new relationship and was adopting a “wait and see” approach to dating. At termination, the Anxiety Disorders Interview Schedule for DSM-IV indicated a score of 3 out of 8 for panic disorder (subsyndromal), 3 out of 8 for agoraphobia, and a score of 0 for generalized anxiety disorder.

These two small adult studies demonstrate preliminary but promising outcomes of psychotherapies for patients with prominent separation anxiety symptoms amid different DSM disorders. Better tracking of separation anxiety throughout treatment course and the development of interventions to relieve its global effects might help in specifically targeting interventions for individual patients.

Separation Anxiety as a Research Model for Developing and Treating Anxiety Disorders

Neuroendocrine Markers

Close relationships can profoundly reassure patients with anxiety disorders and depression. Biological mechanisms and neural circuitry underlie this phenomenon. We note two interrelated systems that might serve as potential biomarkers of anxiety surrounding separation and attachment: respiratory sinus arrhythmia, a marker for parasympathetic nervous system activation, and oxytocin. Lower resting respiratory sinus arrhythmia levels are associated with impairment of adaptive reactivity to stressors (80, 81). Reductions have been noted across anxiety disorders (82), depression (83), and borderline personality disorder (84). Low respiratory sinus arrhythmia correlates with ineffective, inflexible coping responses and insecure attachment (85, 86). Important hypotheses to test are whether low respiratory sinus arrhythmia is associated with separation anxiety and whether improving separation anxiety increases respiratory sinus arrhythmia.

Oxytocin is a hypothalamic neuropeptide that across species suppresses endocrine and behavioral stress responses, relieves pain, and facilitates prosocial behaviors, including maternal behavior and affiliative contact seeking (87). For individuals with an anxious attachment style (88) or borderline personality disorder, intranasal oxytocin aggravates negative reactions to social stimuli (89). Resting plasma oxytocin levels bear a complex, still sketchily mapped relationship to interpersonal empathy, closeness, and trust (25, 90, 91) and to anxiety (25, 92), interpersonal difficulties, and romantic attachment anxiety and distress (93, 94). Higher plasma oxytocin levels are associated with greater anxiety and relationship dissatisfaction in separation anxiety disorder (95). Growing evidence supports peripheral oxytocin levels and the oxytocin receptor polymorphism rs53576 as potential biomarkers of social responsivity and capacity for attachment (25, 96).

Neuroimaging

Neuroimaging research has focused on brain regions and circuits whose activity appears abnormal at a single time point in individuals with anxiety and separation anxiety from close attachments. Biomarkers of underlying vulnerability related to attentional and memory systems that predispose to separation anxiety, but are not themselves anxiety circuitry in the usual sense, may merit exploration.

As social interaction is central to separation anxiety, we must identify neural circuitry involved in “separation-sensitive” social representations or schemas, schemas that predict danger when separation occurs. Extant evidence suggests that the temporal cortex and areas specifically important for social cognition may organize such heteromodal schemas (97). Affective evaluation may be lower than normal in individuals with avoidant attachment and greater than normal in those who are anxiously attached (22).

Conclusions

The apparent clinical centrality of separation anxiety and anxious attachment underscores the need to better understand their significance in empirically delineating the developmental path of anxiety. Separation anxiety must relate to emotion regulation circuits in human psychopathology, epitomized in the emotional processes of attachment and separation. The challenge is to move from clinical observations to a sophisticated understanding of risk, vulnerability, and symptom expression that might hone targeting of interventions.

The description of separation anxiety in this article has been necessarily schematic. Basic information is sparse even in key areas such as prevalence of separation anxiety among patients with mood and anxiety disorders and differential treatment response associated with separation anxiety. The field requires additional research to corroborate or disprove the preceding argument. Results, however, might prove exciting. Closely monitoring separation anxiety may uncover different mechanisms of vulnerability to anxiety and to anxiety that responds poorly to standard treatment interventions. For example, is exposure-based fear extinction more or less effective in individuals with separation anxiety? As a clinical marker for pathological dysregulation of the anxiolytic, stress-buffering effect of close relationships, separation anxiety and its treatment could provide an important window to neural circuits and other biological processes associated with internalization of social support (35, 98).

