The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×
Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1176/ajp.156.10.1522

Abstract

OBJECTIVE: The authors investigated the concurrent and predictive validity of the DSM-III-R diagnosis of personality disorder in adolescents by means of baseline and follow-up assessments of inpatients treated at the Yale Psychiatric Institute. METHOD: One hundred sixty-five hospitalized adolescents were reliably assessed by using a structured interview for personality disorder diagnoses as well as two measures of impairment and distress—the Global Assessment of Functioning Scale and the SCL-90-R. Two years after initial assessment, 101 subjects were independently reassessed with the same measures; their functioning was also assessed at this time. RESULTS: At baseline, adolescents with personality disorders were significantly more impaired than those without personality disorders. At follow-up, adolescents with a personality disorder diagnosis at baseline had used significantly more drugs and had required more inpatient treatment during the follow-up interval. Over time, the scores on the Global Assessment of Functioning Scale and SCL-90-R of adolescents diagnosed with a personality disorder at baseline became more similar to the scores of adolescents without a personality disorder. CONCLUSIONS: The diagnosis of personality disorder in adolescent inpatients has good concurrent validity; however, the predictive validity of the diagnosis is mixed.

Despite common clinical use of the construct of personality disorder in adolescent patients (15), the meaning and validity of personality disorder in this group remains uncertain (612). DSM-III-R and DSM-IV both note that personality disorders begin in childhood or adolescence and are often recognizable by adolescence; therefore, they allow for the diagnosis of personality disorders in adolescents if the disturbance is pervasive and persistent and not likely limited to a developmental stage. Despite this tentative acceptance, the nature of personality disorders in child and adolescent patients—and the appropriateness of the construct of personality disorder in this younger population—remains a much-debated topic (3, 6, 814).

There have been two large-scale studies of the concurrent validity of the diagnosis of personality disorder in adolescents in community-based samples. The Toronto Adolescent Longitudinal Study (15) used DSM-III-R criteria to assess 72 subjects from a community sample at ages 13, 16, and 18 years. They found that adolescent personality disorders were associated with high levels of distress and impairment. Similarly, Bernstein et al. (16) used a large community sample to show that adolescents with personality disorders were more likely to have more social impairment, school problems, contacts with the police, and problems at work.

In recent years there has been increased research in adolescent personality disorders using clinical samples—a more relevant population for the assessment of personality pathology (1747). However, most of these studies have examined borderline personality disorder exclusively (1727) or have assessed personality disorders secondary to investigating other disorders (2840). Overall, these studies have found adolescent personality disorders to be associated with elevated levels of depression, anxiety, anger, dissociation, cognitive distortions and impairment, poor self-concept, attempted and completed suicide, poor treatment response, and such personality traits as novelty-seeking, aggression, assaultiveness, neuroticism, low self-confidence, and hopelessness. In addition, these studies show that personality disorders occur at similar frequencies in adolescents and adults and that personality disorders aggregate in the families of adolescents with personality disorders. Limitations of these studies (e.g., small samples, lack of reliability, lack of comparison groups, cohort differences), however, restrict the extent to which meaningful conclusions can be drawn concerning the concurrent validity of the construct of personality disorder in adolescents.

Previous research in adolescents using community samples has suggested that, although personality disorders are prevalent, have concurrent validity, and are stable over 1–2-month periods (46), they are relatively unstable over long periods of time (15, 16). For example, Bernstein et al. (16) found that adolescent personality disorders did not persist over a 2-year interval. Similarly, adolescent personality disorders have been found to be unstable over long periods of time in inpatient samples (20, 27, 46). For example, in a study of 70 adolescent inpatients diagnosed by using structured clinical interviews and reassessed 2 years later, our own research group found low diagnostic stability for personality disorders (46). However, our research group (20) and Meijer et al. (23) found that a number of symptoms of borderline personality disorder were stable over 2-year and 3-year periods, respectively. Moreover, although adolescent personality disorders may be unstable, such diagnoses may nevertheless predict poor outcome or persistent psychopathology (4, 13).

In the present study, we extend our work on the validity of diagnosis in adolescent inpatients (20, 46). In our previous study, we examined stability over a 2-year period. In the current study, we investigate concurrent and predictive validity of personality disorders in the same adolescent study group by comparing subjects who met DSM-III-R criteria for one or more personality disorders with those who did not meet the criteria for any personality disorder. At baseline, we compared groups on clinician-rated and self-rated measures of psychiatric distress and dysfunction. At 2-year follow-up, groups were compared on the same measures—as well as on a functional assessment of work, school, social relations, family relations, alcohol and drug use, and psychiatric symptoms and treatment. To our knowledge, there have not been any studies assessing the concurrent validity or any prospective studies of the predictive validity of adolescent personality disorders using large clinical study groups.

METHOD

Subjects

Subjects were drawn from adolescent inpatients hospitalized at the Yale Psychiatric Institute between 1986 and 1990. At baseline evaluation, there were 165 such subjects, representing nearly all of the adolescent inpatient admissions during this time. Subjects ranged in age from 12 to 18 years (mean=15.5, SD=1.4). Seventy-two (44%) were female and 93 (56%) were male. All subjects were single, and most were middle class. Of the 165 subjects, 142 were eligible for the follow-up study. A detailed description of the study group and procedures for the Yale Psychiatric Institute Adolescent Follow-Up Study is provided elsewhere (46).

At the time that subjects were invited to participate in the study, written informed consent was obtained after all study procedures had been explained. If the subject was a minor at the time of recruitment, assent was obtained from the subject and consent was obtained from the subject’s parents.

Measures

The Personality Disorder Examination (48) is a semistructured diagnostic interview that assesses the presence or absence of all 11 DSM-III-R personality disorders. According to the manual (48), traits are considered present in adolescent subjects if the traits are pervasive and have persisted for at least 3 years. Previous research with the Personality Disorder Examination has found it useful in assessing personality pathology in adolescent subjects (20, 3133, 44, 46).

The Global Assessment of Functioning Scale provides a single global rating of functioning and symptoms. We used a modified version of the Global Assessment of Functioning Scale included in DSM-III-R. In this version, scores range from a low of 1 (e.g., needs constant supervision, serious suicide attempt with clear intent and expectation of death) to a high of 90 (e.g., superior functioning in a wide range of activities, no symptoms).

The SCL-90-R (49) is a 90-item, self-report measure of clinical functioning tapping nine domains of distress and symptoms within the previous 7 days. Subjects rate items on a 5-point scale of distress ranging from 0 (not at all) to 4 (extremely). In addition to the nine symptom dimensions, the SCL-90-R yields three global indexes of distress: the global severity index (the grand total), the positive symptom total (number of symptoms rated 1 or higher), and the positive symptom distress index (grand total/positive symptom total). The global severity index is the best single indicator of current level of distress. We compared groups on raw scores because no standardized, T-score value norms exist for adolescent psychiatric inpatients. Raw scores were computed by summing each item on a factor and dividing by the number of items making up the factor. Thus, each factor score could range from 0 to 4.

The Functional Interview (50) is a 55-question, semistructured, hour-long interview that assesses functioning since discharge. Generally, for our patients the time frame assessed was the follow-up period (about 2 years), although some questions asked about functioning in the past month. Several domains of functioning are assessed by the Functional Interview, including living arrangements since discharge, educational and occupational functioning, social and familial relations, further psychiatric treatment and symptoms, alcohol and drug use, and legal difficulties. These domains were selected a priori by Edell and Dipietro (50), and initial validity for the Functional Interview has been reported elsewhere (51).

Procedures

Baseline assessment

At admission, all subjects received a systematic diagnostic evaluation, which included administration of the Personality Disorder Examination, Global Assessment of Functioning Scale, and SCL-90-R. Assessments were conducted by doctoral and master’s level clinicians who had been trained to a high level of reliability.

Personality disorder diagnoses were established by the best-estimate method. Besides the Personality Disorder Examination interview data, information was obtained from admission notes, medical records, and clinician descriptions. This method was in accordance with the LEAD (longitudinal, expert, all-data) standard advanced by Spitzer (52) and others (53). The results of the SCL-90-R were not used in making diagnoses, and the interviewers were blind to the scores.

We began administering the Personality Disorder Examination when it became available in 1988. Accordingly, it was given to approximately two-thirds of the subjects. Pairs of raters assessed the interrater reliability for 26 subjects; the kappas ranged from 0.65 (for paranoid personality disorder) to 1.00 (for histrionic, avoidant, and passive-aggressive personality disorders) (mean kappa=0.84). These reliabilities are similar to those reported in previous studies using adult subjects (53, 54).

