The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×
ArticleFull Access

Unmasking Disease-Specific Cerebral Blood Flow Abnormalities: Mood Challenge in Patients With Remitted Unipolar Depression

Abstract

OBJECTIVE: Remitted major depressive disorder is a vulnerable clinical state, suggesting persistence of an underlying disease diathesis between episodes. To investigate neural correlates of such risk and to identify potential depression trait markers, euthymic unipolar patients in remission, acutely depressed patients, and never-depressed volunteers were studied before and after transient sad mood challenge. METHOD: Common and differential changes in regional blood flow among the groups relative to the baseline state were examined with [15O]H2O positron emission tomography after provocation of sadness with autobiographical memory scripts. RESULTS: Mood provocation in both depressed groups resulted in regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF) decreases in medial orbitofrontal cortex Brodmann’s area 10/11, which were absent in the healthy group. In the remitted group, mood provocation produced a unique rCBF decrease in pregenual anterior cingulate 24a. The main effects in healthy subjects, an rCBF increase in subgenual cingulate Brodmann’s area 25 and a decrease in right prefrontal cortex Brodmann’s area 9, were not present in the depressed groups. CONCLUSIONS: Mood challenge in unipolar euthymic patients in full remission unmasks an apparent depression trait marker. The pattern of acute CBF changes is distinct from that seen in euthymic healthy volunteers and mirrors the untreated depressed state seen during a major depressive episode and the pattern of change seen in depressed patients. These findings suggest that disease-specific modifications of pathways mediating transient mood changes are present in unipolar depression independent of clinical illness status. These findings have implications for understanding the vulnerability of remitted patients for illness relapse.

Patients with major unipolar depression experience periods of spontaneous or treatment-induced remission, during which they typically are symptom free or have only mild, subclinical mood or cognitive symptoms (1, 2). However, remitted patients, while clinically well, remain vulnerable to illness relapse. A growing literature suggests correlative relationships between greater sensitivity of recovered patients to acute emotional stress and risk for depression relapse and recurrence (35). While genetics (6), developmental factors (7), and stress (8) are strongly implicated in mechanisms of disease vulnerability, neural correlates of these trait or disease diathesis markers are yet to be characterized.

Functional imaging studies have provided additional perspectives toward understanding neural mechanisms mediating affective symptoms. To date, state effects have been a primary focus, with many studies examining abnormalities during a major depressive episode and after various forms of treatment, with normalization of prefrontal and anterior cingulate abnormalities repeatedly identified (917). Complementary studies of transient induced sadness in healthy volunteers have provided additional confirmation of state-specific neural changes mediating negative mood (1822). Regional changes in the subgenual cingulate Brodmann’s area 25, prefrontal cortex, anterior insula, and posterior cingulate, exactly mirroring those seen with treatment-facilitated resolution of chronic dysphoria in depressed patients, further point to region-specific state effects that wax and wane with the presence and intensity of sad mood (14).

Identification of brain changes that define the underlying disease process (trait or disease effects) rather than symptom severity (state effects) has been more difficult, as few studies have examined fully recovered patients without medication. We know of no studies demonstrating regional abnormalities in at-risk individuals before disease presentation. Nonetheless, a role for the medial orbitofrontal cortex (2325) and rostral anterior cingulate (26, 27) as possible disease markers of major depressive disorder has been suggested by both magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) and postmortem anatomical abnormalities, and persistent resting-state metabolism and blood flow abnormalities, respectively. It is not yet clear, however, whether the volumetric and neuropathological changes in the medial orbital and subgenual prefrontal cortex represent causes or effects of depression (2327). It cannot be concluded that the findings pertaining to persistent rostral anterior cingulate Brodmann’s area 24a are disease trait markers and not a compensatory change precipitated by initial illness onset (26, 27).

Putative brain mechanisms associated with symptom relapse in patients with unipolar depression have been studied by using acute tryptophan depletion and positron emission tomography (PET) during treatment and after recovery. Depressed patients 6 weeks into a course of antidepressant treatment who experienced a tryptophan-depletion-induced acute relapse exhibited glucose metabolic decreases in the orbitofrontal cortex and thalamus relative to patients without clinical symptoms (28). A similar approach was used in a [15O]H2O PET study in recovered depressed men. Comparable regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF) decreases in the ventral anterior cingulate, orbitofrontal cortex, and caudate were observed with tryptophan-induced relapse, suggesting the critical role of these regions in mediating transient, hyperacute clinical symptoms (29). A comparable pattern of regional abnormalities is seen in refractory depressed patients (10, 26, 30) and in patients with depression associated with neurological disorders (3134).

Despite compelling convergence of regional findings, the tryptophan-depletion method appears to precipitate a depressive relapse selectively in acutely depressed patients receiving treatment with selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) and not in patients taking other classes of antidepressants (35). It only appears to do so early in the course of treatment, and it does not consistently trigger mood symptoms in depressed patients in full recovery (between episodes) or in nondepressed subjects. In other words, it appears to be a probe of disease relapse linked to a specific neurotransmitter system, not a general test of relapse vulnerability in all patients with unipolar depression (sick and recovered). In contrast, an alternative method of precipitating acute depression symptoms—negative mood induction (3, 5)—appears to aggravate depressive symptoms and predict relapse in depressed patients in full remission, independent of medication status, and represents a more general and naturalistic model of depression recurrence (5). Since this approach appears to effectively precipitate mood change in fully recovered patients and in never-depressed subjects, it offers a potentially more robust method to identify trait- or disease-related markers of unipolar depression.