From the Department of Psychiatry, Weill Cornell Medical College, New York; New York State Psychiatric Institute, New York; and the Department of Psychiatry, University of Pittsburgh School of Medicine.
Address correspondence to Dr. Milrod ().

Dr. Milrod receives royalties from Taylor & Francis for an academic book. Dr. Markowitz receives research funding support from NIMH and salary support from New York State Psychiatric Institute; minor book royalties from American Psychiatric Publishing, Basic Books, and Oxford University Press; and an editorial stipend from Elsevier Press; none of these constitutes a conflict of interest with the current article. Dr. Altemus has received payment for consultation from Ironwood Pharmaceuticals and Corcept Pharmaceuticals; she has also received research support from Fisher-Wallace Corporation. The other authors report no financial relationships with commercial interests.

Supported in part by a fund in the New York Community Trust established by DeWitt Wallace (Dr. Milrod), by NIMH grants R01 MH70918-01A2 (Dr. Milrod) and R01 MH-079078 (Dr. Markowitz), by the Brain and Behavior Research Foundation (Dr. Milrod), and New York State Psychiatric Institute (Dr. Markowitz).

The authors thank biostatistician Eva Petkova, Ph.D.

References

1 Simpson HB: The RDoC project: a new paradigm for investigating the pathophysiology of anxiety. Depress Anxiety 2012; 29:251–252Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

2 Insel T, Cuthbert B, Garvey M, Heinssen R, Pine DS, Quinn K, Sanislow C, Wang P: Research Domain Criteria (RDoC): toward a new classification framework for research on mental disorders. Am J Psychiatry 2010; 167:748–751LinkGoogle Scholar

3 Freud S: Analysis of a phobia in a five-year-old boy (1909), in Complete Psychological Works, standard ed, vol 10. London, Hogarth Press, 1955Google Scholar

4 Bowlby J: A Secure Base: Parent-Child Attachment and Healthy Human Development. London, Routledge, 1988Google Scholar

5 Bowlby J: Attachment and Loss. New York, Basic Books, 1973Google Scholar

6 Manicavasagar V, Silove D, Curtis J: Separation anxiety in adulthood: a phenomenological investigation. Compr Psychiatry 1997; 38:274–282Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

7 Busch FN, Milrod BL, Singer MB, Aronson AC: Manual of Panic Focused Psychodynamic Psychotherapy: eXtended Range. New York, Routledge, 2011Google Scholar

8 Shear MK: Factors in the etiology and pathogenesis of panic disorder: revisiting the attachment-separation paradigm. Am J Psychiatry 1996; 153(July suppl):125–136LinkGoogle Scholar

9 Gittelman R, Klein DF: Relationship between separation anxiety and panic and agoraphobic disorders. Psychopathology 1984; 17(suppl 1):56–65Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

10 Kossowsky J, Pfaltz MC, Schneider S, Taeymans J, Locher C, Gaab J: The separation anxiety hypothesis of panic disorder revisited: a meta-analysis. Am J Psychiatry 2013; 170:768–781LinkGoogle Scholar

11 Lewinsohn PM, Holm-Denoma JM, Small JW, Seeley JR, Joiner TE: Separation anxiety disorder in childhood as a risk factor for future mental illness. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2008; 47:548–555CrossrefGoogle Scholar

12 Oler JA, Fox AS, Shelton SE, Rogers J, Dyer TD, Davidson RJ, Shelledy W, Oakes TR, Blangero J, Kalin NH: Amygdalar and hippocampal substrates of anxious temperament differ in their heritability. Nature 2010; 466:864–868Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

13 Paquette V, Lévesque J, Mensour B, Leroux JM, Beaudoin G, Bourgouin P, Beauregard M: “Change the mind and you change the brain”: effects of cognitive-behavioral therapy on the neural correlates of spider phobia. Neuroimage 2003; 18:401–409Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