Follow-up assessment

Follow-up assessments included the Personality Disorder Examination, Global Assessment of Functioning Scale, SCL-90-R, and the Functional Interview. These were administered by the clinical research team—none of whom was a baseline interviewer, and all of whom were blind to baseline diagnostic and symptom data. Members of the research team all had at least 1 year of clinical experience (average=3.5 years), and all received 40 hours of intensive training by a doctoral-level psychologist (W.S.E.) over a 4-week period. Weekly training meetings continued throughout the follow-up assessment period. As with the baseline assessments, a series of reliability ratings conducted along with the current Personality Disorder Examination interviews confirmed a high degree of ­interrater reliability of diagnosis. Reliabilities at follow-up were comparable to those at baseline. Pairs of raters also assessed the interrater reliability for 25 subjects for the Functional Interview and Global Assessment of Functioning Scale ratings. The intraclass correlations (ICCs) for the Functional Interview ranged from 0.92 (drug use) to 0.99 (employment level, legal difficulties, psychiatric symptoms, social/romantic relationships, and family relationships) (mean ICC=0.97). For the Global Assessment of Functioning Scale ratings, ICC=0.91 for current ratings and ICC=0.96 for past-year ratings.

RESULTS

Eighty-six (61%) of 142 patients were diagnosed with at least one personality disorder. Borderline personality disorder was the most frequent (N=71), and schizoid and obsessive-compulsive personality disorders were the least frequent (N=2 each). Antisocial personality disorder was not assessed because the vast majority of the subjects did not meet the requirement of being at least 18 years old. Comparisons of the groups with (N=86) and without (N=56) personality disorder showed no differences in demographic variables.

Concurrent Validity

As shown in Table 1, univariate tests at baseline indicate that adolescent inpatients with personality disorders were significantly more impaired than adolescent inpatients with no personality disorder in terms of current and past-year Global Assessment of Functioning Scale scores.

Table 1 also presents a comparison of the two groups’ SCL-90-R scores at baseline. Adolescents with personality disorders scored higher on the somatization, obsessive-compulsive, interpersonal sensitivity, depression, anxiety, hostility, and psychoticism scales—as well as on the global severity index, positive symptom total, and positive symptom distress index scales.

Predictive Validity

We performed a series of repeated measures analyses of variance (ANOVAs) on the Global Assessment of Functioning Scale ratings and the SCL-90-R scores, assessing the effect of the two groups (with versus without personality disorder), the two testing times (baseline versus follow-up), and the group-by-time interaction. In terms of personality disorder diagnosis, there was a main effect for the depression and hostility scores of the SCL-90-R: the adolescents with personality disorders scored significantly higher on both scales. In terms of baseline versus follow-up, there was a significant main effect for the current and past-year Global Assessment of Functioning Scale ratings as well as for the SCL-90-R interpersonal sensitivity, depression, anxiety, phobic anxiety, global severity index, and positive symptom total scales. Trends were also found for the hostility, paranoid ideation, and psychoticism scales of the SCL-90-R between baseline and follow-up. In terms of diagnosis-by-time interaction, a trend was found for both the depression and the positive symptom total scores: the adolescents with personality disorders had a 0.69-point drop on the depression scale and a 17-point drop on the positive symptom total index of the SCL-90-R, but the adolescents with no personality disorders did not have similar changes over time. Thus, over time, the scores of the adolescents with personality disorders became more similar to those of the adolescents with no personality disorders; however, only the changes in the depression and positive symptom total scales approached statistical significance.

A series of one-way (with versus without personality disorders) ANOVAs were performed on the Functional Interview composite scores. The means from these univariate tests are shown in Table 2. The adolescents with personality disorders showed significantly greater dysfunction in the areas of drug use and further psychiatric hospitalizations. There were no differences in functioning, however, with regard to current employment, alcohol abuse, legal difficulties, psychiatric symptoms, or social and family relationships.

DISCUSSION

We examined the concurrent and predictive validity of the personality disorder diagnosis in a large group of hospitalized adolescents by comparing subjects with and without personality disorders on various clinician-rated and self-rated measures of distress and dysfunction. Our study represents a refinement over previous studies in a number of respects. First, the use of a large study group ensured that there was adequate power to detect group effects. Second, we studied both boys and girls, allowing for better generalizability. Third, we used a structured interview with established reliability that applied the DSM-III-R criteria. Fourth, we used validated and reliable measures of impairment and distress (i.e., the Global Assessment of Functioning Scale and the SCL-90-R). Fifth, we included both patient-rated (SCL-90-R) and clinician-rated (Global Assessment of Functioning Scale and Functional Interview) data, assuring method variability. Finally, we examined an inpatient group, for whom the study of personality disorders is particularly relevant to clinical practice.

These advantages notwithstanding, our study group was heterogeneous, with high rates of multiple diagnoses; therefore, it is difficult to associate impairment and distress scores clearly with specific personality disorders. Nevertheless, diagnosing several personality disorders in the same subject is a practice that follows DSM-III-R (and DSM-IV) convention. Moreover, such a study group has ecological validity because it represents a clinical reality. Although we had a large total number of subjects, we did not have sufficient numbers to perform t tests for multiple specific diagnoses. Finally, the fact that our findings are based on an inpatient study group precludes extrapolation to nonhospitalized adolescents with personality disorders.

Concurrent Validity

In general, our findings support the concurrent validity of the diagnosis of personality disorder in adolescent inpatients. At baseline, adolescents with personality disorder diagnoses were more impaired according to their ratings on the Global Assessment of Functioning Scale and on all SCL-90-R scales except for phobic anxiety and paranoid ideation. These findings are consistent with those from studies involving community-based adolescent samples (15, 16). Our findings in adolescent inpatients are also generally consistent with those reported for diverse adult community and outpatient samples, indicating that personality disorders are related to functional impairment (5558).

Predictive Validity

The predictive validity of the adolescent personality disorder construct was modest. We found that the DSM-III-R diagnosis of a personality disorder during adolescence did not predict greater impairment according to the Global Assessment of Functioning Scale or the SCL-90-R. Adolescent personality disorder diagnosis was related to later drug use and to future psychiatric hospitalization, however.

Personality disorder at baseline was associated with greater drug use at follow-up. The co-occurrence of personality disorders—especially borderline personality—and drug use has been well established in adults (5961). It has generally been accepted that patients with personality disorders who abuse drugs do so as a function of their personality (62, 63). Personality disorders, especially cluster B personality disorders, are associated with deficits in affect regulation and with greater impulsivity. Drug users may be attempting to “self-medicate” by modulating affect through unpropitious means (64). Thus, drug use is a way of coping with an array of affects, simultaneously striving to regulate both arousal and negative mood states by artificially managing arousal and by creating positive affect. Although past studies have shown that personality disorders predate substance use, results from this study neither confirm nor disconfirm such findings. Conversely, some investigators have argued that primary problems with drug use create many of the symptoms that constitute personality disorders, or that a subgroup of patients with personality disorders may appear to have personality disorders because of behaviors associated with drug use (59). Our findings suggest that this is an important area for future research.

We also found that the diagnosis of a personality disorder predicted increased use of inpatient psychiatric services during a 2-year follow-up period. This finding offers clear support for the construct validity of the personality disorder diagnosis in adolescence and is consistent with the findings of Bernstein et al. (16), who reported that the personality disorder diagnosis is predictive of psychological distress and functional impairment at follow-up.

The general lack of predictive validity in our study, coupled with other studies that have found personality disorders in adolescents to lack longitudinal stability (16), indicates that personality disorders—at least DSM-III-R personality disorders—may reflect a point-in-time disturbance rather than chronic impairment in adolescents. Adolescence is a time of turbulence and flux, and acute stressors can provoke disruption and regression (20). It may be that symptoms and signs during periods of acute crisis do not reflect the enduring mode of functioning for the individual. During periods of acute crisis, patients may describe themselves as having elevated psychopathology relative to asymptomatic periods (6566). However, there are a number of caveats to that interpretation of our data. It may be that our measures were not sensitive enough to detect subtle differences in functioning at follow-up. For instance, the Global Assessment of Functioning Scale is a relatively gross measure of functioning. Likewise, our follow-up Functional Interview may not have been sensitive to subtle differences in functioning in such ­areas as social relationships, family relationships, or psychiatric symptoms.

However, we found differences between patients with and without personality disorders in both drug use and further psychiatric hospitalizations, which are concrete constructs that are easily defined and measured. Functional assessments were based on self-­report, which are vulnerable to reports of pseudo well-being (67). Additionally, it may have been helpful to use multiple informants in our assessments because some investigators have noted that many patients do not adequately describe their typical personality because they do not possess sufficient insight into their own behavior (68). The literature for adults has also failed to demonstrate impressive stability (58).