Therefore, in the present study, a mood-provocation technique—memory-induced sadness—and PET were used to identify potential depression trait and state markers in asymptomatic, fully recovered patients. Two alternative hypotheses were tested:

1.

Sad-mood induction in euthymic, recovered, depressed patients results in regional effects similar to those observed in never-depressed healthy subjects, particularly involving the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and subgenual anterior cingulate (state account).

2.

Sad mood induction in euthymic, depressed patients results in some changes in common with those observed in healthy subjects (state effects) but also involves unique changes that reflect the temporary or transient unmasking of disease or trait marker effects.

To this aim, autobiographical, script-based, sad memories were employed in three groups of patients: an experimental group of remitted unipolar patients, a comparison group of acute unipolar depressed patients (to control for disease effects), and a comparison group of healthy volunteers (to control for state effects). Remitted patients kept receiving optimal antidepressant medication (nine of 10) so as not to precipitate withdrawal or emerging depressive symptoms before the study. To control for the effects of antidepressant medication and therefore to test for “pure” disease effects, the acute depressed group was medication free. Results from the healthy group have been published elsewhere (14, 21). Here they will be used only as a comparison for the depressed patients’ groups.

Method

Subjects

The experimental group consisted of 10 women (mean age=37 years, SD=9) with a previous DSM-IV diagnosis of major unipolar depression who were currently euthymic and in full remission (mean remission interval=16 months, SD=8). Criteria for inclusion were based on a structured interview performed by a psychiatrist (S.L.B.). Remission was defined as an absence of symptoms (recovery) for a minimum of 6 months with no recurrence (4). Nine patients were taking maintenance antidepressant medication (six were taking SSRIs only, two were taking SSRIs in combination with other antidepressants, and one was taking another antidepressant), and one was medication free. Inclusion criteria were absence of other primary psychiatric or neurological diagnosis or head injury and no history of substance abuse. Remitted unipolar depressed patients were identified and referred by psychiatrists in the San Antonio area in the course of routine follow-up visits.

Seven women with an active major unipolar depressive episode (mean=42 years, SD=15) who were outpatients at the University of Texas Health Science Center’s San Antonio Mood Disorders Clinic formed the acute depression comparison group. The patients all met DSM-IV criteria for active major depression (in the midst of a major unipolar depression episode) and had no other primary diagnosis of psychiatric or neurological disorders, head trauma, or substance abuse. All had recurrent episodes; none was a first-episode patient. All except one were medication free at the time of the study. All patients completed the 17-item Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression on the day of the PET scanning. The Hamilton depression scale was used to evaluate symptom severity in the acute depression group and the presence of residual symptoms in the remitted group. Hamilton depression scale scores were mean=4.8 (SD=4.9, range=0–15) for the remitted group and mean=21.3 (SD=2.9, range=17–25) for the acutely depressed group. Two patients in the remitted group had a Hamilton depression scale score greater than 7. They did not, however, meet DSM-IV criteria for active depression.

The healthy comparison group consisted of eight women (mean age=36 years, SD=6) with no personal or family history of depression and no history of other psychiatric or neurological disease, head trauma, or substance abuse. None was taking any medication. This group has been the focus of two previous studies (14, 21).

Mood Provocation

All subjects were asked to prepare in advance short autobiographical narratives of personal events in which they felt particularly sad. Sad scenarios most commonly centered on loss of relatives, friends, or significant relationships (21). Written informed consent was obtained from all subjects, and all procedures were conducted as approved by the institutional review board of the University of Texas Health Science Center in San Antonio.

Imaging Methods

PET scans were acquired on a GE/Scanditronix 4096 (Uppsala, Sweden) (15 parallel slices, center-to-center interslice distance=6.5 mm, transaxial field of view=10.0 cm) by using measured attenuation correction (68germanium/68gallium transmission scans) and reconstructed with an in-plane resolution of 7 mm, full width at half maximum. CBF was measured by using a bolus [15O]H2O technique (60–65 mCi [15O]H2O dose/scan, [15O]H2O half-life=123 seconds, scan duration=90 seconds).

Subjects were immobilized within the PET scanner by use of individually fitted, thermally molded plastic facial masks (36) and faced a monitor placed 35 cm from their eyes. An antecubital venous catheter was placed for administration of the blood flow radiotracer.

Each subject underwent a series of measurements of brain blood flow during two tasks: eyes closed and at rest and sad provocation. The order of conditions was counterbalanced across subjects. Of importance, scans were acquired only after the sad emotional state was achieved and not on the basis of a standard interscan interval (10 minutes) for tracer decay. Before each emotional scan, radiotracer synthesis was started when the subject reported an intermediate subjective response (score of 3 or 4). After that, we only injected the dose if the reported subjective response was very intense. Otherwise, injection was postponed and the induction scenario was repeated until it achieved the desired intensity. Similarly, after each emotional scan, radiotracer synthesis was delayed until remnants of previous mood states had cleared (subject score <3).

An anatomical MRI scan was also acquired in each subject for the purposes of spatial transformation of PET data, region-of-interest analysis, and parametric image display (Elscint Gyrex 1.9T-DLX, Haifa, Israel) three-dimensional gradient/recall acquisition in the steady state sequence, TR=33 msec, TE=12 msec, flip angle=60°, volume=256×256×127, spatial resolution=1 mm3).