14 DiNardo PA, Brown TA, Barlow DH: Anxiety Disorders Interview Schedule for DSM-IV: Lifetime Version (ADIS-IV-L). New York, Graywinds Publications, 1995Google Scholar

15 American Psychiatric Association: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual for Mental Disorders, 4th ed (DSM-IV). Washington, DC, APA, 1994Google Scholar

16 Shear K, Jin R, Ruscio AM, Walters EE, Kessler RC: Prevalence and correlates of estimated DSM-IV child and adult separation anxiety disorder in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Am J Psychiatry 2006; 163:1074–1083LinkGoogle Scholar

17 Silove DM, Marnane CL, Wagner R, Manicavasagar VL, Rees S: The prevalence and correlates of adult separation anxiety disorder in an anxiety clinic. BMC Psychiatry 2010; 10:21Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

18 Pini S, Abelli M, Shear KM, Cardini A, Lari L, Gesi C, Muti M, Calugi S, Galderisi S, Troisi A, Bertolino A, Cassano GB: Frequency and clinical correlates of adult separation anxiety in a sample of 508 outpatients with mood and anxiety disorders. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2010; 122:40–46Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

19 Ainsworth M, Blehar M, Waters E, Wall S: Patterns of Attachment. Hillsdale, NJ, Erlbaum, 1978Google Scholar

20 Manicavasagar V, Marnane C, Pini S, Abelli M, Rees S, Eapen V, Silove D: Adult separation anxiety disorder: a disorder comes of age. Curr Psychiatry Rep 2010; 12:290–297Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

21 Roberson-Nay R, Eaves LJ, Hettema JM, Kendler KS, Silberg JL: Childhood separation anxiety disorder and adult onset panic attacks share a common genetic diathesis. Depress Anxiety 2012; 29:320–327Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

22 Vrtička P, Vuilleumier P: Neuroscience of human social interactions and adult attachment style. Front Hum Neurosci 2012; 6:212Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

23 Soliman F, Glatt CE, Bath KG, Levita L, Jones RM, Pattwell SS, Jing D, Tottenham N, Amso D, Somerville LH, Voss HU, Glover G, Ballon DJ, Liston C, Teslovich T, Van Kempen T, Lee FS, Casey BJ: A genetic variant BDNF polymorphism alters extinction learning in both mouse and human. Science 2010; 327:863–866Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

24 Hartley CA, McKenna MC, Salman R, Holmes A, Casey BJ, Phelps EA, Glatt CE: Serotonin transporter polyadenylation polymorphism modulates the retention of fear extinction memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2012; 109:5493–5498Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

25 Costa B, Pini S, Gabelloni P, Abelli M, Lari L, Cardini A, Muti M, Gesi C, Landi S, Galderisi S, Mucci A, Lucacchini A, Cassano GB, Martini C: Oxytocin receptor polymorphisms and adult attachment style in patients with depression. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2009; 34:1506–1514Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

26 Walum H, Westberg L, Henningsson S, Neiderhiser JM, Reiss D, Igl W, Ganiban JM, Spotts EL, Pedersen NL, Eriksson E, Lichtenstein P: Genetic variation in the vasopressin receptor 1a gene (AVPR1A) associates with pair-bonding behavior in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2008; 105:14153–14156Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

27 Gillath O, Shaver PR, Baek JM, Chun DS: Genetic correlates of adult attachment style. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 2008; 34:1396–1405Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

28 Brunelli SA, Hofer MA: Selective breeding for infant rat separation-induced ultrasonic vocalizations: developmental precursors of passive and active coping styles. Behav Brain Res 2007; 182:193–207Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

29 Hofer MA: Multiple regulators of ultrasonic vocalization in the infant rat. Psychoneuroendocrinology 1996; 21:203–217Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

30 Coplan JD, Mathew SJ, Abdallah CG, Mao X, Kral JG, Smith EL, Rosenblum LA, Perera TD, Dwork AJ, Hof PR, Gorman JM, Shungu DC: Early-life stress and neurometabolites of the hippocampus. Brain Res 2010; 1358:191–199Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