Another issue is that ours was not a naturalistic study, in that subjects were drawn from a clinical population and received psychiatric treatment. Insofar as treatment is effective for patients with personality disorders (69), treatment variables may have confounded findings. Thus, some of the lack of predictive validity may be due to the beneficial effects of treatment. Data from clinical populations are necessary to advance the understanding of developmental psychopathology (70, 71); however, the lack of predictive validity in treated study groups cannot—without epidemiologic validation—be generalized to the population as a whole (72).

Another possibility is that alternative conceptualizations may be more useful to understanding adolescent personality disorders (1, 3) and that alternative assessment methods may prove useful as well. For example, dimensional approaches to the assessment of adolescent personality disorders may have more validity than categorical classification (73). Indeed, a number of studies have found better predictive validity in adolescents with disruptive behavior disorders when using dimensional approaches than when using categorical approaches (74).

A final issue that may affect the limited predictive validity of DSM-III-R adolescent personality disorders is DSM-III-R’s lack of attention to the developmental complexities of this stage (75, 76). Although DSM-III-R does note that personality disorders have an onset in adolescence or early adulthood, DSM-III-R personality disorder diagnoses rely on symptoms and signs originally designed for adults. Adolescent personality disorders may be phenomenologically similar to adult personality disorders (1), but it is unlikely that they would be completely isomorphic with adult criteria. Therefore, adolescent personality disorders may require a classification system that incorporates a developmental perspective and considers how personality disorders derive from disruptions in normal developmental processes (77). Examining this broader picture of developmental psychopathology in adolescence allows for the examination of heterotypic continuity and helps determine the construct validity of adolescent personality disorders (78).

CONCLUSIONS

Our findings suggest that, although DSM-III-R-defined personality disorders in adolescent inpatients have good concurrent validity, important questions remain concerning the predictive validity of such diagnoses in this population. Future research should consider broader conceptions of personality functioning and should incorporate a developmental perspective as well as dimensional approaches.

Presented in part at the 146th annual meeting of the American Psychiatric Association, San Francisco, May 22–27, 1993. Received Dec. 26, 1996; revisions received Dec. 17, 1998, and March 23, 1999; accepted April 13, 1999. From the Yale Psychiatric Institute and the Department of Psychiatry, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, Conn. Address reprint requests to Mr. Levy, Department of Psychology, New York Presbyterian Hospital-Weill Medical College, Cornell University, Westchester Division, 21 Bloomingdale Rd., White Plains, NY 10605; (e-mail). The authors thank David Greenfeld for institutional support as well as Diana Bowling, Pamela Harding, Steven Joy, Helen Sayward, Rachel Yehuda, and Kathy Zampano for help in conducting the baseline assessments and Diane Bacon, Robert Cubeta, Eric Hancock, Terry Harran, Foluke Morris, and Jeffrey Muller for help in conducting follow-up assessments.

TABLE 1
TABLE 2

References

1. Kernberg O: The diagnosis of borderline conditions in adolescence. Adolesc Psychiatry 1978; 6:298–312MedlineGoogle Scholar

2. Kernberg PF: Resolved: borderline personality exists in children under twelve: affirmative. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1990; 29:479–480Google Scholar

3. Masterson J: The borderline adolescent. Adolesc Psychiatry 1973; 2:240–268Google Scholar

4. Petti TA, Vela RM: Borderline disorders of childhood: an overview. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1990; 29:327–337Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

5. Robson K: The Borderline Child. New York, McGraw-Hill, 1983Google Scholar

6. Behar D, Stewart MA: Aggressive conduct disorder of children. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1982; 65:210–220Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

7. Behar D, Stewart MA: The borderline diagnosis for children (letter). Am J Psychiatry 1987; 144:1108LinkGoogle Scholar

8. Famularo R, Kinscherff R, Fenton T: Posttraumatic stress disorder among children clinically diagnosed as borderline personality disorder. J Nerv Ment Dis 1991; 42:41–53Google Scholar

9. Gualtieri CT, Van Bourgondien ME: So-called borderline children (letter). Am J Psychiatry 1987; 144:832LinkGoogle Scholar

10. Gualtieri CT, Koriath U, Van Bourgondien ME: “Borderline” children. J Autism Dev Disord 1983; 13:67–72Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

11. Kutcher SP, Korenblum M: Borderline personality disorder in adolescents: a critical review, novel speculations, and suggested future directions, in Handbook of Borderline Disorders. Edited by Silver D, Rosenbluth M. Madison, Conn, International Universities Press, 1992, pp 535–552Google Scholar

12. Shapiro T: Resolved: borderline personality exists in children under twelve: negative. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1990; 29:480–482MedlineGoogle Scholar

13. Lofgren DP, Bemporad J, King J, Lindem K, O’Driscoll G: A prospective follow-up study of so-called borderline children. Am J Psychiatry 1991; 148:1541–1547Google Scholar

14. Paris J: Borderline Personality Disorder: A Multidimensional Approach. Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Press, 1994Google Scholar

15. Korenblum M, Marton P, Golembeck H, Stein B: Personality status: changes through adolescence. Adolescence 1990; 13:389–399Google Scholar

16. Bernstein DP, Cohen P, Velez CN, Schwab-Stone M, Siever LJ, Shinsato L: Prevalence and stability of DSM-III-R personality disorders in a community-based survey of adolescents. Am J Psychiatry 1993; 150:1237–1243Google Scholar

17. Atlas JA: Association between history of abuse and borderline personality disorder for hospitalized adolescent girls. Psychol Rep 1995; 77(3, part 2):1346Google Scholar

18. Atlas JA, Wolfson MA: Depression and dissociation as features of borderline personality disorder in hospitalized adolescents. Psychol Rep 1996; 78:624–626Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

19. Bradley SJ: The relationship of early maternal separation to borderline personality in children and adolescents: a pilot study. Am J Psychiatry 1979; 136:424–426MedlineGoogle Scholar

20. Garnet KE, Levy KN, Mattanah JJF, Edell WS, McGlashan TH: Borderline personality disorder in adolescents: ubiquitous or specific? Am J Psychiatry 1994; 151:1380–1382Google Scholar

21. Ludolph PS, Westen D, Misle B, Jackson A, Wixom J, Wiss FC: The borderline diagnosis in adolescents: symptoms and developmental history. Am J Psychiatry 1990; 147:470–476LinkGoogle Scholar

22. McManus M, Lerner H, Robbins D, Barbour C: Assessment of borderline symptomatology in hospitalized adolescents. J Am Acad Child Psychiatry 1984; 23:685–694Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

23. Meijer M, Goedhart AW, Treffers PDA: The persistence of borderline personality disorder in adolescence. J Personal Disord 1998; 12:13–22Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

24. Pinto A, Grapentine WL, Francis G, Picariello CM: Borderline personality disorder in adolescents: affective and cognitive features. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1996; 35:1338–1343Google Scholar

25. Westen D, Ludolph P, Block MJ, Wixom J, Wiss FC: Developmental history and object relations in psychiatrically disturbed adolescent girls. Am J Psychiatry 1990; 147:1061–1068Google Scholar

26. Westen D, Ludolph P, Silk K, Kellam A, Gold L, Lohr N: Object relations in borderline adolescents and adults: developmental differences. Adolesc Psychiatry 1990; 17:360–384MedlineGoogle Scholar

27. Wixom J, Ludolph P, Westen D: The quality of depression in adolescents with borderline personality disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1993; 32:1172–1177Google Scholar

28. Burket RC, Myers WC: Axis I and personality comorbidity in adolescents with conduct disorder. Bull Am Acad Psychiatry Law 1995; 23:73–82MedlineGoogle Scholar

29. Eppright TD, Kashani JH, Robison BD, Reid JC: Comorbidity of conduct disorder and personality disorders in an incarcerated juvenile population. Am J Psychiatry 1993; 150:1233–1236Google Scholar

30. Friedman RC, Clarkin JF, Corn R, Aronoff MS, Hurt SW, Murphy MC: DSM-III and affective pathology in adolescents. J Nerv Ment Dis 1982; 170:511–521Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

31. Grilo CM, Becker DF, Walker ML, Levy KN, Edell WS, McGlashan TH: Comorbidity of adolescent substance users. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1995; 34:1085–1091Google Scholar

32. Grilo CM, Levy KN, Becker DF, Edell WS, McGlashan TH: Comorbidity of DSM-III-R axis I and II disorders among female inpatients with eating disorders. Psychiatr Serv 1996; 47:426–429LinkGoogle Scholar

33. Kutcher SP, Marton P, Korenblum M: Adolescent bipolar illness and personality disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1990; 29:355–358Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

34. Marton P, Korenblum M, Kutcher S, Stein B, Kennedy B, Pakes J: Personality dysfunction in depressed adolescents. Can J Psychiatry 1989; 34:810–813Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

35. Marttunen MJ, Aro HM, Henriksson MM, Loonnquist JK: Mental disorders in adolescent suicide: DSM-III-R axes I and II diagnoses in suicides among 13- to 19-year-olds in Finland. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1991; 48:834–839Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

36. McManus M, Alessi NE, Grapentine WL, Brickman A: Psychiatric disturbances in serious delinquents. J Am Acad Child Psychiatry 1984; 23:602–615Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

37. Myers WC, Burket RC, Otto TA: Conduct disorder and personality disorders in hospitalized adolescents. J Clin Psychiatry 1993; 54:21–26MedlineGoogle Scholar

38. Myers WC, Burket RC, Harris HE: Adolescent psychopathology in relation to delinquent behaviors, conduct disorder, and personality disorders. J Forensic Sci 1995; 40:435–439MedlineGoogle Scholar

39. Runeson B, Rich C: Diagnostic comorbidity of mental disorders among suicides. Int Rev Psychiatry 1992; 4:197–203CrossrefGoogle Scholar

40. Westen D, Moses MJ, Silk KR, Lohr NE, Cohen R, Segal H: Quality of depressive experiences in borderline personality disorder and major depression: when depression is not just depression. J Personal Disord 1992; 6:382–393CrossrefGoogle Scholar

41. Brent DA, Zelenak JP, Bukstein O, Brown RV: Reliability and validity of the structured interview for personality disorders in adolescents. J Am Acad Child Psychiatry 1990; 29:349–354CrossrefGoogle Scholar

42. Brent DA, Johnson B, Bartle S: Personality disorder, tendency to impulsive violence, and suicidal behavior in adolescents. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1993; 32:69–75Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

43. Brent DA, Johnson BA, Perper J, Connolly J, Bridge J, Bartle S, Rather C: Personality disorder, personality traits, impulsive violence, and completed suicide in adolescents. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1994; 33:1080–1086Google Scholar

44. Grilo CM, McGlashan TH, Quinlan DM, Walker ML, Greenfeld D, Edell WS: Frequency of personality disorders in two age cohorts of psychiatric inpatients. Am J Psychiatry 1998; 155:140–142LinkGoogle Scholar

45. Johnson BA, Brent DA, Connolly J, Bridge J, Matta J, Constantine D, Rather C, White T: Familial aggregation of adolescent personality disorders. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1995; 34:798–804Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

46. Mattanah JJF, Becker DF, Levy KN, Edell WS, McGlashan TH: Diagnostic stability in adolescents followed up 2 years after hospitalization. Am J Psychiatry 1995; 152:889–894LinkGoogle Scholar

47. Johnson JG, Hyler SE, Skodol AE, Bornstein RF, Sherman M: Personality disorder symptomatology associated with adolescent depression and substance abuse. J Personal Disord 1995; 9:318–329CrossrefGoogle Scholar

48. Loranger AW, Susman VL, Oldham JM, Russakoff M: The Personality Disorder Examination (PDE) Manual. Yonkers, NY, DV Communications, 1988Google Scholar

49. Derogatis LR: SCL-90-R: Administration, Scoring, and Procedures Manual, II. Towson, Md, Clinical Psychometric Research, 1983Google Scholar

50. Edell WS, Dipietro S: Functional Interview. New Haven, Conn, Yale University, 1987Google Scholar

51. Edell WS, Hoffman RE, Dipietro SA, Harcherik DF: Effects of long-term psychiatric hospitalization for young, treatment-refractory patients. Hosp Community Psychiatry 1990; 41:780–785AbstractGoogle Scholar

52. Spitzer RL: Psychiatric diagnosis: are clinicians still necessary? Compr Psychiatry 1983; 24:399–411Google Scholar

53. Pilkonis PA, Heape CL, Ruddy J, Serrao P: Validity in the diagnosis of personality disorders: the use of the LEAD standard. Psychol Assessment 1991; 3:46–54CrossrefGoogle Scholar

54. Standage K, Ladha N: An examination of the reliability of the Personality Disorder Examination and a comparison with other methods of identifying personality disorders in a clinical sample. J Personal Disord 1988; 2:267–271CrossrefGoogle Scholar

55. Drake RE, Vaillant GE: A validity study of axis II of DSM-III. Am J Psychiatry 1985; 142:553–558LinkGoogle Scholar

56. Zimmerman M, Coryell W: DSM-III personality disorder diagnoses in a nonpatient sample: demographic correlates and comorbidity. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1989; 46:682–689Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

57. Nakao K, Gunderson JG, Phillips KA, Tanaka N, Yorifuji K, Takaishi J, Nishimura T: Functional impairment in personality disorders. J Personal Disord 1992; 6:24–33CrossrefGoogle Scholar

58. Grilo CM, McGlashan TH, Oldham JM: Course and stability of personality disorders. J Practical Psychiatry and Behavioral Health 1998; 4:61–75CrossrefGoogle Scholar

59. Dulit RA, Fyer MR, Haas GL, Sullivan T, Frances AJ: Substance use in borderline personality disorder. Am J Psychiatry 1990; 147:1002–1007Google Scholar

60. Grilo CM, Martino S, Walker ML, Becker DF, Edell WS, McGlashan TH: Controlled study of psychiatric comorbidity in psychiatrically hospitalized young adults with substance abuse disorders. Am J Psychiatry 1997; 154:1305–1307Google Scholar

61. Zanarini MC, Gunderson JG, Frankenberg FR: Axis I phenomenology of borderline personality disorder. Compr Psychiatry 1989; 30:149–156Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

62. Shedler J, Block J: Adolescent drug use and psychological health: a longitudinal inquiry. Am Psychol 1990; 45:612–630Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

63. Windle M: A longitudinal study of antisocial behaviors in early adolescence as predictors of late adolescent substance use: gender and ethnic group differences. J Abnorm Psychol 1990; 26:86–91CrossrefGoogle Scholar

64. Khantzian EJ: The self-medication hypothesis of addictive disorders: focus on heroin and cocaine dependence. Am J Psychiatry 1985; 142:1259–1264Google Scholar

65. Hirschfeld RMA, Klerman GL, Clayton PJ, Keller MB, McDonald-Scott P, Larkin BH: Assessing personality: effects of depressive state on trait measurement. Am J Psychiatry 1983; 140:695–699LinkGoogle Scholar

66. Reich J: Instruments measuring DSM-III and DSM-III-R personality disorders. J Personal Disord 1987; 1:220–240CrossrefGoogle Scholar

67. Shedler J, Maymen M, Manis M: The illusion of mental health. Am Psychol 1993; 48:1117–1131Google Scholar

68. Zimmerman M, Pfohl B, Stangl D, Corenthal C: Assessment of DSM-III personality disorders: the importance of interviewing an informant. J Clin Psychiatry 1986; 47:261–263MedlineGoogle Scholar

69. Sanislow C, McGlashan TH: Treatment outcome of personality disorders. Can J Psychiatry 1998; 43:237–250Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

70. Cicchetti D: Perspective on the interface between normal and atypical development. Dev Psychopathol 1991; 2:329–333CrossrefGoogle Scholar

71. Sroufe LA, Rutter M: The domain of developmental psychopathology. Child Dev 1984; 83:173–189Google Scholar

72. Costello EJ: Developments in child psychiatric epidemiology. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1989; 28:836–841Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

73. Widiger TA: Categorical versus dimensional classification: implications from and for research. J Personal Disord 1992; 6:287–300CrossrefGoogle Scholar

74. Hinshaw SP, Lahey BB, Hart EL: Issues of taxonomy and comorbidity in the development of conduct disorder. Dev Psychopathol 1993; 5:31–49CrossrefGoogle Scholar

75. Cicchetti D: Developmental psychopathology in infancy: illustration from the study of maltreated youngsters. J Consult Clin Psychol 1987; 55:837–845Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

76. Rutter M, Shaffer D: DSM-III: a step forward or back in terms of the classification of child psychiatric disorders? J Am Acad Child Psychiatry 1980; 19:371–394Google Scholar

77. Rutter M: Temperament, personality and personality disorder. Br J Psychiatry 1987; 150:443–458Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

78. Rutter M: Pathways from childhood to adult life. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1989; 30:23–51Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar