The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×
This article has been corrected | View Correction
New ResearchFull Access

Familial Transmission and Heritability of Childhood Disruptive Disorders

Abstract

Objective:

There is substantial evidence of a link between parental substance use disorders and antisocial behavior and childhood disruptive disorders in offspring, but it is unclear whether this transmission is specific to particular disorders or if a general liability accounts for familial resemblance. The authors examined whether the association between parental externalizing disorders and childhood disruptive disorders in preadolescent offspring is a result of the transmission of general or disorder-specific liabilities and estimated the genetic and environmental contributions to variation in these general and specific liability indicators.

Method:

Participants were 1,069 families consisting of 11-year-old twins and their biological mother and father. Structural equation modeling was used to simultaneously estimate the general and specific transmission effects of four parental externalizing disorders (conduct disorder, adult antisocial behavior, alcohol dependence, and drug dependence) on childhood disruptive disorders (attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, conduct disorder, and oppositional defiant disorder).

Results:

Parent-child resemblance was accounted for by the transmission of a general liability to externalizing disorders, and this general liability was highly heritable. Specific effects were also detected, but for sibling rather than parental transmission. Specific genetic and nonshared environmental effects were detected for each childhood disruptive disorder, but only conduct disorder exhibited a significant shared environmental effect.

Conclusions:

A highly heritable general liability accounts for the parent-child transmission of externalizing psychopathology from parents to their preadolescent offspring. This general liability should be a focus of research for both etiology and intervention.

Numerous studies have demonstrated that parental substance use disorders and antisocial behavior are associated with elevated levels of childhood disruptive disorders in offspring. For example, investigations using family, twin, and offspring-of-twin designs have reported that a history of parental alcohol dependence is associated with increased rates of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) (1), conduct disorder (2, 3), and oppositional defiant disorder (3). A number of other studies have also established a link between offspring conduct disorder and parental antisocial behavior and illicit drug abuse (46).

Given the number of parent-child effects, it is reasonable to ask the following question: Are there specific associations between parental substance use disorders and antisocial behavior and childhood disruptive disorders, or can a transmission of some type of general liability account for parent-child resemblance? For example, some well-controlled family and adoption studies have reported specific transmission effects (2, 4, 7, 8). On the other hand, a growing body of literature supports the notion of a general transmission effect (4, 911).

A conceptual model that can help account for this general transmission effect derives from examining the comorbidity among substance use disorders and antisocial behavior. Specifically, twin studies have demonstrated that the comorbidity among substance use disorders and antisocial behavior can best be accounted for by a highly heritable (approximately 0.80) general liability dimension, typically referred to as the latent externalizing factor 1218. A similar general factor (albeit somewhat more influenced by shared environmental factors) seems to underlie the comorbidity among childhood disruptive disorders (ADHD, conduct disorder, and oppositional defiant disorder) 1921. For instance, recent data suggest that symptoms of hyperactivity-impulsivity and oppositionality represent the same underlying liability dimension (22). It may be that externalizing is a latent, nonspecific liability that accounts for the general transmission effects between parents and children.

One method of disentangling the general and specific effects entails fitting a model that estimates general and specific transmission effects simultaneously. Hicks et al. (13) tested such a model by utilizing a twin-family design to simultaneously estimate the general and specific transmission effects from parents onto 17-year-old offspring for conduct disorder, adult antisocial behavior (the adult criteria for antisocial personality disorder), alcohol dependence, and drug dependence. Results demonstrated that the transmission of a general externalizing factor liability could account for the parent-child resemblance for the four externalizing disorders and that this general liability was highly heritable. Additionally, Hicks et al. found evidence for specific transmission for conduct disorder, alcohol dependence, and drug dependence, but across siblings rather than from parents to offspring (i.e., horizontal as opposed to vertical transmission). As such, this study provided a model for examining familial transmission in terms of delineating the relative contributions of general and specific effects.

In the present study, we sought to replicate and extend the findings by Hicks et al. by employing a similar family transmission model to clarify the association between substance use disorders and antisocial behavior in parents and childhood disruptive disorders in preadolescent offspring. Specifically, we aimed to determine whether the transmission of the general externalizing factor liability alone could account for parent-child resemblance while also estimating the genetic and environmental contributions to variability in the general and specific transmission effects. We again employed a twin-family design such that each family included both biological parents and a twin pair. However, in contrast to the study conducted by Hicks et al., the twins were from a different cohort and approximately 11 years of age. Moreover, we focused on transmission to childhood disruptive disorders, namely, ADHD, oppositional defiant disorder, and conduct disorder, externalizing disorders that were developmentally appropriate to assess at this age. Additionally, a multi-informant approach (child, mother, and teacher) was used to assess the childhood disruptive disorders. Finally, the sample size for the current investigation included >1,000 families (roughly twice the size employed by Hicks et al.), providing excellent power to detect effects. In line with the findings by Hicks et al., we hypothesized that most of the parent-child resemblance would be mainly accounted for by the transmission of a highly heritable general liability to externalizing psychopathology.

Method

For a full description of the sample recruitment, demographic characteristics, measures, and statistical analysis plan, see the data supplement accompanying the online version of this article. All parents provided written informed consent for themselves and their offspring, and all twins gave written informed assent. The study protocols were reviewed and approved by an internal review board.

Sample

Participants were members of 1,069 families (N=4,276) from the Minnesota Twin Family Study. There were 685 monozygotic twin pairs (male: 52%) and 384 dizygotic twin pairs (male: 49%). The mean age for the twins was 11.8 years (SD=0.4), for the mothers it was 40.2 years (SD=4.9), and for the fathers it was 42.6 years (SD=5.4). Ninety-six percent of the families were Caucasian.

Measures

Parent and child externalizing disorders were assessed via the Substance Abuse Module of the Composite International Diagnostic Interview (23), a slightly modified version of the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R-II that includes additional probes to assess the frequency of symptoms for conduct disorder and antisocial personality disorder (24), and the Diagnostic Interview for Children and Adolescents–Revised (25), respectively. We assessed parental symptoms of lifetime conduct disorder, adult antisocial behavior, alcohol dependence, and drug dependence. We also assessed symptoms of offspring ADHD, conduct disorder, and oppositional defiant disorder using the Diagnostic Interview for Children and Adolescents–Revised. For twins, a symptom was considered present if it was reported by either the mother or the twin. Finally, teacher ratings of ADHD, conduct disorder, and oppositional defiant disorder were collected for up to three teachers per twin. The mean of the teacher ratings was used for the twin's teacher rating variable, and a composite of the mother, child, and teacher ratings was made for the indices of childhood disruptive disorders by taking a mean of the three reports.

Statistical Analysis

We used structural equation modeling to investigate the link between adult externalizing disorders in parents and childhood disruptive disorders in the preadolescent offspring of these parents as well as the generality versus specificity of transmission effects. The general liability to externalizing disorders was conceptualized as a latent externalizing factor phenotype (12, 13, 15, 19, 26). For parents, externalizing factor was defined by alcohol dependence, drug dependence, conduct disorder, and adult antisocial behavior and referred to as the externalizing P (parent) factor. For the twins, externalizing factor was defined by ADHD, conduct disorder, and oppositional defiant disorder and referred to as the externalizing O (offspring) factor. Symptom counts were used for all disorders. General transmission effects were operationalized as the latent correlations between the externalizing P and O phenotypes. We estimated the specific transmission effects of a given disorder by allowing the residual variance (i.e., variance unaccounted for by externalizing factors) of the parental symptom count variables to covary with the residuals of the twins' symptom count.

We also used standard biometric models to examine the influence of additive genetic, shared environmental, and nonshared environmental influences (the ACE model) on the externalizing O factor as well as on each childhood disruptive disorder. The additive genetic component (a2) refers to the effect of individual genes summed over loci on trait variance. Genetic influences are inferred if the monozygotic correlation is greater than the dizygotic correlation for a given trait. Shared environmental (c2) effects refer to environmental influences that increase similarity between members of a twin pair. Shared environmental effects are inferred if the dizygotic correlation is more than one-half the monozygotic correlation. Nonshared environmental (e2) effects refer to environmental factors that contribute to differences between members of a twin pair. Measurement error is also included in the estimate of e2. Additionally, if the monozygotic correlation is more than one-half the dizygotic correlation, it is common practice to investigate nonadditive genetic (dominance [d2]) models as an alternative to ACE models. Because c2 and d2 are estimated using the same information, the two parameters cannot be estimated simultaneously. Biometric analyses were conducted using the Mx computer program (27).

Model fitting for family transmission effects was conducted in Mplus 5.0 (28) using a robust maximum-likelihood estimator, which is appropriate when analyzing non-normal variables. The non-normality was the result of positive skew in the symptom count variables, which is typical for population-based samples. The fit of models was evaluated using the mean-adjusted (Satorra-Bentler) chi-square fit statistic (29), the Bayesian information criterion (χ2–df [ln N]) (30), and the root mean square error of approximation (31). The mean adjusted chi-square provides an overall estimate of model fit for non-normal data. For nested models, the difference in chi-square can be used to determine whether additional parameters significantly improve the model fit. Because we conducted 27 tests of specific transmission, we used a Bonferroni-corrected alpha of 0.0019 (0.05/27) when comparing Δχ2 for nested models. The Bayesian information criterion is a function of a model's chi-square value and degrees of freedom and penalizes the model fit for the retention of unnecessary parameters. This fit index is not interpreted in isolation; rather, it is used to compare alternative models such that lower Bayesian information criterion scores are indicative of a better fit. When comparing models, a difference in Bayesian information criterion of 0–2 is considered weak evidence in support of the model with the lower criterion value, a difference of 2–6 is considered positive evidence, a difference of 6–10 is considered strong evidence, and a difference >10 is considered very strong evidence (32). The root mean square error of approximation provides an estimate of discrepancy in the model fit per degree of freedom, with values of 0.08 indicating a good fit and ≤0.05 indicating a very good fit to the data (31).

Results

General Versus Specific Transmission of Childhood Disruptive Disorders

Results of the model fitting are provided in Table 1. An initial model that allowed for only general transmission of externalizing disorders from parents to offspring provided an adequate fit to the data (χ2=699.96, df=189, Bayesian information criterion=−618.22, root mean square error of approximation=0.071). This model served as the comparison model for all subsequent analyses.

TABLE 1. Indexes of Fit for Alternate Models for Family Transmission of Disruptive Disorders in 11-Year-Old Twin Offspringa

ModelΧ2dfRoot Mean Square Error of ApproximationBayesian Information CriterionΔΧ2df
General transmission only699.961890.071−618.22
Specific transmission effects of paternal externalizing disorders on offspring disruptive disorders
Mother conduct disorder
    ADHD691.141880.071−620.068.821
    Conduct disorder699.661880.071−611.550.301
    Oppositional defiant disorder695.051880.071−616.164.911
Mother adult antisocial behavior
    ADHD692.311880.071−618.897.651
    Conduct disorder698.481880.071−6 12.721.481
    Oppositional-defiant disorder699.231880.071−611.970.731
Mother alcohol dependence
    ADHD698.761880.071−612.441.201
    Conduct disorder700.711880.071−610.490.751
    Oppositional defiant disorder699.631880.071−611.580.331
Mother drug dependence
    ADHD698.111880.071−613.101.851
    Conduct disorder698.761880.071−612.441.201
    Oppositional defiant disorder698.851880.071−612.361.111
Father conduct disorder
    ADHD698.371880.071−612.831.591
    Conduct disorder699.171880.071−612.030.791
    Oppositional defiant disorder698.531880.071−612.681.431
Father adult antisocial behavior
    ADHD699.661880.071−611.540.301
    Conduct disorder695.351880.071−615.854.611
    Oppositional defiant disorder698.011880.071−613.191.951
Father alcohol dependence
    ADHD697.161880.071−614.042.801
    Conduct disorder696.151880.071−615.053.811
    Oppositional defiant disorder698.081880.071−613.121.881
Father drug dependence
    ADHD699.231880.071−611.970.731
    Conduct disorder699.051880.071−612.150.911
    Oppositional defiant disorder698.801880.071−612.401.161
Cross-twin (siblings)
    ADHD493.671870.055−810.35206.29b2
    Conduct disorder528.711870.058−775.52171.25b2
    Oppositional defiant disorder560.991870.061−743.24138.97b2
Best fitting model
General transmission from parents to offspring with cross-twin specific effects for ADHD, conduct disorder, and oppositional defiant disorder230.971830.022−1045.36468.99b6
    Equate maternal and paternal general transmission231.551840.022−1051.76468.41b5

aAll chi-square change values reflect the difference in chi-square relative to the first (general transmission only) model. A Bonferroni-corrected alpha of 0.0019 (0.05 divided by 27 tests of specific transmission) was used as the threshold for a significant Δχ2. None of the specific parent-to-child effects exceeded this threshold (the largest effect had a p value of 0.0029 and accounted for only 0.56% of the variance in the childhood disorder). Paternal externalizing factors analyzed were conduct disorder, adult antisocial behavior, alcohol dependence, and drug dependence. Childhood/offspring disruptive disorders analyzed were ADHD, conduct disorder, and oppositional defiant disorder. Abbreviations: ADHD=attention deficit hyperactivity disorder.

bChi-square change significantly different at pa0.001.

TABLE 1. Indexes of Fit for Alternate Models for Family Transmission of Disruptive Disorders in 11-Year-Old Twin Offspringa

Enlarge table

To test for disorder-specific effects, we allowed the residual variances of parental disorders to covary with the residual variances of offspring disorders (e.g., mother conduct disorder and twin conduct disorder, father alcohol dependence and twin ADHD). If the model fit improved, this indicated that parents and offspring were more similar than would be expected given only general transmission. To maximize power to detect specific effects, we conducted one degree of freedom test separately for each paternal and maternal disorder (Table 1), which is differentiated from the general transmission only model by a reduction of one degree of freedom and subsequent changes in model fit indexes. We conducted 24 tests to determine whether a specific effect model supporting parent-offspring transmission fit better than the general transmission model. None of the specific transmission effects resulted in a significant reduction in chi-square (Bonferroni corrected-alpha <0.0019), a drop in Bayesian information criterion of at least two (rather, almost all specific effects resulted in higher Bayesian information criterion values), or a lower root mean square error of approximation value. Therefore, the results show that a general transmission only model can account for familial resemblance in adult externalizing disorders in parents and childhood disruptive disorders in their offspring.

Next, we examined specific cross-twin (sibling) effects for ADHD, conduct disorder, and oppositional defiant disorder by allowing the residual variance of each disorder to correlate across members of the twin pair. We allowed the paths to vary by zygosity, and therefore there were two degree of freedom tests for each disorder. Cross-twin specific effects were detected for each childhood disorder (Table 1), indicating familial liability factors to these disorders that are independent of the general liability but that do not stem from parental externalizing disorders.

Maternal and paternal general transmission effects were estimated as the latent correlation between externalizing P and O factors. The latent correlations between the mother and father P factors and the O factor were r=0.26 (95% confidence interval [CI]=0.13−0.38) and r=0.20 (95% CI=0.12–0.29), respectively. As such, in the next model, we equated the effects of the externalizing P factor on the externalizing O factor. Equating the parental effects yielded a lower Bayesian information criterion value, indicating that the strength of maternal and paternal transmission was not significantly different. The resulting latent correlation between the externalizing P and O factors (r=0.23; 95% CI=0.14–0.31) was moderate and represents the effect of the general transmission of parental externalizing disorders on offspring childhood disruptive disorders (Figure 1).

FIGURE 1.

FIGURE 1. Final Model for General and Specific Transmission Effects of Maternal and Paternal Externalizing Factors on Preadolescent Monozygotic and Dizygotic Twin Offspringaa

aAll path coefficients are standardized. All paths shown are significant (p<0.01). The latent externalizing variables represent the general liability factors common among the four diagnoses for parents and three disorders for offspring. The circular blank latent variables associated with each disorder are the residual variances and represent liability factors that are specific to the disorder or unaccounted for by the general externalizing liability factor. Double-headed arrows linking maternal and paternal externalizing factors to twin A and twin B externalizing factors represent the general transmission effect for the four disorders. All parent-to-offspring effects were constrained to be equal, since model fitting results indicated that there was not a statistically significant difference in the strength of maternal and paternal transmission. The double-headed arrow that links twin A and twin B externalizing factor indexes monozygotic/dizygotic twin similarity for the general liability to the three disorders. The double-headed arrows linking the residual variances of ADHD, conduct disorder, and oppositional defiant disorder across members of the twin pair represent disorder-specific liabilities that increase sibling similarity but that are independent of the general externalizing factor liability. Abbreviations: AAB=adult antisocial behavior; CD=conduct disorder; ALD=alcohol dependence; DD=drug dependence; ADHD=attention deficit hyperactive disorder; ODD=oppositional defiant disorder; EXT-P=paternal externalizing factor; EXT-O=offspring externalizing factor.

To ensure that the parental transmission effects did not differ by the gender of the offspring, we conducted the same tests of specific and general transmission effects (i.e., following the aforementioned procedure and models) separately for male and female twins. The general transmission effects could be equated for male and female twins without a loss in model fit. Moreover, we did not detect any specific transmission from parents to offspring for either male or female twins. To ascertain that these results did not differ by informant, we fit separate models using parent, child, and teacher reports for offspring disorders. The results were consistent with those shown in Figure 1.

Genetic and Environmental Contributions to Variation in Externalizing Factors and Disorder-Specific Liabilities

In testing the magnitude of genetic influences on cross-twin effects, we first examined the latent correlation between the mother and father externalizing P factors. The correlation between parents sometimes increases the similarity of dizygotic twins in twin studies, thereby decreasing the study heritability (33). There was a significant correlation between the maternal and paternal externalizing factors (r=0.45; 95% CI=0.31–0.60). Despite this finding, the cross-twin correlations for the externalizing O factor differed strongly across zygosity, indicating a strong genetic influence. However, the dizygotic correlation was greater than one-half the monozygotic correlation, suggesting shared environmental effects on the general externalizing O factor. For the disorder-specific effects (Figure 1), the monozygotic correlations for ADHD and oppositional defiant disorder were larger than the dizygotic twin correlations, indicting specific genetic effects. In contrast, the monozygotic and dizygotic correlations were quite similar for conduct disorder, indicating a shared environmental effect.

Next, we fit biometric models to decompose the variance of each disorder into its additive genetic (a2), shared environmental (c2), and nonshared environmental (e2) factors (Table 2). Genetic effects were large for ADHD and oppositional defiant disorder and moderate for conduct disorder. Notably, because the monozygotic correlation for ADHD was more than one-half the dizygotic correlation, we also investigated a nonadditive genetic model, with dominance (d2) replacing the c2 effect. This model did not fit significantly better than the ACE model (change in Bayesian information criterion was only –1.62), and dominance was not significant (d2=0.27; 95% CI=0.00–0.60). Thus, an ACE model was fit for each disorder. Only conduct disorder evidenced a notable shared environmental component. Small nonshared environmental effects were detected for each disorder. Finally, we examined the generality and specificity of the genetic and environmental effects on the three childhood disruptive disorders by fitting a biometric factor model (Figure 2). The general externalizing O factor was primarily influenced by genetic factors (a2=0.65; 95% CI=0.51–0.81) but also exhibited a notable shared environmental component (c2=0.23; 95% CI=0.07–0.36), with the remaining variance as a result of nonshared environmental effects (e2=0.12; 95% CI=0.10–0.15). This indicates largely common genetic factors underlying the comorbidity among the childhood disruptive disorders. We also detected several specific ACE effects on the individual disorders. For each disorder, we detected specific genetic effects. These effects were large for ADHD (a2=0.36; 95% CI=0.31–0.41), moderate for oppositional defiant disorder (a2=0.16; 95% CI=0.10–0.19), and modest for conduct disorder (a2=0.10; 95% CI=0.03–0.18). Only conduct disorder exhibited a strong shared environmental effect (c2=0.12; 95% CI=0.05–0.18). Last, all three disorders exhibited moderate specific nonshared environmental effects (ADHD: e2=0.20; 95% CI=0.18–0.23; oppositional defiant disorder: e2=0.14; 95% CI=0.12–0.15; conduct disorder: e2=0.11; 95% CI=0.09–0.12).

FIGURE 2.

FIGURE 2. ACE Model Analysis of Childhood Disruptive Disorders Among Preadolescent Monozygotic and Dizygotic Twin Offrspingaa

a Path coefficients and factor loadings are standardized, and 95% confidence intervals (CIs) are shown in parentheses beneath each coefficient. All coefficients whose CI does not include zero are significant. The percentage of variance accounted for by a given variable in another variable can be determined by squaring the path coefficient on the path connecting the first variable with the second variable. The sum of all the squared loadings (effects from the general externalizing factor as well as the specific ACE loadings) equals 1.The total effect can be calculated by summing the general effect (squared factor loading for a given disorder multiplied by the squared A, C, or E path coefficient on the externalizing factor) and the specific effect (squared A, C, or E specific effect on a given disorder). Using conduct disorder as an example, h2=general [(0.81)2×(0.82)2;specific [(0.31)2]=0.54. Abbreviations: A=additive genetic effects; C=shared environmental effects; E=nonshared environmental effects; ADHD=attention deficit hyperactivity disorder; CD=conduct disorder; ODD=oppositional defiant disorder; EXT-O=offspring externalizing factor.

TABLE 2. Correlations and Univariate Parameter Estimates for Additive, Shared Environmental, and Nonshared Environmental Effects Among 11-Year-Old Twin Offspring

VariableMonozygotic Twin Pairs (N=685)Dizygotic Twin Pairs (N=384)Additive Genetic Effects (A)
Shared Environmental Effects (C)
Nonshared Environmental Effects (E)
r95% Confidence Intervalr95% Confidence Intervalr95% Confidence Interval
ADHD
    Boys0.740.300.740.62–0.780.000.00–0.110.260.22–0.31
    Girls0.710.260.730.65–0.770.000.00–0.060.270.23–0.32
    Total0.720.280.730.68–0.760.000.00–0.050.270.24–0.30
Conduct disorder
    Boys0.840.560.520.37–0.700.320.13–0.460.160.14–0.20
    Girls0.720.480.500.33–0.700.220.03–0.380.280.24–0.33
    Total0.810.540.510.39–0.630.300.18–0.410.190.17–0.22
Oppositional defiant disorder
    Boys0.780.390.780.58–0.810.000.00–0.190.220.19–0.26
    Girls0.740.430.650.46–0.780.090.00–0.270.260.22–0.30
    Total0.760.400.730.59–0.790.040.00–0.170.240.21–0.26

TABLE 2. Correlations and Univariate Parameter Estimates for Additive, Shared Environmental, and Nonshared Environmental Effects Among 11-Year-Old Twin Offspring

Enlarge table

Discussion

We examined whether the link between parental substance use disorders and antisocial behavior and childhood disruptive disorders in the offspring is a result of the transmission of a general liability to multiple disorders or disorder-specific liabilities. Consistent with previous findings for the transmission of adult externalizing disorders to offspring in late adolescence (13), the present results indicated that a general transmission factor, externalizing, accounted for the co-occurrence of adult externalizing disorders in parents and childhood disruptive disorders in their offspring. In other words, parents pass on a general liability to externalizing psychopathology that is then manifested in their offspring in the form of attention problems, hyperactivity, oppositionality, and conduct problems. We also detected disorder-specific effects that were present across siblings rather than transmitted from parents to their children. Together, these results provide a conceptual model for the transmission of externalizing from parents to their offspring that accounts for the link between adult and child externalizing psychopathology.

We also estimated the genetic and environmental contribution to variations in both the general and specific liabilities to childhood disruptive disorders. The general externalizing O liability was highly heritable, indicating that common genetic risk factors predominately account for the comorbidity among childhood disruptive disorders. However, the externalizing O factor also exhibited a moderate shared environmental effect, indicating that certain environmental influences that increase twin similarity also account for the co-occurrence among childhood disruptive disorders. Thus, our results are consistent with previous work with adolescent and adult samples that have implicated a highly heritable externalizing factor (15, 34, 35) as well as with studies (and a recent meta-analysis) utilizing samples of children and reporting a strong influence of shared environmental effects (26, 36).

One notable finding concerns the large specific genetic effect on ADHD, suggesting that there are substantial genetic factors contributing to the variation in this disorder that are independent of the general liability to the externalizing O factor. This effect is not unique, since similar findings have been reported previously (37). This effect likely stems from the content differences between ADHD versus conduct disorder and oppositional defiant disorder. In particular, the latter disorders involve explicit violation of norms or rules, the rights others, and disobeying authority. ADHD, on the other hand, does not involve the violation of others' rights; rather, much of the content focuses on the child being difficult to manage and instruct. In this way, the content differences between the disorders might account for the specific genetic effect in ADHD.

Another notable finding was that shared environment influenced conduct disorder alone. The robust shared environmental influence on conduct disorder has been reported previously (21, 38). However, shared environmental influences on antisocial behavior seem to decline with age such that antisocial behavior in late adolescence and adulthood is influenced mainly by genetic factors (39, 40). Thus, our results revive the importance of examining shared environmental variables as processes underlying the comorbidity among childhood disruptive disorders.

Some limitations of the present study should be noted. First, the cross-sectional nature of the study precludes any inferences regarding generalizability of these findings at different ages. Second, the study utilized a homogenous sample of mostly Caucasian families. Third, although there was evidence for a correlation between paternal and maternal externalizing, there was no way of determining whether this correlation reflected a tendency of like individuals to mate with each other (assortative mating) or the tendency for spouses to become more similar with time. Fourth, although less of a limitation than a future direction, the present study did not examine the exact nature of the environmental influences on externalizing, such as socioeconomic status or neighborhood disruption, which could also have moderated the genetic effects on externalizing. The identification of the role of environmental factors may add precision to future work. Finally, we utilized a standard additive ACE model to estimate the effects of genes and environment. However, recent investigation focused on the genetics of externalizing behavior suggests the presence of gene-by-environment interactions as well. Future models following the present research will benefit from testing this model with and without the presence of gene-by-environment interaction effects.

To conclude, our results provide important insight into the familial aggregation of externalizing disorders. Specifically, they indicate that a focus of research should be to identify the common risk factors across disorders because this general liability accounts for the link between substance use disorders and antisocial behavior in parents and childhood disruptive disorders in their offspring. The knowledge of the general transmission effect and support for a common liability for childhood disruptive disorders might be of interest to clinicians as well. In particular, these results suggest that these common—more so than specific effects—should be the targets of prevention and intervention efforts.

These results also provide insight regarding how genetic and environmental factors can change as a function of developmental context. Although genetic factors are large and present throughout development, shared environmental factors are major contributors to the variation in the co-occurrence of externalizing behaviors in childhood (36), whereas in late adolescence and adulthood this is not necessarily true. A potential mechanism for this shift may be a transition from passive gene-environment correlation processes in childhood to evocative and active gene-environment correlation processes in adolescence and adulthood. Further research that integrates the current findings within a developmental context with a focus on delineating these mechanisms of gene-environment interplay will yield insights into the etiology of disruptive disorders. Such knowledge can inform intervention strategies to help alleviate the stress experienced by individuals and families.

From the Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis.
Address correspondence and reprint requests to Dr. Bornovalova,
Departments of Psychology and Mental Health Law and Policy, University of South Florida, 4202 East Fowler Ave., PCD4118G, Tampa, Fla. 33620
; (e-mail).

Received Sept. 7, 2009; revisions received Oct. 16, 2009 and Feb. 23, 2010; accepted March 11, 2010

The authors report no financial relationships with commercial interests.

Supported by National Institutes of Health grants DA-05147, DA-13240, DA-024417; AA-09367; and MH-017069.

All authors had full access to all of the data in the study and take responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.

References

1. Knopik VS , Heath AC , Jacob T , Slutske WS , Bucholz KK , Madden PAF , Waldron M , Martin NG: Maternal alcohol use disorder and offspring ADHD: disentan-gling genetic and environmental effects using a children-of-twins design. Psychol Med 2006; 36:1461–1471Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

2. Lahey BB , Piacentini JC , McBurnett K , Stone P , Hartdagen S , Hynd G: Psychopathology in the parents of children with conduct disorder and hyperactivity. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1988; 27:163–170Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

3. Malone SM , Iacono WG , McGue M: Drinks of the father: father's maximum number of drinks consumed predicts externalizing disorders, substance use, and substance use disorders in preadolescent and adolescent offspring. Alcoholism 2002; 26:1823–1832Google Scholar

4. Herndon RW , Iacono WG: The familial transmission of antisocial behavior from parent to child. Psychol Med 2005; 35:1815–1824Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

5. Jacob T , Waterman B , Heath A , True W , Bucholz KK , Haber R , Scherrer J , Fu Q: Genetic and environmental effects on offspring alcoholism: new insights us-ing an offspring-of-twins design. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2003; 60:1265–1272Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

6. Marmorstein NR , Iacono WG , McGue M: Alcohol and illicit drug dependence among parents: associations with offspring externalizing disorders. Psychol Med 2009; 39:149–155Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

7. Cadoret RJ , Troughton E , Ogorman TW: Genetic and environmental factors in alcohol abuse and antisocial personality. J Stud Alcohol 1987; 48:1–8Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

8. Cadoret RJ , Yates WR , Troughton E , Woodworth G , Stewart MA: Adoption study demonstrating two genetic pathways to drug abuse. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1995; 52:42–52Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

9. Chronis AM , Lahey BB , Pelham WE , Kipp HL , Baumann BL , Lee SS: Psychopathology and substance abuse in parents of young children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2003; 42:1424–1432Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

10. Merikangas KR , Dierker LC , Szatmari P: Psychopathology among offspring of parents with substance abuse and/or anxiety disorders: a high-risk study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1998; 39:711–720Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

11. Vanyukov MM , Kirisci L , Moss L , Tarter RE , Reynolds MD , Maher BS , Kirillova GP , Ridenour T , Clark DB: Measurement of the risk for substance use disor-ders: phenotypic and genetic analysis of an index of common liability. Behav Genet 2009; 39:596–610CrossrefGoogle Scholar

12. Hicks BM , Blonigen DM , Kramer MD , Krueger RF , Patrick CJ , Iacono WG , McGue M: Gender differences and developmental change in externalizing disorders from late adolescence to early adulthood: a longitudinal twin study. J Abnorm Psychol 2007; 116:433–447Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

13. Hicks BM , Krueger RF , Iacono WG , McGue M , Patrick CJ: Family transmission and heritability of externalizing disorders: a twin-family study. Arch Gen Psy-chiatry 2004; 61:922–928Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

14. Kendler KS , Prescott CA , Myers J , Neale MC: The structure of genetic and environmental risk factors for common psychiatric and substance use disorders in men and women. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2003; 60:929–937Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

15. Krueger RF , Hicks BM , Patrick CJ , Carlson SR , Iacono WG , McGue M: Etiologic connections among substance dependence, antisocial behavior, and person-ality: modeling the externalizing spectrum. J Abnorm Psychol 2002; 111:411–424Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

16. Krueger RE , Markon KE: Reinterpreting comorbidity: a model-based approach to understanding and classifying psychopathology. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 2006; 2:111–133Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

17. Krueger RF , Caspi A , Moffitt TE , Silva PA: The structure and stability of common mental disorders: a longitudinal-epidemiological study. J Abnorm Psychol 1998; 107:216–227Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

18. Krueger RF , Caspi A , Moffitt TE , Silva PA , McGee R: Personality traits are differentially linked to mental disorders: a multitrait-multidiagnosis study of an ado-lescent birth cohort. J Abnorm Psychol 1996; 105:299–312Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

19. Burt SA , Krueger RF , McGue M , Iacono W: Parent-child conflict and the comorbidity among childhood externalizing disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2003; 60:505–513Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

20. Dick DM , Viken RJ , Kaprio J , Pulkkinen L , Rose RJ: Understanding the covariation among childhood externalizing symptoms: genetic and environmental in-fluences on conduct disorder, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, and oppositional defiant disorder symptoms. J Abnorm Child Psychol 2005; 33:219–229Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

21. Eaves LJ , Silberg JL , Meyer JM , Maes HH , Simonoff E , Pickles A , Rutter M , Neale MC , Reynolds CA , Erikson MT , Heath AC , Loeber R , Truett KR , Hewitt JK: Genetics and developmental psychopathology, II: the main effects of genes and environment on behavioral problems in the Virginia Twin Study of Adolescent Behavioral Development. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discip 1997; 38:965–980Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

22. Wood AC , Rijsdijk F , Asherson P , Kuntsi J: Hyperactive-impulsive symptom scores and oppositional behaviours reflect alternate manifestations of a single li-ability. Behav Genet 2009; 39:447–460Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

23. Robins LN , Baber T , Cottler LB: International Diagnostic Interview: Expanded Substance Abuse Module. St Louis, Mo, Washington University School of Medi-cine, Department of Psychiatry, 1987Google Scholar

24. First MB , Spitzer RL , Gibbon M , Williams JB , Benjamin LS: Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis II Personality Disorders (SCID-II). Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Publishing, 1997Google Scholar

25. Reich W , Weiner Z: Revised Version of the Diagnostic Interview for Children and Adolescents (DICA-R). St. Louis, Mo, Washington University School of Medi-cine, Department of Psychiatry, 1988Google Scholar

26. Burt SA , McGue M , Krueger RF , Iacono WG: Sources of covariation among the child-externalizing disorders: informant effects and the shared environment. Psychol Med 2005; 35:1133–1144Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

27. Neale MC: Mx: Statistical Modeling, 3rd ed. Richmond, Va, Virginia Commonwealth University, Department of Psychiatry, 1997Google Scholar

28. Muthén LKM , Muthén BO: Mplus User's Guide, 5th ed. Los Angeles, Muthén and Muthén, 2007Google Scholar

29. Satorra A , Bentler PM: Corrections to test statistics and standard errors in covariance structure analysis, in Latent Variable Analysis: Applications to Develop-mental Research. Edited by von Eye AClifford CC. Newbury Park, Calif, Sage Publications, 1994Google Scholar

30. Schwarz G: Estimating the dimension of a model. Ann Stat 1978; 6:461–464CrossrefGoogle Scholar

31. Browne MW , Cudeck R: Alternative ways of assessing model fit, in Testing Structural Equation Models. Edited by Bollen KALong JS. Newbury Park, Calif, Sage Publications, 1993Google Scholar

32. Raftery AE: Bayesian model selection in social research. Sociol Methodol 1995; 25:111–163CrossrefGoogle Scholar

33. Plomin R , DeFries J , McClearn G , Rutter M: Behavioral Genetics. New York, WH Freeman, 1997Google Scholar

34. Slutske WS , Heath AC , Dinwiddie SH , Madden PAF , Bucholz KK , Dunne MP , Statham DJ , Martin NG: Common genetic risk factors for conduct disorder and alcohol dependence. J Abnorm Psychol 1998; 107:363–374Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

35. Young SE , Stallings MC , Corley RP , Krauter KS , Hewitt JK: Genetic and environmental influences on behavioral disinhibition. Am J Med Genet 2000; 96:684–695Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

36. Burt SA: Rethinking environmental contributions to child and adolescent psychopathology: a meta-analysis of shared environmental influences. Psychol Bull 2009; 135:608–637Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

37. Nadder TS , Rutter M , Silberg JL , Maes HH , Eaves LJ: Genetic effects on the variation and covariation of attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and oppositional defiant disorder/conduct disorder (ODD/CD) symptomatologies across informant and occasion of measurement. Psychol Med 2002; 32:39–53Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

38. Meyer JM , Rutter M , Silberg JL , Maes HH , Simonoff E , Shillady LL , Pickles A , Hewitt JK , Eaves LJ: Familial aggregation for conduct disorder symptoma-tology: the role of genes, marital discord and family adaptability. Psychol Med 2000; 30:759–774Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

39. Bergen SE , Gardner CO , Kendler KS: Age-related changes in heritability of behavioral phenotypes over adolescence and young adulthood: a meta-analysis. Twin Res Hum Genet 2007; 10:423–433Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

40. Lyons MJ: Epidemiology of personality disorders, in Psychiatric Epidemiology. Edited by Tsuang MTTohen MZahner GP. New York, John Wiley and Sons, 1995, pp 407–436Google Scholar