Mood Induction

Details about the mood-provocation procedure have been reported elsewhere (21). In brief, before all “sad scans,” scripts were presented on the screen. Subjects were asked to generate a state of sadness comparable to that originally experienced. Every 2 minutes, the subjects were asked to rate their level of sadness, anxiety, and relaxation on a 0–7-point scale. After the desired mood intensity was achieved, subjects closed their eyes and were instructed to stop visualizing, thinking, or ruminating on the script and to focus on their feelings of sadness. After each emotional scan, subjects were engaged in 3–5 minutes of neutral conversation to facilitate return to a baseline euthymic state. All subjects were debriefed after each emotional scan to ensure that they followed the instructions and to inquire about content of the experience, the dominant modality of imagery, and whether they became tearful or experienced intense bodily changes.

PET Data Analysis

All analyses were performed by using previously validated methods and in-house software. Differential patterns of sadness-related changes in the three groups were detected by using change-distribution analysis (37) and interpreted by using atlas-based coordinates and Brodmann’s areas. Automatic alignment and reslicing of the PET images was first performed to correct for head motion by using a modified version of the Woods et al. algorithm (38). PET and MRI images were then spatially transformed into proportional bicommissural coordinate space (39) relative to the 1988 stereotaxic atlas of Talairach and Tournoux (40). Regional tissue uptake of [15O]H2O was globally normalized to a whole-brain rCBF mean, with images scaled to an arbitrary mean of 1,000. Value and spatially normalized images were trilinearly interpolated, resampled (60 slices, 8 mm3 voxels), and Gaussian filtered to a final resolution of 9.9 mm (full width at half maximum) before statistical analysis.

Pairwise voxel-by-voxel statistical contrasts were performed to identify common and differential regional changes associated with sadness provocation (i.e., sad provocation versus eyes closed and at rest) in the experimental group (remitted group), the acute depressed comparison group, and in the healthy comparison group. Condition-specific, within-subject regional changes were then averaged across individuals. A beta-2 statistic measuring kurtosis of the histogram of the difference image (the change distribution curve) was used as an omnibus test to assess overall significance (41). The beta-2 test was implemented in MIPS software (Research Imaging Center, San Antonio, Tex.) in a manner similar to the use of the gamma-2 statistic (37). The beta-2 improves on the gamma-2 by using a better estimate of the degrees of freedom, i.e., the number of resyls in the PET images (42). The omnibus test was followed by maxima and minima searches to identify local extrema within a search volume measuring 125 mm3(43). Last, group mean subtraction images were converted to statistical parametric images of z scores and superimposed on group mean MRI images for the subject group. Locations of focal maxima and minima exceeding a z score of 2.9 (p<0.002) were recorded with the peak voxel (search cube volume=125 mm3) of each extrema described in x, y, and z coordinates as millimeters relative to the anterior commissure and to the spatial extent in mm3. To facilitate visualization, data are displayed at a p<0.05 significance level.

A second approach was used to visualize common and unique regional changes associated with sadness provocation between the experimental group (remitted depressed) and each of the comparison groups (healthy volunteers and acute depressed patients). Pairwise, between groups, voxel-by-voxel contrasts of significant sadness-related changes were carried out separately for rCBF increases or decreases (logical contrasts). The threshold for significance was a z score of 1.96 (p<0.05).

Results

Behavioral Effects

Mean rating scores for sadness intensity are reported for each scan and patient group in Table 1. Remitted patients reported no sadness in the baseline relaxed state, while they reported a mean score of 6.3 on a 7-point scale (SD=1.1) after sadness provocation. Eight of 10 had visible tears. No significant anxiety was reported during the resting state (mean=1.1, SD=0.9) or during the sad state (mean=1.2, SD=0.9).

Healthy comparison subjects reported no sadness during the resting scans, while they reported a mean score of 6.5 (SD=0.7) after sadness provocation. Seven of eight had visible tears. No significant anxiety was reported during the resting state (mean=0.8, SD=1.0) or during the sad state (mean=0.9, SD=0.5).

The acutely depressed group achieved a similar higher level of sadness after provocation (mean=6.5, SD=0.7), and six of seven patients had visible tears. A slightly higher level of sadness was reported by members of this group at baseline (mean=1.6, SD=1.2) because of their status of acute illness. Similarly, their level of anxiety was slightly higher than that of the other two groups (at rest: mean=1.4, SD=1.3; after sadness provocation: mean=2.1, SD=1.4). There were no statistically significant differences in scores across groups.

PET Results

Sadness relative to the resting state resulted in areas of both significant rCBF increases and decreases among all groups. Significant changes in the remitted and acute groups are shown in Table 2 and Table 3, respectively (z scores >2.7, p<0.003). Data for the healthy group have been the focus of two previous reports (14, 21) and are not repeated here. The column called “Group(s) With Change” indicates whether the rCBF changes were shared or unique to the various groups, as defined by a common Brodmann’s designation of local maxima or minima.

Remitted Depression

Sadness provocation resulted in significant rCBF changes (omnibus significance: beta-2=3.34, df=1972, p=0.0008). Activations were observed in the ventral anterior insular cortex, dorsal anterior cingulate, globus pallidus, caudate, cerebellar vermis, lateral inferofrontal cortex, premotor and sensorimotor cortex, and visual cortex. Deactivations were present in medial orbitofrontal cortex Brodmann’s areas 10, 11, and 32 and in pregenual cingulate 24a, the posterior cingulate cortex, medial and lateral posterior parietal cortex, posterior inferior temporal cortex (Brodmann’s area 21/20), lateral visual cortex Brodmann’s area 18, anterior thalamus, and posterior insula (Table 2).

Acute Depression

As in the previous group, sad mood precipitation resulted in significant rCBF changes (omnibus significance: beta-2=3.29, df=1972, p=0.001). Activations were present in the ventral insula, dorsal anterior cingulate, motor and premotor cortex, midline cerebellum, and inferior frontal cortex. Deactivations were present in the medial orbitofrontal and prefrontal cortex, posterior cingulate cortex, right anterior thalamus, bilateral inferior parietal cortex, bilateral inferior temporal cortex Brodmann’s area 20/21, and right parahippocampal gyrus (Table 3).

Sadness in Depressed and Healthy Subjects

Common and unique significant effects of mood provocation across the three groups are shown in Figure 1 and Figure 2. Common effects of sadness across the three groups were rCBF activations of the ventral insula, motor/premotor cortex, and cerebellar vermis and deactivations in the posterior cingulate, inferior parietal cortex, and inferior temporal cortex. Unique effects of sadness shared by both depressed groups but not the healthy group were increases in activation in the dorsal anterior cingulate and lateral inferior frontal cortex and deactivations in the medial orbitofrontal cortex, anterior thalamus, and occipital cortex.

Unique effects of sadness in the healthy group were the activation of ventral subgenual anterior cingulate Brodmann’s area 25 and the left parahippocampal gyrus and deactivation of right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex Brodmann’s area 9/46 (Figure 1).

Unique changes in the remitted depression group were activations in the globus pallidus and deactivations of pregenual cingulate 24a. Unique changes in the acute group were deactivations in the middle temporal gyri and right parahippocampal gyrus. Differences in the relative size of changes (area of involvement) between depressed groups were a greater involvement of the insula, basal ganglia, parietal cortex, and posterior cingulate in the remitted group and a greater involvement of the dorsal anterior cingulate, motor/premotor cortex, cerebellum, and inferior temporal cortex in the acutely depressed group.

Discussion

The main finding of this study is that the pattern of brain activity after sad mood precipitation in euthymic, fully remitted, depressed patients overlapped with that observed in acutely depressed patients rather than that observed in never-depressed healthy subjects. More specifically, mood provocation in unipolar depressed outpatients—independent of current clinical status—resulted in rCBF decreases in medial orbitofrontal cortex Brodmann’s area 10/11, extending dorsally to medial Brodmann’s areas 10, 9, and 8 and the anterior thalamus. In addition, in the remitted group, mood provocation produced a unique rCBF decrease in pregenual anterior cingulate 24a. The main effects of sadness induction in healthy subjects—i.e., an rCBF increase in subgenual cingulate Brodmann’s area 25 and a decrease in right prefrontal cortex Brodmann’s area 9 (14, 21)—were not observed in either of the unipolar depressed groups. A number of regional changes were, however, common to sad mood provocation in healthy subjects and in unipolar depressed subjects, i.e., rCBF increases in the insular cortex, cerebellar vermis, and motor/premotor cortex and rCBF decreases in the parietal cortex, posterior cingulate cortex, and posterior inferior temporal cortex Brodmann’s area 20/21 and may reflect shared circuitry in mood regulation in health and disease.

Taken together, this pattern of findings—similar to the regional abnormalities found in untreated primary (10, 26, 30) or secondary (3134) unipolar depression—is consistent with our second hypothesis, i.e., that provoked sadness in recovered depressed patients results in an unmasking of a purported disease-specific effect or marker.

Brodmann’s Area 10/11

The main finding in this study is decreased medial orbitofrontal cortex rCBF with sad mood precipitation in both remitted and acutely depressed patients, which was absent in healthy subjects. Similar abnormally decreased rCBF and metabolism have been observed before in more severely ill, refractory, or inpatient groups with major depression relative to healthy subjects (10, 2630), while different regional abnormalities have been reported in less severely ill outpatient cohorts. Furthermore, similar smaller medial orbitofrontal cortex effects have been found in patients with secondary depression after Parkinson’s disease (31), Huntington’s disease (32), caudate stroke (33), or complex partial seizures (34) relative to nondepressed comparison groups with the same disorder. The similarity of these findings in the medial orbitofrontal cortex suggests that mood provocation in patients with remitted depression unmasks a disease marker of hyperacute unipolar depression.

Furthermore, a similar decrease in rCBF in the medial orbitofrontal cortex has been reported in two published reports of tryptophan-induced depression relapse in recovered depressed patients relative to a similar group of patients without depletion-induced symptom aggravation (28, 29), suggesting that decreased activity in the medial orbitofrontal cortex is a common feature of acute depression relapse.

The ventromedial inferior frontal and orbitofrontal cortex have been identified as substrates of reward mechanisms in animals and humans (44), such as the ability to respond to feedback in planning and guessing tasks and in gambling tasks (45, 46). Impairments have been described in clinically depressed patients (47) who also failed to show activation of the medial orbitofrontal cortex during such tasks relative to comparison subjects (48). Our findings suggest that medial orbitofrontal cortex function is selectively affected in severe clinical depression (melancholia), but it is preserved in milder forms of depression (nonmelancholic) or in transient sadness in healthy volunteers and could perhaps explain differences between such entities.

Brodmann’s Area 47/45

Activation in the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex was observed in both depressed groups after sadness provocation and to a lesser degree in the healthy group. Similarly, increased activation in the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex than in healthy comparison groups has been reported in similar cohorts of outpatients with major depression (49, 50), including a group that responded to psychotherapy (51). In addition, in a number of studies of sadness in healthy volunteers, similar ventrolateral prefrontal cortex activations were found when contrasting sadness to a resting state (1821). It is suggested that ventrolateral prefrontal cortex activation reflects the use or unmasking of more ruminative elements of depression that may dominate the picture in nonmelancholic outpatients but not be present in more severely ill, melancholic inpatients. In support of this contention, no ventrolateral prefrontal cortex effects have been found in any studies in our laboratory involving more severely ill inpatient cohorts (10, 15, 26). In a critical test of this theory in our laboratory, healthy subjects were scanned at a resting euthymic state or after memory-driven induction of sadness, anxiety, or a neutral relaxed state. Ventrolateral prefrontal cortex activation was present in all active conditions relative to the resting state, and it disappeared when sadness or anxiety were contrasted to the neutral memory state (14, 21).

Brodmann’s Area 24a

Remitted depressed patients provoked into depression displayed a unique deactivation of pregenual anterior cingulate 24a that was not present in the acute group. While no changes in this region were observed in previous studies as a function of state (i.e., mood) change in healthy subjects with sadness and in patients with illness remission (14), the resting metabolism of Brodmann’s area 24a was found to predict response to 6 weeks of fluoxetine treatment; it was higher than normal in responders and lower than normal in nonresponders. Therefore, mood provocation in our remitted group appeared to unmask a functional marker associated with severe refractory depression (26, 27).

Of importance, acutely ill patients showed a more widespread increase in rCBF in the dorsal anterior cingulate region, including a small cluster in pregenual 24a. One interpretation of this finding is that pregenual 24a may be uniquely sensitive to the acute unmasking of severe depression, which only happens in the remitted group. An alternative interpretation considers recent findings from studies in healthy volunteers demonstrating that pregenual anterior cingulate 24a mediates emotion-attention interactions as tapped by the Emotional Stroop Task (52). In a related study (53), we demonstrated that pregenual 24a activity during a sad state was dramatically and selectively increased during augmentation of the level of attentional conflict of a Stroop task. It is conceivable that the sadness procedure is more effortful in the acute group, perhaps accounting for the unique activation of this region.

Brodmann’s Area 25

Subgenual anterior cingulate Brodmann’s area 25 had been previously identified as one of the main regions involved in state-dependent mood changes across health and disease (14, 19–22). In contrast, no significant changes with sadness provocation were observed in the present study in either the remitted or acute depressed groups at any statistical threshold. To further explore the role of this area in depression, we conducted a secondary analysis, addressing the comparison of rCBF in this region during the resting state in the remitted depression group relative to the healthy comparison group. Resting rCBF in Brodmann’s area 25 in the remitted group was significantly lower than that in the healthy comparison group, suggesting that Brodmann’s area 25 becomes deactivated at rest and remains suppressed after mood provocation. Possible interpretations of this finding are that suppression of Brodmann’s area 25 may be a requisite for mood normalization in recovered patients or that suppression of Brodmann’s area 25 may be due to antidepressant medication (in all but one subject). The latter interpretation is unlikely in light of the fact that no change in Brodmann’s area 25 was observed with mood provocation in the acute depressed group (all but one patient was unmedicated). We hypothesize that full recovery in unipolar depression is accompanied by a functional reorganization of limbic-cortical pathways, resulting in sustained suppression of area 25 and an absence of transient recruitment of the same region during negative mood shifts associated with sadness provocation. In contrast, in the acute depression group, it is possible that rCBF in Brodmann’s area 25 was already elevated at rest and did not change with mood provocation.

Brodmann’s Area 9/46

A second major change previously associated with mood/state effects—i.e., in right prefrontal cortex Brodmann’s area 9/46—was not observed with mood precipitation in the remitted group or in the acute depression group. In both groups, rCBF did not change significantly after sadness induction. Similar group-specific effects in the right prefrontal cortex were also found in a separate event-related potential study in which the same subjects in the three groups performed a sustained-attention, target-detection task (54) specifically tapping into the function of the right prefrontal cortex (55).

These combined findings suggest that depressed patients undergo a functional reorganization of limbic-cortical networks relevant to sadness regulation affecting particularly the subgenual anterior cingulate 25 and right prefrontal cortex 9, which may limit the downstream effects of mood state on cognitive functioning. In support of this interpretation, the two regions have known bilateral anatomical connections; they have displayed a significant intercorrelation across changes of mood state in health and disease (14, 21), and a recent covariance analysis (56) suggested specific changes in effective connectivity between the two regions in both depressed groups.

Conclusions and Clinical Implications

The combined findings of the present study suggest that provocation of transient sadness in unipolar depression patients in clinical remission precipitates a change in selective brain regions associated with treatment response and characteristic of regional abnormalities seen in chronic treatment-resistant patients (10, 26, 30). Overall, this transient stressor appears to unmask hyperacute depressive symptoms similar to those observed in recovered depressed patients after symptom relapse with tryptophan depletion (28, 29). We postulate that use of an acute mood challenge in unipolar patients in clinical remission unmasks syndrome-specific functional lesions that are the primary sites of aberrant mood regulation in this disorder. We further propose that pregenual 24a and occipitofrontal 10/11 represent sites of vulnerability in patients with unipolar depression. This conclusion is consistent with the identified role of pregenual 24a as a predictor of treatment response in acutely depressed patients (26, 27). To further emphasize such a role, rCBF in 24a was decreased after acute mood challenge, transiently resembling the hyperacute state of severe refractory depression (26, 27).

It could be argued that the regional changes observed in the remitted group may be due to residual, subsyndromal depression symptoms rather than to the purported disease marker. Two patients in the remitted group, although not meeting DSM-IV criteria for a major depressive episode, had a Hamilton depression score greater than 7. To rule out the possibility that the neural changes observed in the remitted group were due to symptom persistence, a secondary analysis was carried out in the remitted group after exclusion of the two patients with Hamilton depression scale scores greater than 7. The conditional contrast of sadness versus rest in this restricted group (N=8) yielded identical results to those in the entire group, thus corroborating our main interpretation of the findings.

We emphasize that transient sadness is construed here as a stressor able to trigger a hyperacute depressive state before any compensatory mechanism is initiated. By no means is transient sadness equated to (considered synonymous with) the chronic dysphoria of clinical depression. Transient sadness lacks other syndromal symptoms of clinical depression, although cognitive changes may be present (54).

Memory-induced relapse appears to be a more naturalistic model of temporary illness recurrence, as observed in remitted patients after exposure to emotional stressors. Unlike the tryptophan-depletion method, which has been used to characterize relapse early in the course of SSRI treatment (5), mood-provoked symptoms can be observed in patients who have been in a recovered state for over a year—with or without medication. Future studies will require additional studies to link challenge-induced changes to long-term outcome. It is important to stress, however, that induced sadness requires a very careful study design, thus constraining its usefulness in psychiatric research.

Finally, it could also be argued that the functional reorganization observed in the remitted unipolar depression group may be the result of a compensatory state produced by antidepressant treatment (nine of 10 patients were taking SSRIs or combined treatment). However, the most important findings of the study were also observed in the acute unipolar depression group, which was for the most part (six of seven not taking medications) medication free at the time of the study. It is still possible that the effects observed may be partially explained by previous long-term antidepressant treatment or chronic illness, since all unipolar depression patients (remitted or acute) had a previous history of treatment of their recurrent episodes of major depression. Future studies with never-medicated unipolar depressed patients should fully assess the influence of long-term treatment. Also, it is not known whether the observed changes represent a marker of disease vulnerability or if there is a reorganization in brain function because of disease. Future studies of family members at risk will be needed to test this hypothesis.

TABLE 1
TABLE 2
TABLE 3

Presented in part at the Third International Conference on Functional Mapping of the Human Brain, Copenhagen, May 19–23, 1997, and the 28th annual meeting of the Society for Neuroscience, New Orleans, Oct. 25–30, 1997. Received Nov. 14, 2001; revision received May 22, 2002; accepted May 29, 2002. From the Research Imaging Center and the Departments of Psychiatry and Neurology, University of Texas Health Science Center, San Antonio, TX. Address reprint requests to Dr. Liotti, Division of Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine and Health Sciences, University of Nottingham, A Floor, South Block, Queens Medical Centre, Nottingham NG7 2UH, U.K.; (e-mail). Supported by grants from the Charles A. Dana foundation to Dr. Mayberg and the National Alliance for Research on Schizophrenia and Depression to Dr. Liotti.

Figure 1.

Figure 1. Images of Changes in Regional Cerebral Blood Flow (rCBF) During Provoked Sadness in Healthy Comparison Subjects (N=8), Patients With Remitted Unipolar Depression (N=10), and Patients With Acute Unipolar Depression (N=7)a

aSignificant rCBF increases (in red) and decreases (in green) after sadness provocation overlaid on each group’s averaged magnetic resonance image. Top row: sagittal view at x=–4. Bottom row: coronal view at y=+3x; x, y, z coordinates are expressed in millimeters from the anterior commissure.

Figure 2.

Figure 2. Overlap of Significant Changes in Regional Cerebral Blood Flow (rCBF) During Provoked Sadness in Patients With Remitted Unipolar Depression (N=10) and Patients With Acute Unipolar Depression (N=7)a

az score threshold: ±1.96, p<0.05, uncorrected.

References

1. Frank E, Thase ME: Natural history and preventative treatment of recurrent mood disorders. Annu Rev Med 1999; 50:453-468Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

2. Fava M, Kendler KS: Major depressive disorder. Neuron 2000; 28:335-341Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

3. Teasdale JD, Segal ZV, Williams JM, Ridgeway VA, Soulsby JM, Lau MA: Prevention of relapse/recurrence in major depression by mindfulness-based cognitive therapy. J Consult Clin Psychol 2000; 68:615-623Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

4. Kupfer DJ, Frank E: The interaction of drug and psychotherapy in the long-term treatment of depression. J Affect Disord 2001; 62:131-137Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

5. Segal ZV, Gemar M, Williams S: Differential cognitive response to a mood challenge following successful cognitive therapy or pharmacotherapy for unipolar depression. J Abnorm Psychol 1999; 108:3-10Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

6. Kendler KS, Thornton LM, Gardner CO: Genetic risk, number of previous depressive episodes, and stressful life events in predicting onset of major depression. Am J Psychiatry 2001; 158:582-586LinkGoogle Scholar

7. Jacobs BL, Praag H, Gage FH: Adult brain neurogenesis and psychiatry: a novel theory of depression. Mol Psychiatry 2000; 5:262-269Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

8. Sapolsky RM: The possibility of neurotoxicity in the hippocampus in major depression: a primer on neuron death. Biol Psychiatry 2000; 48:755-765Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

9. Baxter LR Jr, Schwartz JM, Phelps ME, Mazziotta JC, Guze BH, Selin CE, Gerner RH, Sumida RM: Reduction of prefrontal cortex glucose metabolism common to three types of depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1989; 46:243-250Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

10. Mayberg HS, Lewis PJ, Regenold W, Wagner HN Jr: Paralimbic hypoperfusion in unipolar depression. J Nucl Med 1994; 35:929-934MedlineGoogle Scholar

11. Bench CJ, Friston KJ, Brown RG, Scott LC, Frackowiak RS, Dolan RJ: The anatomy of melancholia—focal abnormalities of cerebral blood flow in major depression. Psychol Med 1992; 22:607-615Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

12. Bench CJ, Frackowiak RS, Dolan RJ: Changes in regional cerebral blood flow on recovery from depression. Psychol Med 1995; 25:247-261Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

13. Buchsbaum MS, Wu J, Siegel BV, Hackett E, Trenary M, Abel L, Reynolds C: Effect of sertraline on regional metabolic rate in patients with affective disorder. Biol Psychiatry 1997; 41:15-22Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

14. Mayberg HS, Liotti M, Brannan SK, McGinnis S, Mahurin RK, Jerabek PA, Silva JA, Tekell JL, Martin CC, Lancaster JL, Fox PT: Reciprocal limbic-cortical function and negative mood: converging PET findings in depression and normal sadness. Am J Psychiatry 1999; 156:675-682AbstractGoogle Scholar

15. Mayberg HS, Brannan SK, Tekell JL, Silva JA, Mahurin RK, McGinnis S, Jerabek PA: Regional metabolic effects of fluoxetine in major depression: serial changes and relationship to clinical response. Biol Psychiatry 2000; 48:830-843Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

16. Mayberg HS, Silva JA, Brannan SK, Tekell JL, Mahurin RK, McGinnis S, Jerabek PA: The functional neuroanatomy of the placebo effect. Am J Psychiatry 2002; 159:728-737LinkGoogle Scholar

17. Brody AL, Saxena S, Silverman DH, Alborzian S, Fairbanks LA, Phelps ME, Huang SC, Wu HM, Maidment K, Baxter LR Jr: Brain metabolic changes in major depressive disorder from pre- to post-treatment with paroxetine. Psychiatry Res 1999; 91:127-139Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

18. Pardo JV, Pardo PJ, Raichle ME: Neural correlates of self-induced dysphoria. Am J Psychiatry 1993; 150:713-719LinkGoogle Scholar

19. George MS, Ketter TA, Parekh PI, Horwitz B, Herscovitch P, Post RM: Brain activity during transient sadness and happiness in healthy women. Am J Psychiatry 1995; 152:341-351LinkGoogle Scholar

20. Lane RD, Reiman EM, Ahern GL, Schwartz GE, Davidson RJ: Neuroanatomical correlates of happiness, sadness, and disgust. Am J Psychiatry 1997; 154:926-933LinkGoogle Scholar

21. Liotti M, Mayberg HS, Brannan SK, McGinnis S, Jerabek P, Fox PT: Differential limbic-cortical correlates of sadness and anxiety in healthy subjects: implications for affective disorders. Biol Psychiatry 2000; 48:30-42Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

22. Damasio AR, Grabowski TJ, Bechara A, Damasio H, Ponto LL, Parvizi J, Hichwa RD: Subcortical and cortical brain activity during the feeling of self-generated emotions. Nat Neurosci 2000; 3:1049-1056Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

23. Rajkowska G, Miguel-Hidalgo JJ, Wei J, Dilley G, Pittman SD, Meltzer HY, Overholser JC, Roth BL, Stockmeier CA: Morphometric evidence for neuronal and glial prefrontal cell pathology in major depression. Biol Psychiatry 1999; 45:1085-1098Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

24. Drevets WC, Price JL, Simpson JR Jr, Todd RD, Reich T, Vannier M, Raichle ME: Subgenual prefrontal abnormalities in mood disorders. Nature 1997; 386:824-827Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

25. Ongur D, Drevets WC, Price JL: Glial reduction in the subgenual prefrontal cortex in mood disorders. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1998; 95:13290-13295Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

26. Mayberg HS, Brannan SK, Mahurin RK, Jerabek PA, Brickman JS, Tekell JL, Silva JA, McGinnis S, Glass TG, Martin CC, Fox PT: Cingulate function in depression: a potential predictor of treatment response. Neuroreport 1997; 8:1057-1061Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

27. Brannan SK, Mayberg HS, McGinnis S, Silva JA, Tekell J, Mahurin RK, Jerabek PA, Fox PT: Cingulate metabolism predicts treatment response: a replication (abstract). Biol Psychiatry 2000; 47:107SCrossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

28. Bremner JD, Innis RB, Salomon RM, Staib LH, Ng CK, Miller HL, Bronen RA, Krystal JH, Duncan J, Rich D, Price LH, Malison R, Dey H, Soufer R, Charney DS: Positron emission tomography measurement of cerebral metabolic correlates of tryptophan depletion-induced depressive relapse. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1997; 54:364-374Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

29. Smith KA, Morris JS, Friston KJ, Cowen PJ, Dolan RJ: Brain mechanisms associated with depressive relapse and associated cognitive impairment following tryptophan depletion. Br J Psychiatry 1999; 174:525-529Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

30. Ketter TA, Kimbrell TA, George MS, Willis MW, Benson BE, Danielson A, Frye MA, Herscovitch P, Post RM: Baseline cerebral hypermetabolism associated with carbamazepine response, and hypometabolism with nimodipine response in mood disorders. Biol Psychiatry 1999; 46:1364-1374Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

31. Mayberg HS, Starkstein SE, Sadzot B, Preziosi T, Andrezejewski PL, Dannals RF, Wagner HN Jr, Robinson RG: Selective hypometabolism in the inferior frontal lobe in depressed patients with Parkinson’s disease. Ann Neurol 1990; 28:57-64Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

32. Mayberg HS, Starkstein SE, Peyser CE, Brandt J, Dannals RF, Folstein SE: Paralimbic frontal lobe hypometabolism in depression associated with Huntington’s disease. Neurology 1992; 42:1791-1797Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

33. Mayberg HS: Frontal lobe dysfunction in secondary depression. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 1994; 6:428-442Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

34. Bromfield EB, Altshuler L, Leiderman DB, Balish M, Ketter TA, Devinsky O, Post RM, Theodore WH: Cerebral metabolism and depression in patients with complex partial seizures. Arch Neurol 1992; 49:617-623; correction, 49:976Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

35. Leyton M, Ghadirian AM, Young SN, Palmour RM, Blier P, Helmers KF, Benkelfat C: Depressive relapse following acute tryptophan depletion in patients with major depressive disorder. J Psychopharmacol 2000; 14:284-287Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

36. Fox PT, Perlmutter JS, Raichle ME: A stereotactic method of anatomical localization for positron emission tomography. J Comput Assist Tomogr 1985; 9:141-153Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

37. Fox PT, Mintun MA: Noninvasive functional brain mapping by change-distribution analysis of averaged PET images of [15O]H2O tissue activity. J Nucl Med 1989; 30:141-149MedlineGoogle Scholar

38. Woods RP, Cheery SR, Mazziotta JC: Rapid automated algorithm for aligning and reslicing PET images. J Comput Assist Tomogr 1992; 16:620-633Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

39. Lancaster JL, Glass TG, Lankipalli BR, Downs H, Mayberg HS, Fox PT: A modality independent approach to spatial normalization of tomographic images of the human brain. Hum Brain Mapp 1995; 3:209-223CrossrefGoogle Scholar

40. Talairach J, Tournoux P: Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain: Three-Dimensional Proportional System. New York, Thieme Medical, 1988Google Scholar

41. Agostino RB, Belanger A, D’Agostino RB Jr: A suggestion for using powerful and informative tests of normality. Am Statistician 1990; 44:316-321Google Scholar

42. Worsley KJ, Evans AC, Marrett S, Neelin A: Three-dimensional statistical analysis for CBF activation studies in human brain. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 1992; 12:900-918Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

43. Mintun MA, Fox PT, Raichle ME: A highly accurate method of localizing regions of neuronal activation in the human brain with positron emission tomography. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 1989; 9:96-103Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

44. Rolls E: The Brain and Emotion. Oxford, UK, Oxford University Press, 1999Google Scholar

45. Elliott R, Rees G, Dolan RJ: Ventromedial prefrontal cortex mediates guessing. Neuropsychologia 1999; 37:403-411Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

46. Elliott R, Friston KJ, Dolan RJ: Dissociable neural responses in human reward systems. J Neurosci 2000; 20:6159-6165Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

47. Elliott R, Sahakian BJ, Herrod JJ, Robbins TW, Paykel ES: Abnormal response to negative feedback in unipolar depression: evidence for a diagnosis-specific impairment. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1997; 63:74-82Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

48. Elliott R, Sahakian BJ, Michael A, Paykel ES, Dolan RJ: Abnormal neural response to feedback on planning and guessing tasks in patients with unipolar depression. Psychol Med 1998; 28:559-571Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

49. Drevets WC, Videen TO, Price JL, Preskorn SH, Carmichael ST, Raichle ME: A functional anatomical study of unipolar depression. J Neurosci 1992; 12:3628-3641Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

50. Baxter LR Jr, Phelps ME, Mazziotta JC, Guze BH, Schwartz JM, Selin CE: Local cerebral glucose metabolic rates in obsessive-compulsive disorder: a comparison with rates in unipolar depression and in normal controls. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1993; 50:498-501Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

51. Brody AL, Saxena S, Stoessel P, Gillies LA, Fairbanks LA, Alborzian S, Phelps ME, Huang SC, Wu HM, Ho ML, Ho MK, Au SC, Maidment K, Baxter LR Jr: Regional brain metabolic changes in patients with major depression treated with either paroxetine or interpersonal therapy: preliminary findings. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2001; 58:631-640Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

52. Whalen PJ, Bush G, McNally RJ, Wilhelm S, McInerney SC, Jenike MA, Rauch SL: The emotional counting Stroop paradigm: a functional magnetic resonance imaging probe of the anterior cingulate affective division. Biol Psychiatry 1998; 44:1219-1228Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

53. Liotti M, Mayberg HS, Jones VM, Agan LC, Cook CI, Woldorff MG, Jerabek PA, Fox PT: Interactive effects in the anterior cingulate of sadness and selective attention: a PET study (abstract). Biol Psychiatry 2000; 47:125S54Google Scholar

54. Liotti M, Mayberg HS: The role of functional neuroimaging in the neuropsychology of depression. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2001; 23:121-136Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

55. Casey BJ, Forman SD, Franzen P, Berkowitz A, Braver TS, Nystrom LE, Thomas KM, Noll DC: Sensitivity of prefrontal cortex to changes in target probability: a functional MRI study. Hum Brain Mapp 2001; 13:26-33Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

56. Mayberg HS, Westmacott R, McIntosh A: Network analysis of trait and state abnormalities in depression (abstract). Neuroimage 2001; 13:S1071Google Scholar