31 Jackowski A, Perera TD, Abdallah CG, Garrido G, Tang CY, Martinez J, Mathew SJ, Gorman JM, Rosenblum LA, Smith EL, Dwork AJ, Shungu DC, Kaffman A, Gelernter J, Coplan JD, Kaufman J: Early-life stress, corpus callosum development, hippocampal volumetrics, and anxious behavior in male nonhuman primates. Psychiatry Res 2011; 192:37–44Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

32 Coplan JD, Abdallah CG, Kaufman J, Gelernter J, Smith EL, Perera TD, Dwork AJ, Kaffman A, Gorman JM, Rosenblum LA, Owens MJ, Nemeroff CB: Early-life stress, corticotropin-releasing factor, and serotonin transporter gene: a pilot study. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2011; 36:289–293Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

33 Diorio J, Meaney MJ: Maternal programming of defensive responses through sustained effects on gene expression. J Psychiatry Neurosci 2007; 32:275–284MedlineGoogle Scholar

34 Champagne FA, Weaver IC, Diorio J, Dymov S, Szyf M, Meaney MJ: Maternal care associated with methylation of the estrogen receptor-alpha1b promoter and estrogen receptor-alpha expression in the medial preoptic area of female offspring. Endocrinology 2006; 147:2909–2915Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

35 Cassano GB, Michelini S, Shear MK, Coli E, Maser JD, Frank E: The panic-agoraphobic spectrum: a descriptive approach to the assessment and treatment of subtle symptoms. Am J Psychiatry 1997; 154(June suppl):27–38LinkGoogle Scholar

36 Vanderwerker LC, Jacobs SC, Parkes CM, Prigerson HG: An exploration of associations between separation anxiety in childhood and complicated grief in later life. J Nerv Ment Dis 2006; 194:121–123Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

37 Cassano GB, Dell’Osso L, Frank E, Miniati M, Fagiolini A, Shear K, Pini S, Maser J: The bipolar spectrum: a clinical reality in search of diagnostic criteria and an assessment methodology. J Affect Disord 1999; 54:319–328Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

38 Dell’osso L, Carmassi C, Corsi M, Pergentini I, Socci C, Maremmani AG, Perugi G: Adult separation anxiety in patients with complicated grief versus healthy control subjects: relationships with lifetime depressive and hypomanic symptoms. Ann Gen Psychiatry 2011; 10:29Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

39 Feske U, Frank E, Mallinger AG, Houck PR, Fagiolini A, Shear MK, Grochocinski VJ, Kupfer DJ: Anxiety as a correlate of response to the acute treatment of bipolar I disorder. Am J Psychiatry 2000; 157:956–962LinkGoogle Scholar

40 Frank E, Cyranowski JM, Rucci P, Shear MK, Fagiolini A, Thase ME, Cassano GB, Grochocinski VJ, Kostelnik B, Kupfer DJ: Clinical significance of lifetime panic spectrum symptoms in the treatment of patients with bipolar I disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2002; 59:905–911Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

41 Fagiolini A, Frank E, Rucci P, Cassano GB, Turkin S, Kupfer DJ: Mood and anxiety spectrum as a means to identify clinically relevant subtypes of bipolar I disorder. Bipolar Disord 2007; 9:462–467Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

42 Perugi G, Akiskal HS: The soft bipolar spectrum redefined: focus on the cyclothymic, anxious-sensitive, impulse-dyscontrol, and binge-eating connection in bipolar II and related conditions. Psychiatr Clin North Am 2002; 25:713–737Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

43 Toni C, Perugi G, Frare F, Tusini G, Fountoulakis KN, Akiskal KK, Akiskal HS: The clinical-familial correlates and naturalistic outcome of panic-disorder-agoraphobia with and without lifetime bipolar II comorbidity. Ann Gen Psychiatry 2008; 7:23Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

44 Perugi G, Toni C, Maremmani I, Tusini G, Ramacciotti S, Madia A, Fornaro M, Akiskal HS: The influence of affective temperaments and psychopathological traits on the definition of bipolar disorder subtypes: a study on bipolar I Italian national sample. J Affect Disord 2012; 136:e41–e49Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

45 Silove D, Marnane C, Wagner R, Manicavasagar V: Brief report—associations of personality disorder with early separation anxiety in patients with adult separation anxiety disorder. J Pers Disord 2011; 25:128–133Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

46 Benvenuti A, Rucci P, Calugi S, Cassano GB, Miniati M, Frank E: Relationship of residual mood and panic-agoraphobic spectrum phenomenology to quality of life and functional impairment in patients with major depression. Int Clin Psychopharmacol 2010; 25:68–74Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

47 Frank E, Shear MK, Rucci P, Cyranowski JM, Endicott J, Fagiolini A, Grochocinski VJ, Houck P, Kupfer DJ, Maser JD, Cassano GB: Influence of panic-agoraphobic spectrum symptoms on treatment response in patients with recurrent major depression. Am J Psychiatry 2000; 157:1101–1107LinkGoogle Scholar

48 Miniati M, Calugi S, Rucci P, Shear MK, Benvenuti A, Santoro D, Mauri M, Cassano GB: Predictors of response among patients with panic disorder treated with medications in a naturalistic follow-up: the role of adult separation anxiety. J Affect Disord 2012; 136:675–679Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

49 Coryell W, Fiedorowicz JG, Solomon D, Leon AC, Rice JP, Keller MB: Effects of anxiety on the long-term course of depressive disorders. Br J Psychiatry 2012; 200:210–215Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

50 Weissman MM, Markowitz JC, Klerman GL: Clinicians’ Quick Guide to Interpersonal Psychotherapy. New York, Oxford University Press, 2007CrossrefGoogle Scholar

51 Aaronson CJ, Shear MK, Goetz RR, Allen LB, Barlow DH, White KS, Ray S, Money R, Saksa JR, Woods SW, Gorman JM: Predictors and time course of response among panic disorder patients treated with cognitive-behavioral therapy. J Clin Psychiatry 2008; 69:418–424Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

52 Last CG, Hersen M, Kazdin A, Orvaschel H, Perrin S: Anxiety disorders in children and their families. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1991; 48:928–934Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

53 Rapee RM, Kennedy SJ, Ingram M, Edwards SL, Sweeney L: Altering the trajectory of anxiety in at-risk young children. Am J Psychiatry 2010; 167:1518–1525LinkGoogle Scholar

54 Warren SL, Huston L, Egeland B, Sroufe LA: Child and adolescent anxiety disorders and early attachment. J Am Acad Child Adol Psychiatry 1997; 36:637–644Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

55 Hudson JL, Rapee RM: Parent-child interactions in clinically anxious children and their siblings. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol 2002; 31:548–555Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

56 Rosenbaum JF, Biederman J, Gersten M, Hirshfeld DR, Meminger SR, Herman JB, Kagan J, Reznick JS, Snidman N: Behavioral inhibition in children of parents with panic disorder and agoraphobia: a controlled study. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1988; 45:463–470Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

57 Last CG, Hersen M, Kazdin AE, Francis G, Grubb HJ: Psychiatric illness in the mothers of anxious children. Am J Psychiatry 1987; 144:1580–1583LinkGoogle Scholar

58 Herren C, In-Albon T, Schneider S: Beliefs regarding child anxiety and parenting competence in parents of children with separation anxiety disorder. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry 2013; 44:53–60Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

59 Milrod B, Bush F, Shapiro T: Psychodynamic Approaches to the Adolescent with Panic Disorder. Malabar, Fla, Krieger, 2004Google Scholar

60 Benoit D, Parker KC: Stability and transmission of attachment across three generations. Child Dev 1994; 65:1444–1456Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

61 Mahler MS, Pine F, Bergman A: The Psychological Birth of the Human Infant Symbiosis and Individuation. New York, Basic Books, 1975Google Scholar

62 Groh AM, Roisman GI, van Ijzendoorn MH, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Fearon RP: The significance of insecure and disorganized attachment for children’s internalizing symptoms: a meta-analytic study. Child Dev 2012; 83:591–610Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

63 Fonagy P, Steele M, Steele H, Leigh T, Kennedy R, Mattoon G, Target M: Attachment, the reflective self, and borderline states: the predictive specificity of the Adult Attachment Interview and pathological emotional development, in Attachment Theory: Social, Developmental, and Clinical Perspectives. Edited by Goldberg SMuir RKerr J. Hillsdale, NJ, Analytic Press, 1995, pp 233–278Google Scholar

64 Baram TZ, Davis EP, Obenaus A, Sandman CA, Small SL: Fragmentation and unpredictability of early-life experience in mental disorders. Am J Psychiatry 2012; 169:907–915LinkGoogle Scholar

65 Fonagy P: The human genome and the representational world: the role of early mother-infant interaction in creating an interpersonal interpretive mechanism. Bull Menninger Clin 2001; 65:427–448Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

66 Laor N, Wolmer L, Mayes LC, Golomb A, Silverberg DS, Weizman R, Cohen DJ: Israeli preschoolers under Scud missile attacks: a developmental perspective on risk-modifying factors. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1996; 53:416–423Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

67 Markowitz JC, Milrod B, Bleiberg KL, Marshall RD: Interpersonal factors in understanding and treating posttraumatic stress disorder. J Psychiatr Pract 2009; 15:133–140Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

68 Saxe GN, Stoddard F, Hall E, Chawla N, Lopez C, Sheridan R, King D, King L, Yehuda R: Pathways to PTSD, part I: children with burns. Am J Psychiatry 2005; 162:1299–1304LinkGoogle Scholar

69 Markowitz JC, Milrod BL: The importance of responding to negative affect in psychotherapies. Am J Psychiatry 2011; 168:124–128LinkGoogle Scholar

70 Stefini A, Horn H, Winkelmann K, Geiser-Elze A, Hartmann M, Kronmüller KT: Attachment styles and outcome of psychoanalytic psychotherapy for children and adolescents. Psychopathology 2013; 46:192–200Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

71 Levy KN, Ellison WD, Scott LN, Bernecker SL: Attachment style. J Clin Psychol 2011; 67:193–203Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

72 Torres B, Alonso-Arbiol I, Cantero MJ, Abubakar A: Infant-mother attachment can be improved through group intervention: a preliminary evaluation in Spain in a non-randomized controlled trial. Span J Psychol 2011; 14:630–638Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

73 Ramsauer B, Gehrke J, Lotzin A, Powell B, Romer G: [Attachment and attachment-based intervention: the Circle of Security intervention project in Hamburg]. Prax Kinderpsychol Kinderpsychiatr 2011; 60:417–429Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

74 Levy KN, Meehan KB, Kelly KM, Reynoso JS, Weber M, Clarkin JF, Kernberg OF: Change in attachment patterns and reflective function in a randomized control trial of transference-focused psychotherapy for borderline personality disorder. J Consult Clin Psychol 2006; 74:1027–1040Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

75 Rudden M, Milrod B, Target M, Ackerman S, Graf E: Reflective functioning in panic disorder patients: a pilot study. J Am Psychoanal Assoc 2006; 54:1339–1343Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

76 Fonagy P, Leigh T, Steele M, Steele H, Kennedy R, Mattoon G, Target M, Gerber A: The relation of attachment status, psychiatric classification, and response to psychotherapy. J Consult Clin Psychol 1996; 64:22–31Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

77 Cyranowski JM, Frank E, Shear MK, Swartz H, Fagiolini A, Scott J, Kupfer DJ: Interpersonal psychotherapy for depression with panic spectrum symptoms: a pilot study. Depress Anxiety 2005; 21:140–142Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

78 Milrod B, Leon AC, Busch F, Rudden M, Schwalberg M, Clarkin J, Aronson A, Singer M, Turchin W, Klass ET, Graf E, Teres JJ, Shear MK: A randomized controlled clinical trial of psychoanalytic psychotherapy for panic disorder. Am J Psychiatry 2007; 164:265–272LinkGoogle Scholar

79 Siev J, Chambless DL: Specificity of treatment effects: cognitive therapy and relaxation for generalized anxiety and panic disorders. J Consult Clin Psychol 2007; 75:513–522Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

80 Bornstein MH, Suess PE: Child and mother cardiac vagal tone: continuity, stability, and concordance across the first 5 years. Dev Psychol 2000; 36:54–65Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

81 Sack M, Hopper JW, Lamprecht F: Low respiratory sinus arrhythmia and prolonged psychophysiological arousal in posttraumatic stress disorder: heart rate dynamics and individual differences in arousal regulation. Biol Psychiatry 2004; 55:284–290Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

82 Friedman BH: An autonomic flexibility-neurovisceral integration model of anxiety and cardiac vagal tone. Biol Psychol 2007; 74:185–199Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

83 Rottenberg J: Cardiac vagal control in depression: a critical analysis. Biol Psychol 2007; 74:200–211Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

84 Weinberg A, Klonsky ED, Hajcak G: Autonomic impairment in borderline personality disorder: a laboratory investigation. Brain Cogn 2009; 71:279–286Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

85 Fabes RA, Eisenberg N: Regulatory control and adults’ stress-related responses to daily life events. J Pers Soc Psychol 1997; 73:1107–1117Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

86 Friedman BH, Thayer JF: Autonomic balance revisited: panic anxiety and heart rate variability. J Psychosom Res 1998; 44:133–151Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

87 Insel TR: The challenge of translation in social neuroscience: a review of oxytocin, vasopressin, and affiliative behavior. Neuron 2010; 65:768–779Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

88 Bartz JA, Zaki J, Ochsner KN, Bolger N, Kolevzon A, Ludwig N, Lydon JE: Effects of oxytocin on recollections of maternal care and closeness. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2010; 107:21371–21375Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

89 Bartz J, Simeon D, Hamilton H, Kim S, Crystal S, Braun A, Vicens V, Hollander E: Oxytocin can hinder trust and cooperation in borderline personality disorder. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2011; 6:556–563Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

90 Bartz JA, Zaki J, Bolger N, Hollander E, Ludwig NN, Kolevzon A, Ochsner KN: Oxytocin selectively improves empathic accuracy. Psychol Sci 2010; 21:1426–1428Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

91 Feldman R, Weller A, Zagoory-Sharon O, Levine A: Evidence for a neuroendocrinological foundation of human affiliation: plasma oxytocin levels across pregnancy and the postpartum period predict mother-infant bonding. Psychol Sci 2007; 18:965–970Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

92 Gordon I, Zagoory-Sharon O, Leckman JF, Feldman R: Prolactin, oxytocin, and the development of paternal behavior across the first six months of fatherhood. Horm Behav 2010; 58:513–518Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

93 Marazziti D, Dell’Osso B, Baroni S, Mungai F, Catena M, Rucci P, Albanese F, Giannaccini G, Betti L, Fabbrini L, Italiani P, Del Debbio A, Lucacchini A, Dell’Osso L: A relationship between oxytocin and anxiety of romantic attachment. Clin Pract Epidemol Ment Health 2006; 2:28Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

94 Declerck CH, Boone C, Kiyonari T: Oxytocin and cooperation under conditions of uncertainty: the modulating role of incentives and social information. Horm Behav 2010; 57:368–374Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

95 Hoge EA, Pollack MH, Kaufman RE, Zak PJ, Simon NM: Oxytocin levels in social anxiety disorder. CNS Neurosci Ther 2008; 14:165–170Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

96 Meinlschmidt G, Heim C: Sensitivity to intranasal oxytocin in adult men with early parental separation. Biol Psychiatry 2007; 61:1109–1111Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

97 Zahn R, Moll J, Krueger F, Huey ED, Garrido G, Grafman J: Social concepts are represented in the superior anterior temporal cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2007; 104:6430–6435Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

98 Preter M, Lee SH, Petkova E, Vannucci M, Kim S, Klein DF: Controlled cross-over study in normal subjects of naloxone-preceding-lactate infusions; respiratory and subjective responses: relationship to endogenous opioid system, suffocation false alarm theory and childhood parental loss. Psychol Med 2011; 41:385–393Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar