The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×

Abstract

Objective: Impaired cognitive-behavioral flexibility is regarded as a trait marker in anorexia nervosa patients. The authors sought to investigate the neural correlates of this deficit in executive functioning in anorexia nervosa. Method: Fifteen women with anorexia nervosa and 15 age-matched healthy comparison women underwent event-related functional MRI while performing a target-detection task. The task distinguished between shifts in behavioral response and shifts in cognitive set. It involved infrequent target and non-target distractor stimuli embedded in a sequence of prepotent standard stimuli. Results: Relative to comparison subjects, anorexia nervosa patients showed a significantly higher error rate in behavioral response shifting, independent of whether those runs also involved cognitive set shifting. During behavioral response shifting, patients showed reduced activation in the left and right thalamus, ventral striatum, anterior cingulate cortex, sensorimotor brain regions, and cerebellum that differed significantly from the comparison group but showed dominant activation in frontal and parietal brain regions. These differential activations in patients and comparison subjects were specific to shifts in behavioral response: except for thalamic activation, they were not observed in response to non-target distractor trials that required no alteration in behavioral response. Conclusion: Impaired behavioral response shifting in anorexia nervosa seems to be associated with hypoactivation in the ventral anterior cingulate-striato-thalamic loop that is involved in motivation-related behavior. In contrast, anorexia nervosa patients showed predominant activation of frontoparietal networks that is indicative of effortful and supervisory cognitive control during task performance.

Anorexia nervosa is a serious psychosomatic illness with high morbidity and significant lifetime mortality. About half of patients with anorexia nervosa show a chronic course of the disease despite treatment (1) . Clinical characteristics seen in anorexia nervosa patients are maladaptive preoccupations with food, weight, and body shape that manifest in stereotyped and rigid behaviors controlling eating and weight. These characteristics are seen as symptomatic expressions of obsessive-compulsive temperament traits, which frequently precede the onset of the disorder (2) and are associated with a negative course of the disease (1) . Consistent with an obsessive-compulsive phenotype, anorexia nervosa patients show high concern over mistakes and a relentless perfectionism (3) .

On a neuropsychological level, these clinical traits of cognitive-behavioral inflexibility in anorexia nervosa can be conceptualized as impaired cognitive set shifting (i.e., concrete and rigid approaches to changing rules) and impaired behavioral response shifting (i.e., stereotyped or perseverative behaviors). The construct of set shifting in anorexia nervosa has been used as a broad and underspecified term without a clear differentiation of cognitive set shifting from behavioral response shifting. A meta-analysis of neuropsychological studies (4) found a consistent deficit in set-shifting ability in anorexia nervosa; deficits were found in the ill and recovered stage of the disease and also in discordant sister pairs, which suggests that impaired set shifting is a characteristic trait in anorexia nervosa (4 , 5) .

Numerous studies have shown that fronto-striato-thalamic pathways play a pivotal role in shifts of cognitive set and behavioral response (68) . In functional neuroimaging studies, the lateral prefrontal cortex and the anterior cingulate/ventromedial prefrontal cortex have been regularly found to be related to cognitive set shifting and inhibitory control (79) . Subcortical regions such as the thalamus, the subthalamic nucleus, and the striatum facilitate shifts in behavioral response mediated by the prefrontal cortex (10 , 11) .

Brain imaging studies addressing frontal-subcortical circuits in the resting state in anorexia nervosa have shown increased metabolic rates in the subcortical striatum, thalamus, and brainstem and prefrontal alterations in the medial prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex (1215) . There is preliminary evidence for altered dopamine signaling in the striatum that is known to be involved in executive control, habit learning, and reward pathways (16 , 17) .

Functional MRI (fMRI) studies of anorexia nervosa have been primarily concerned with the functional neuroanatomy of the processing of eating disorder-related and reward-relevant cues (18) . In this study, we used an established visual flexibility paradigm with non-affect-laden stimuli (geometric figures) that allows for a differentiation between shifts in behavioral response with and without shifts in cognitive set (8) . Behavioral response shifting required study subjects to inhibit a prepotent impulse in favor of an alternative, less automatic behavior. Cognitive set shifting required the subject to change stimulus-response associations.

We hypothesized, first, that anorexia nervosa patients would show impaired behavioral response shifting and, second, that behavioral findings would be more profound in anorexia nervosa in behavioral-shift trials when additional shifts in cognitive set are required. In addition, we predicted that these impairments in executive control in anorexia nervosa would be associated with altered activations in fronto-striato-thalamic loops.

Method

Participants

A total of 30 women were enrolled: 15 anorexia nervosa patients (seven with the restricting subtype) and 15 age-matched healthy women in the normal weight range and with no lifetime diagnoses of psychiatric illness. Diagnosis was made using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders (SCID; 19). Inclusion criteria were age between 18 and 30 years and right-handedness. Exclusion criteria were metallic implants, claustrophobia, current psychopharmacological medication, life-threatening condition, poor German-speaking ability, and lifetime diagnosis of obsessive-compulsive disorder, major depressive disorder, bipolar disorder, psychosis, and alcohol or drug abuse or dependence. Patients were recruited from our wards and outpatient clinic, and comparison subjects were recruited through public advertisements. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants, as approved by the ethics committee of the University of Heidelberg Medical School.

Procedure

Before entering the study, participants were interviewed with the SCID to screen for lifetime psychiatric diseases (19) . They also completed a battery of self-report questionnaires, including the Eating Disorder Inventory–2 (20) , the nine-item depression module of the Patient Health Questionnaire (21) , the anxiety subscale of the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (22) , and the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Personality Disorders (19) for an obsessive-compulsive personality disorder. Participants were asked to avoid eating and drinking caffeine-containing beverages for 1 hour and alcohol for 24 hours before the experiment.

fMRI Task

We used a visual target-detection task in an event-related design (8) . The stimuli were geometric shapes (squares, triangles, and circles) of different colors and sizes. Participants were required to classify each stimulus on the basis of its shape as target, non-target, or standard stimuli. They were required to respond to non-target and standard stimuli with one button press and to target stimuli with an alternative button press.

The stimuli were centered on a white background for 500 msec, with a stimulus interval of 1000 msec. In each of the 11 runs, infrequently occurring target (five per run) and non-target (five per run) stimuli were embedded in frequently occurring standard (154 per run) stimuli. The infrequent target and non-target stimuli were separated by a minimum of nine standard stimuli to allow adequate spacing between events to prevent an overlap of event-related hemodynamic responses.

Before each run, the target stimulus was defined as either a circle (C) or a triangle (T). The target changed after every two runs (TTCCTTCC), resulting in conditions where the target either changed at the start of the run (TC or CT: “shift” runs) or stayed the same from the previous run (TT, CC: “maintain” runs). Targets occurring during “maintain” runs required behavioral shifts only, that is, changing which response button to press. In contrast, those occurring during “shift” runs required shifts in behavioral response and stimulus-response associations (cognitive set shifting), that is, the target had changed from the previous run.

Immediately before the scanning session, participants were trained on the task using a personal computer. In the scanner, participants viewed the stimuli on a semitransparent screen via a head-coil-mounted mirror system. Accuracy (percent correct) and reaction time data were acquired for each trial.

fMRI Acquisition

Images were acquired with a Siemens Trio 3-T scanner equipped with a standard single-channel head coil. We used a rapid echo-planar imaging sequence covering the whole brain with the following parameters: repetition time=2 seconds, echo time=30 msec, flip angle=80 degrees, 33 slices parallel to the anterior commissure-posterior commissure line, slice thickness=4 mm, in-plane resolution 3.4×3.4 mm. In addition, a high-resolution sagittal T 1 -weighted image was acquired with the following parameters: 176 slices, voxel size 1×1×1 mm, repetition time=11 msec, echo time=4.92 msec, flip angle=15 degrees.

Data Analysis

Data analysis was performed with SPM2 (Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, Institute of Neurology, London) implemented in MATLAB, version 7 (MathWorks, Natick, Mass.). The images of the first run were discarded (setup of the first “maintain” run), and the analyses were based on the images of the five “maintain” and five “shift” runs that followed. Functional images were corrected for differences in slice acquisition timing and were realigned with the allowed motion limited to ±3 mm translation and to ±3 degree rotation. Coregistration was performed with the high-resolution T 1 -weighted image. Both functional and structural images were normalized to the Montreal Neurological Institute template. Functional images were spatially smoothed with a kernel of 8 mm full width at half maximum. Low-frequency signal drifts were removed using a 128-second high-pass filter, and temporal autocorrelation in the fMRI time series was corrected using a first-order autoregressive model.

A general linear model was fitted to the single-subject data using six regressors of interest for each subject (target “shift,” target “maintain,” non-target “shift,” non-target “maintain,” standard “maintain,” and standard “shift”), which were modeled as stick functions convolved with the canonical hemodynamic response function. Only correct trials were considered for analyses, except for target trials. T contrasts were calculated for the following comparisons: 1) correct target relative to standard stimuli; 2) correct target “shift” relative to standard stimuli; 3) correct target “maintain” relative to standard stimuli; 4) correct non-target relative to standard stimuli; and 5) incorrect relative to correct target stimuli.

For the group analysis, single-subject contrast images were entered into a hierarchical model equivalent to a random-effects model, allowing population inference (23) . Generic group maps for anorexia nervosa patients and comparison subjects and between-group comparisons were computed with one-sample t tests and two-sample t tests, respectively. Correlations of depression and anxiety with brain activation were determined by entering scores from the Patient Health Questionnaire or the anxiety subscale of the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale and contrast images (correct target relative to standard stimuli) into a simple regression model. All results reported are significant at a cluster-level-corrected threshold of p<0.05.

The behavioral data were analyzed with repeated-measures analyses of variance to determine within-subject effects and group-by-stimulus interaction. Between-group differences were analyzed with Student’s t tests or chi-square tests as appropriate. A p value <0.05 (two-tailed) was considered statistically significant.

Results

The demographic and clinical characteristics of the participants are summarized in Table 1 . Patients and comparison subjects did not differ significantly with respect to age, gender, marital status, or educational level.

Behavioral Performance

Data on accuracy and reaction time (for correct trials only) to the different stimulus categories are summarized in Figure 1 .

Figure 1. Mean Reaction Time (For Correct Trials Only) and Accuracy Score in Anorexia Nervosa Patients (N=15) and Healthy Comparison Subjects (N=15) in Response to Three Stimulus Categories Followed by Target Stimuli Divided Into “Shift” and “Maintain” Runs a

a A significant interaction was observed between group and stimulus category (F=4.6, df=2, 28, p=0.035). Relative to comparison subjects, anorexia nervosa patients had less accuracy to target stimuli but performed equally in response to non-target and standard stimuli.

Accuracy

All participants were less accurate in response to target stimuli compared with both non-target and standard stimuli (main effect of stimulus category: F=103.9, df=1, 28, p<0.001). The comparison between patients and comparison subjects showed a significant group-by-stimulus category interaction effect (F=4.6, df=2, 28, p=0.035): relative to comparison subjects, anorexia nervosa patients had fewer correct responses to target stimuli but had an equivalent performance to non-target and standard stimuli. Accuracy was increased in “maintain” runs relative to “shift” runs (main effect of stimulus category: F=13.0, df=1, 28, p=0.001) in both patients and comparison subjects (group-by-stimulus category interaction: F<1). Commission rather than omission errors were most common (ratio of 9:1).

Reaction time

The response time for correct results to target and non-target stimuli was longer than that to standard stimuli (main effect of stimulus category: F=129.0, df=1, 28, p<0.001). There were no differences in reaction time between patients and comparison subjects. The reaction time for correct target responses was significantly longer in “shift” runs than in “maintain” runs (main effect of stimulus category: F=24.9, df=1, 28, p<0.001) in both patients and comparison subjects (group-by-stimulus category interaction: F<1).

The reaction time was shorter for incorrect compared with correct target responses (main effect of correctness: F=199.0, df=1, 28, p<0.001), without significant differences between patients and comparison subjects (group-by-correctness interaction: F<1). Both patients and comparison subjects showed post-error slowing: mean reaction time to the first two standard trials after an erroneous target response compared with a correct target response (main effect of post-error processes: F=7.0, df=1, 28, p=0.013), but no significant group-by-post-error processes interaction (F<1).

Imaging Data

Within-group activations

Compared with standard trials, correct target trials in the comparison group were associated with large clusters with peak activations in the thalamus bilaterally, the left putamen including the anterior insula, the prefrontal cortex bilaterally (Brodmann’s area 9, 10), sensorimotor brain regions extending into the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (Brodmann’s area 24), the right inferior parietal cortex (Brodmann’s area 40), the posterior cingulate cortex (Brodmann’s area 23), the secondary visual cortex bilaterally (Brodmann’s area 18, 19), and the cerebellum ( Table 2 , Figure 2 ).

Figure 2. Brain Activation in Response to Target and Non-Target Relative to Standard Trials in Healthy Comparison Subjects (N=15) and Anorexia Nervosa Patients (N=15) a

a Panel A shows representative brain slices and surface renderings (right hemisphere) of brain activation in response to target relative to standard trials in both groups. Panel B shows brain regions that showed greater activations in comparison subjects relative to patients in response to target and non-target trials relative to standard trials. Brain slices are shown in neurological convention (right hemisphere is on the right-hand side of the axial and coronal image). The color of the cluster represents the intensity of activation (T value) according to the gradation of the color bar.

In patients, correct target trials compared with standard trials engaged the left and right anterior temporal pole, anterior insula, and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (Brodmann’s area 13, 22, 47); the right middle and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (Brodmann’s area 9, 10), temporoparietal junction (Brodmann’s area 37, 40), and posterior parietal cortex with right hemispheric dominance; the secondary visual cortex bilaterally (Brodmann’s area 19); and the cerebellum ( Table 2 , Figure 2 ). The separation of correct trials in “maintain” runs and “shift” runs showed brain networks in each group similar to those in the overall target trials, but with differences in the level of regional brain activation. During “shift” compared with “maintain” runs, the comparison group showed greater activation of the ventral putamen bilaterally and less activation of the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. The patients showed greater right lateral prefrontal cortex activation and less right temporoparietal activation during “shift” compared with “maintain” runs (data not shown). Correct responses to non-target compared with standard trials differed from target trials in that both patients and comparison subjects showed no basal ganglia activation (data not shown).

In comparison subjects, incorrect compared with correct target responses showed greater activation of the dorsal anterior cingulate (Brodmann’s area 32) extending into the supplemental motor area (Brodmann’s area 6) and the posterior cingulate/parahippocampal gyrus (Brodmann’s area 30). In patients, there was no significant activation to incorrect compared with correct responses to target trials ( Table 2 ).

Between-group activations

Relative to comparison subjects, patients showed decreased activation in the thalamus bilaterally (including the subthalamic nucleus), the rostral cingulate cortex (Brodmann’s area 32), the sensorimotor brain regions (Brodmann’s area 3, 4, 6), and the cerebellum to correct target versus standard trials ( Table 3 ). These findings seemed to be independent of task performance, as confirmed by an analysis of subgroups (N=11 in both groups) matched for task performance (group-by-stimulus category interaction: F<1).

In correct target “shift” trials (compared with standard trials), the anorexia nervosa group activated the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex and the ventral putamen to a lesser extent than the comparison group, and in target “maintain” trials (compared with standard trials), the anorexia nervosa group engaged the rostral anterior cingulate and the middle and dorsal striatum to a lesser degree than the comparison group.

Group comparison for correct non-target trials (compared with standard trials) showed no significant activation differences, except for the thalamus (x=–3, y=–24, z=3; size=163 voxels; T value=5.22).

There were no brain regions in response to correct target and non-target trials (compared with standard trials) that showed greater activation in patients relative to the comparison group.

Incorrect compared with correct responses to target trials showed decreased activation in patients relative to comparison subjects in the dorsal anterior cingulate (Brodmann’s area 32) extending to the medial frontal gyrus (Brodmann’s area 6, 8).

Correlation with anxiety and depression

There were no clusters showing a significant correlation between comorbid anxiety (anxiety subscale of the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale) and depression (Patient Health Questionnaire) and task-related brain activation to correct target compared with standard trials in anorexia nervosa patients.

Discussion

This is the first study to show that anorexia nervosa patients have altered activation in fronto-striato-thalamic pathways during the performance of behavioral response shifting. These alterations were specific to shifts in behavioral response: except for thalamic activation, they were not observed in response to non-target trials that required recognition of the stimulus but no alteration in behavioral response. In the main analysis we focused on correct trials, but we also addressed error-related brain activation to investigate control processes. Anorexia nervosa patients showed less error-related activation than did comparison subjects in prefrontal regions involved in conflict monitoring and error detection. These findings are in accord with a previous EEG study that reported decreased error monitoring (i.e., error-related negativity) in anorexia nervosa (24) .

The executive function paradigm differentiated the anorexia nervosa patients from the comparison group in that anorexia nervosa patients were less accurate (i.e., they made more commission errors) in behavioral response shifting. Commission errors in the patient group were not due to greater impulsivity or careless responses (i.e., shorter reaction times) but rather resulted from increased behavioral rigidity. Behavioral adjustments following an erroneous response did not differ between patients and comparison subjects.

Contrary to our second hypothesis, the deficit in executive control was independent of whether behavioral “shift” trials involved additional shifts in cognitive set; patients and comparison subjects showed a similarly increased switching cost (longer reaction times, higher error rates) to additional cognitive set shifting. Previous evidence suggests that anorexia nervosa patients might be especially prone to impaired cognitive set shifting, although none of the published studies used selected tasks designed to differentiate specific processes of cognitive-behavioral inflexibility (4) . For example, the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, one of the most frequently used tasks, has a complex design that includes additional cognitive processes, such as implicit learning (rules are covertly changed). Our data in this study indicate that anorexia nervosa patients demonstrate no deficit in cognitive set shifting when the change of the stimulus-response association is overtly displayed. Instead, clinical manifestations of stereotyped and rigid behaviors in anorexia nervosa may reflect poor generation of shifts in behavioral response. This finding is supported by impaired behavioral response shifting in neuromotor function tests, such as diadochokinesis and two-finger tapping (25 , 26) . Further studies that systematically differentiate between shifts in behavioral response and cognitive set in anorexia nervosa are needed to corroborate our findings. The comparison group activated the established neural network involved in shifts of behavioral response and cognitive set, including subcortical brain regions (the thalamus and striatum), the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, the anterior insula/ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, the parietal cortex, premotor brain regions reaching into the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex, and the cerebellum (7 , 8) . In contrast, anorexia nervosa patients showed a failure to recruit subcortical (thalamus, striatum) and frontal (anterior cingulate cortex, supplementary motor area) brain regions, which differed significantly from the comparison group. Patients showed preserved neural activation in the right ventral frontoparietal network (the right middle frontal gyrus and the temporoparietal junction bilaterally) that was not observed in the comparison group. This cortical system is involved in shifting attention and interruption of ongoing cognitive activity, when infrequent behaviorally relevant stimuli occur (27) . The dominance of higher-order association cortices (the frontoparietal network) over impaired ascending motivational processes has been reported in a study using a gambling task (17) . These findings suggest that anorexia nervosa patients show a pattern of predominant effortful and supervisory cognitive control in response to diverse tasks. The results were independent of task performance, so these different activation patterns are likely to reflect the use of alternative strategies during behavioral response shifting.

The most prominent finding in patients relative to comparison subjects was decreased bilateral thalamus activation during the processing of infrequent events (target and non-target stimuli). The thalamus is part of an alerting network that allows reflexive distribution of relevant sensory information to numerous divergent brain regions (28 , 29) and also plays a relevant role in higher-order cognition through reciprocal cortico-thalamo-cortical projections (30 , 31) . Notably, the thalamus activation in comparison subjects was greater to behavioral response shifting compared with non-target processing, which supports top-down priming of target selectivity and reactivity within the thalamus. The mediodorsal nuclei of the thalamus were most activated. This area is closely connected to the ventral striatum and prefrontal cortex as part of a neural circuit that facilitates behavioral response shifting mediated by the prefrontal cortex (6 , 11) . The finding of impaired thalamus function in anorexia nervosa patients is supported by brain imaging studies in the resting state (32 , 33) .

Patients also had impaired activation of the anterior cingulate cortex and the putamen during behavioral response shifting. The anterior cingulate cortex subserves performance monitoring and cognitive conflict detection (34 , 35) and has been implicated in motivational and evaluative aspects (i.e., reward, reinforcement learning) to control and initiate behavior (36 , 37) . Therefore, the findings of decreased anterior cingulate cortex and supplementary motor area activation in anorexia nervosa patients may indicate reduced motivation and initiative drive to nonroutine (infrequent) challenges. The impaired activation of the anterior cingulate in anorexia nervosa is supported by previous morphometric and functional brain imaging studies (1214) .

Relative to comparison subjects, patients showed decreased activation of the ventral striatum (putamen). The ventral striatal circuit receives input from the anterior cingulate cortex, ventromedial prefrontal cortex, hippocampus, and amygdala (6) , forming a pathway involved in motivational processing and the elimination of inappropriate response options (38 , 39) . This is in accord with studies of anorexia nervosa that have reported reduced ventral striatal activation to wins and losses, altered dopamine D 2 /D 3 receptor binding of the ventral striatum, or a generally reduced sensitivity of the appetitive motivational system (16 , 17 , 40) . However, the similarities with the gambling task of Wagner et al. (17) were restricted to the ventral striatum system, as we found no group differences between patients and comparison subjects in dorsal striatum activation.

In summary, we found decreased task-related activation at different levels within the anterior cingulate-striato-thalamic network. This network is involved in motivational processes subserving the initiation of goal-directed behavior. Thus, the neural findings support the thesis that the behavioral impairment reflects poor initiation of behavioral response shifting.

Limitations

Our sample size was insufficient to examine diagnostic differences between subgroups (e.g., restricting versus purging subtypes of anorexia nervosa). Since we investigated exclusively chronically ill patients, we were not able to control for any potential exacerbating effects of prolonged starvation. Additional region-of-interest analyses do not indicate a pattern of a global and nonspecific effect of hypoactivation/reduced blood flow in anorexia nervosa patients. Further studies in patients who have recovered will be important to clarify this issue. Comorbid depression and anxiety may have influenced the findings. However, the levels of depression and anxiety were not correlated with brain activation. Focusing the main outcome on correct trials may have limited our ability to test our second hypothesis, namely, the differentiation of shifts in behavioral response and cognitive set. This was acceptable, however, because on a behavioral level, no group differences between the two conditions were observed.

Conclusion

These data provide novel insights in neural mechanisms of impaired cognitive-behavioral flexibility in anorexia nervosa that may represent a core dysfunction within the brain’s fronto-striato-thalamic circuitry. This network has been implicated in motivational, emotional, and incentive processes to control cognitive operations and to attain specific goals. Behavioral and neural results suggest that impaired flexibility in anorexia nervosa may not be primarily related to poor cognitive set shifting but may be secondary to impaired behavioral response shifting.

From the Center for Psychosocial Medicine, University Hospital Heidelberg, Department of Psychosomatic and General Internal Medicine. Received May 25, 2008; revisions received Aug. 13 and Oct. 9, 2008; accepted Nov. 6, 2008 (doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.08050775). Address correspondence and reprint requests to Dr. Friederich, Centre for Psychosocial Medicine, University Hospital Heidelberg, Department of Psychosomatic and General Internal Medicine, Thibautstr. 2, 69115 Heidelberg, Germany; hans-christoph.friederich@med. uni-heidelberg.de (e-mail).

All authors report no competing interests.

Drs. Zastrow and Kaiser contributed equally to this work.

Supported in part by the German Federal Ministry of Education and Research (BMBF: 01GV0624).

The authors thank Tamara Bullinger, Carina Frick, and Sandra Schild for assistance with several aspects of this work.

References

1. Steinhausen H-C: The outcome of anorexia nervosa in the 20th century. Am J Psychiatry 2002; 159:1284–1293Google Scholar

2. Anderluh MB, Tchanturia K, Rabe-Hesketh S, Treasure J: Childhood obsessive-compulsive personality traits in adult women with eating disorders: defining a broader eating disorder phenotype. Am J Psychiatry 2003; 160:242–247Google Scholar

3. Bulik CM, Tozzi F, Anderson C, Mazzeo SE, Aggen S, Sullivan PF: The relation between eating disorders and components of perfectionism. Am J Psychiatry 2003; 160:366–368Google Scholar

4. Roberts ME, Tchanturia K, Stahl D, Southgate L, Treasure J: A systematic review and meta-analysis of set-shifting ability in eating disorders. Psychol Med 2007; 37:1075–1084Google Scholar

5. Holliday J, Tchanturia K, Landau S, Collier D, Treasure J: Is impaired set-shifting an endophenotype of anorexia nervosa? Am J Psychiatry 2005; 162:2269–2275Google Scholar

6. Alexander GE, Crutcher MD, DeLong MR: Basal ganglia-thalamocortical circuits: parallel substrates for motor, oculomotor, “prefrontal,” and “limbic” functions. Prog Brain Res 1990; 85:119–146Google Scholar

7. Cools R, Clark L, Robbins TW: Differential responses in human striatum and prefrontal cortex to changes in object and rule relevance. J Neurosci 2004; 24:1129–1135Google Scholar

8. Shafritz KM, Kartheiser P, Belger A: Dissociation of neural systems mediating shifts in behavioral response and cognitive set. Neuroimage 2005; 25:600–606Google Scholar

9. Aron AR, Fletcher PC, Bullmore ET, Sahakian BJ, Robbins TW: Stop-signal inhibition disrupted by damage to right inferior frontal gyrus in humans. Nat Neurosci 2003; 6:115–116Google Scholar

10. Monchi O, Petrides M, Strafella AP, Worsley KJ, Doyon J: Functional role of the basal ganglia in the planning and execution of actions. Ann Neurol 2006; 59:257–264Google Scholar

11. Block AE, Dhanji H, Thompson-Tardif SF, Floresco SB: Thalamic-prefrontal cortical-ventral striatal circuitry mediates dissociable components of strategy set shifting. Cereb Cortex 2007; 17:1625–1636Google Scholar

12. Mühlau M, Gaser C, Ilg R, Conrad B, Leibl C, Cebulla MH, Backmund H, Gerlinghoff M, Lommer P, Schnebel A, Wohlschläger AM, Zimmer C, Nunnemann S: Gray matter decrease of the anterior cingulate cortex in anorexia nervosa. Am J Psychiatry 2007; 164:1850–1857Google Scholar

13. Naruo T, Nakabeppu Y, Deguchi D, Nagai N, Tsutsui J, Nakajo M, Nozoe S: Decreases in blood perfusion of the anterior cingulate gyri in anorexia nervosa restricters assessed by SPECT image analysis. BMC Psychiatry 2001;1:2 [epub June 4, 2001]Google Scholar

14. Uher R, Murphy T, Brammer MJ, Dalgleish T, Phillips ML, Ng VW, Andrew CM, Williams SCR, Campbell IC, Treasure J: Medial prefrontal cortex activity associated with symptom provocation in eating disorders. Am J Psychiatry 2004; 161:1238–1246Google Scholar

15. Delvenne V, Goldman S, De Maertelaer V, Wikler D, Damhaut P, Lotstra F: Brain glucose metabolism in anorexia nervosa and affective disorders: influence of weight loss or depressive symptomatology. Psychiatry Res 1997; 74:83–92Google Scholar

16. Frank GK, Bailer UF, Henry SE, Drevets W, Meltzer CC, Price JC, Mathis CA, Wagner A, Hoge J, Ziolko S, Barbarich-Marsteller N, Weissfeld L, Kaye WH: Increased dopamine D2/D3 receptor binding after recovery from anorexia nervosa measured by positron emission tomography and [ 11 C]raclopride. Biol Psychiatry 2005; 58:908–912 Google Scholar

17. Wagner A, Aizenstein H, Venkatraman VK, Fudge J, May JC, Mazurkewicz L, Frank GK, Bailer UF, Fischer L, Nguyen V, Carter C, Putnam K, Kaye WH: Altered reward processing in women recovered from anorexia nervosa. Am J Psychiatry 2007; 164:1842–1849Google Scholar

18. Treasure J, Friederich HC: Neuroimaging, in Eating Disorders in Children and Adolescents. Edited by Jaffa T, McDermott B. Cambridge, England, Cambridge University Press, 2007, pp 95–107Google Scholar

19. Wittchen HU, Zaudig M, Fydrich T: Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV—German Version. Göttingen, Hogrefe, 1997Google Scholar

20. Thiel A, Jacobi C, Horstmann S, Paul T, Nutzinger DO, Schussler G: [A German version of the Eating Disorder Inventory EDI-2.] Psychother Psychosom Med Psychol 1997; 47:365–376 (German)Google Scholar

21. Lowe B, Spitzer RL, Grafe K, Kroenke K, Quenter A, Zipfel S, Buchholz C, Witte S, Herzog W: Comparative validity of three screening questionnaires for DSM-IV depressive disorders and physicians’ diagnoses. J Affect Disord 2004; 78:131–140Google Scholar

22. Zigmond AS, Snaith RP: The Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1983; 67:361–370Google Scholar

23. Holmes AP, Friston KJ: Generalisability, random effects and population inference. Neuroimage 1998; 7(suppl):S754Google Scholar

24. Pieters GL, de Bruijn ER, Maas Y, Hulstijn W, Vandereycken W, Peuskens J, Sabbe BG: Action monitoring and perfectionism in anorexia nervosa. Brain Cogn 2007; 63:42–50Google Scholar

25. Wentz E, Gillberg IC, Gillberg C, Rastam M: Ten-year follow-up of adolescent-onset anorexia nervosa: physical health and neurodevelopment. Dev Med Child Neurol 2000; 42:328–333Google Scholar

26. Green MW, Elliman NA, Wakeling A, Rogers PJ: Cognitive functioning, weight change, and therapy in anorexia nervosa. J Psychiatr Res 1996; 30:401–410Google Scholar

27. Corbetta M, Shulman GL: Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci 2002; 3:201–215Google Scholar

28. Fan J, McCandliss BD, Fossella J, Flombaum JI, Posner MI: The activation of attentional networks. Neuroimage 2005; 26:471–479Google Scholar

29. Kiehl KA, Stevens MC, Laurens KR, Pearlson G, Calhoun VD, Liddle PF: An adaptive reflexive processing model of neurocognitive function: supporting evidence from a large-scale (N=100) fMRI study of an auditory oddball task. Neuroimage 2005; 25:899–915Google Scholar

30. Sherman SM: The thalamus is more than just a relay. Curr Opin Neurobiol 2007; 17:417–422Google Scholar

31. Klostermann F, Wahl M, Marzinzik F, Schneider GH, Kupsch A, Curio G: Mental chronometry of target detection: human thalamus leads cortex. Brain 2006; 129:923–931Google Scholar

32. Takano A, Shiga T, Kitagawa N, Koyama T, Katoh C, Tsukamoto E, Tamaki N: Abnormal neuronal network in anorexia nervosa studied with I-123-IMP SPECT. Psychiatry Res 2001; 107:45–50Google Scholar

33. Husain MM, Black KJ, Doraiswamy PM, Shah SA, Rockwell WJ, Ellinwood EH Jr, Krishnan KR: Subcortical brain anatomy in anorexia and bulimia. Biol Psychiatry 1992; 31:735–738Google Scholar

34. Bush G, Luu P, Posner MI: Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex. Trends Cogn Sci 2000; 4:215–222Google Scholar

35. Kerns JG, Cohen JD, MacDonald AW III, Cho RY, Stenger VA, Carter CS: Anterior cingulate conflict monitoring and adjustments in control. Science 2004; 303:1023–1026Google Scholar

36. Bush G, Vogt BA, Holmes J, Dale AM, Greve D, Jenike MA, Rosen BR: Dorsal anterior cingulate cortex: a role in reward-based decision making. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2002; 99:523–528Google Scholar

37. Kennerley SW, Walton ME, Behrens TE, Buckley MJ, Rushworth MF: Optimal decision making and the anterior cingulate cortex. Nat Neurosci 2006; 9:940–947Google Scholar

38. Heimer L: A new anatomical framework for neuropsychiatric disorders and drug abuse. Am J Psychiatry 2003; 160:1726–1739Google Scholar

39. Zink CF, Pagnoni G, Martin ME, Dhamala M, Berns GS: Human striatal response to salient nonrewarding stimuli. J Neurosci 2003; 23:8092–8097Google Scholar

40. Friederich HC, Kumari V, Uher R, Riga M, Schmidt U, Campbell IC, Herzog W, Treasure J: Differential motivational responses to food and pleasurable cues in anorexia and bulimia nervosa: a startle reflex paradigm. Psychol Med 2006; 36:1327–1335Google Scholar