The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×
Published Online:

Abstract

Objective: Synchronization of neural activity preceding self-generated actions may reflect the operation of the forward model, which acts to dampen sensations resulting from those actions. If this is true, pre-action synchrony should be related to subsequent sensory suppression. Deficits in this mechanism may be characteristic of schizophrenia and related to positive symptoms, such as auditory hallucinations. If so, schizophrenia patients should have reduced neural synchrony preceding movements, especially patients with severe hallucinations. Method: In 24 patients with schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder and 25 healthy comparison subjects, the authors related prespeech neural synchrony to subsequent auditory cortical responsiveness to the spoken sound, compared prespeech neural synchrony in schizophrenia patients and healthy comparison subjects, and related prespeech neural synchrony to auditory hallucination severity in patients. To assess neural synchrony, phase coherence of single-trial EEG preceding talking was calculated at a single site across repeated trials. To assess auditory cortical suppression, the N1 event-related brain potentials to speech sound onset during talking and listening were compared. Results: In healthy comparison subjects, prespeech neural synchrony was related to subsequent suppression of responsiveness to the spoken sound, as reflected in reduction of N1 during talking relative to listening. There was greater prespeech synchrony in comparison subjects than in patients, especially those with severe auditory hallucinations. Conclusions: These data suggest that EEG synchrony preceding speech reflects the action of a forward model system, which dampens auditory responsiveness to self-generated speech and is deficient in patients who hallucinate.

Neuroscientists traditionally thought that information processing was revealed by changes in firing patterns of “smart” neurons in a bottom-up fashion (1) . Such a conceptualization is relatively hard wired (2) and fails to reflect the flexibility needed to cope with top-down constraints, such as attention, expectations, and context. Constraints on bottom-up processing come from high-level executive systems, such as conscious focusing of attention, but can also come from automatically invoked, lower-level systems. A forward model system involving transmission of an “efference copy” of motor commands to the sensory cortex to generate “corollary discharges” that prepare it for impending sensory consequences of self-initiated motor acts can help us unconsciously disregard sensations resulting from our own actions. Helmholtz (3) first described the need for a mechanism that would allow us to discriminate between moving objects and movements on the retina resulting from eye movements. Von Holst and Mittelstaedt (4) and Sperry (5) later suggested that a motor action is accompanied by an efference copy of the action that produces a corollary discharge in the sensory cortex. Subsequently, this feed-forward mechanism has been described in numerous other systems, including the auditory system, where it serves to suppress auditory cortical responses to speech sounds as they are being spoken (6 , 7) . This may result from partial cancellation of sensation by the corollary discharge, which represents the expected sound of our own speech. Indeed, auditory cortical suppression is not as evident when the speech sound is artificially distorted as it is spoken (8) .

The transmission of an efference copy to the appropriate sensory cortex may be an emergent property of a self-organizing system, accomplished by synchronization of oscillatory activity among distributed neuronal assemblies (1) . The specific frequency of synchronous oscillations may identify neural populations as belonging to the same functional network of spatially distributed neuronal assemblies (9) . If the forward model mechanism involves self-coordinated communication between motor and sensory systems, enhancement of neural synchrony should be evident before execution of motor acts, such as talking. Consistent with this hypothesis, local field potential recordings from somatosensory cells in rats showed neural synchrony that preceded exploratory whisking in both 7–12 Hz (10) and 30–35 Hz (11) bands. Hamada and colleagues (11) suggested that neural oscillations might be triggered by transfer of an efference copy of motor preparation to the somatosensory cortex, happening several hundred milliseconds before the action and seen as oscillations phase-locked to it.

Time-frequency analyses of human EEG, time-locked to specific events, now allow us to measure phase synchrony on a millisecond time scale to investigate integrated neural systems and their compromise in complex neuropsychiatric disorders such as schizophrenia. Dysfunctional regional coordination, communication, or connectivity (12) , possibly associated with deficient synchronization of neuronal oscillations (2 , 1315) , may be responsible for a wide range of schizophrenia symptoms (2) . Intertrial coherence (16) is a measure of phase synchronization of neural oscillations across individual event-locked EEG epochs, reflecting the degree to which a particular type of stimulus is associated with changes in phase synchrony of ongoing oscillations at specific frequencies. With intertrial coherence, millisecond-by-millisecond changes in phase synchrony can be assessed independent of changes in EEG power. Intertrial coherence was described by Tallon-Baudry et al. (17) as a “phase-locking factor.” Intertrial coherence can also be thought of as “temporal coherence” and is different from “spatial coherence,” which is calculated between different brain regions or electrode sites (see reference 18). Theta band (4–7 Hz) oscillation and synchrony may be involved in the mechanisms of sensorimotor integration and provide voluntary motor systems with continually updated feedback on performance (19) .

In addition to dampening irrelevant sensations resulting from our own actions, the forward model provides a mechanism for automatic distinction between internally and externally generated percepts across sensory modalities and may even operate in the realm of covert thoughts, which have been viewed as our most complex motor act (20) . Failures of this mechanism may contribute to self-monitoring deficits and auditory verbal hallucinations characteristic of schizophrenia (21) . Specifically, if an efference copy of a thought, memory, or other inner experiences does not produce a corollary discharge of the expected auditory consequences, internally generated percepts may be experienced as having an external source.

Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence for dysfunction of the forward model system in schizophrenia is growing and extends to auditory, visual, and somatosensory modalities. With the N1 component of the event-related potential, we showed that the auditory cortical response dampening observed in healthy comparison subjects during talking was not evident in patients with schizophrenia. Lindner et al. (22) reported that nondelusional patients were better able to perceive a stable environment during eye movements than were delusional patients, suggesting that delusions might be due to a specific deficit in the perceptual cancellation of sensory consequences of one’s own actions (e.g., references 21 and 23 ). Deficits in the self-monitoring of action may also underlie the failure of schizophrenia patients to correct action errors when only proprioceptive feedback is available (24) . Shergill and colleagues (25) used a motor force-matching task and demonstrated sensory attenuation of self-produced stimulation. Patients with schizophrenia exhibited significantly decreased attenuation of the resulting sensation, and the authors suggested that this was due to a failure of self-monitoring and faulty internal predictions.

Current Approach

Our primary goal was to quantify the neural correlates of the hypothesized efference copy associated with speaking, to assess group differences in this neural signal, and to relate it to auditory hallucinations. We predicted that there would be a larger signal preceding speaking than listening and that this difference would be reduced in patients, especially those with severe auditory hallucinations.

Our secondary goal was to validate neural synchrony as a reflection of the corollary discharge by relating its strength to subsequent cortical responsiveness. In a similar analysis of self-paced button-press data (unpublished report by Ford et al.), we suggested that synchrony of neural activity preceding the press reflected the corollary discharge from the motor to the sensory cortex. Although we predicted that the strength of the corollary discharge would be related to suppression of the subsequent cortical response to the button press, we did not have a good measure of postpress response suppression. Here we used the difference in N1 amplitude during talking and listening, as we have done previously (26) . We predicted that the strength of the prespeech corollary discharge would be directly related to suppression of N1 to speech onset during talking compared to listening.

Our final goal was to confirm our earlier findings (26) of a reduction in N1 suppression to speech-onset patients with schizophrenia but with a left hemisphere locus (27) .

Methods and Materials

Participants

EEG data were acquired from 24 patients (four women) and 25 healthy comparison subjects (six women). All gave written informed consent after the procedures had been fully described. The patients and comparison subjects were matched for age and parental socioeconomic status. Demographic and clinical data are summarized in Table 1 .

The patients were recruited from community mental health centers as well as from inpatient and outpatient services of the Veterans Affairs Health Care System in Palo Alto, Calif., and San Francisco. All patients were taking stable doses of antipsychotic medications and met DSM-IV (28) criteria for schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder either based on the diagnosis from the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID [29] ) conducted by a psychiatrist or psychologist or by consensus of a SCID interview conducted by a trained research assistant and a clinical interview by a psychiatrist or psychologist. The patients were excluded if they met DSM-IV criteria for alcohol or drug abuse within 30 days before the study. In addition, patient and comparison participants were excluded for significant head injury, neurological disorders, or other medical illnesses compromising the CNS. Symptoms during the last week were rated with the Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS) (30) , the Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms (SAPS) (31) , and the Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale (BPRS) (32) by two (and sometimes three) independent raters attending the same rating session.

The comparison subjects were recruited by newspaper advertisements and word of mouth, screened with the telephone with questions from the SCID nonpatient screening module (29) , and excluded for any history of axis I psychiatric illness.

Task Design

The subjects uttered “ah” while a cue (yellow X) was on the screen (1.66 seconds). This was repeated five times in each talk block. For comparison with functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) data collected in a different session (data to be presented separately), each talk block was followed by a rest block in which the subjects saw a sequence of five black cues instead of five yellow cues. The subjects were instructed to watch the screen during the rest block. “TALK” appeared at the beginning of each talk block and “REST” at the beginning of each rest block. Each talk-rest pair of blocks was repeated six times. At the end of six repeats, “END” appeared on the screen. All visual stimuli were uppercase against a blue background.

During the talk task, utterances were recorded for playback during the listen task. During the listen task, the visual display was similar to the talk task; cues were seen as before, but the instruction “TALK” was replaced with “LISTEN.” Also, for comparison with the fMRI data, magnetic resonance noise from the clustered acquisition sequence was played between the cues such that it was absent when speech was produced or played back.

The subjects were trained to produce uniform, brisk utterances with minimal tongue, jaw, and throat movements. Before data acquisition, the subjects uttered “ah” several times to facilitate sound system calibration and acclimation to the environment.

Instrumentation

An audio presentation system (Reaktor, Native Instruments, Berlin, Germany) allowed us to detect the subject’s vocalization and to amplify and play it back through headphones essentially in real time. The same program rectified and low-pass filtered incoming audio signals, detected the rising edge of the rectified and filtered signal, generated a trigger pulse, and inserted it into the EEG data collection system.

EEG/Event-Relation Potential Acquisition

EEG data were acquired (0.05–100 Hz band-pass filter, 1000 Hz analogue-to-digital conversion rate) from 27 sites referenced to the nose (F7, F3, Fz, F4, F8, FT7, FC3, FCz, FC4, FT8, T5, C3, Cz, C4, T6, TP7, CP3, CPz, CP4, TP8, P7, P3, Pz, P4, P8, Tp9, and Tp10). During preprocessing, data were rereferenced to the mastoid electrodes (TP9 and TP10). Additional electrodes were placed on the outer canthi of both eyes and above and below the right eye to measure eye movements and blinks (vertical and horizontal electro-oculogram). EEG data were separated into 500-msec epochs and time-locked to onset of the first speech sound on every trial. Trials containing artifacts (voltages exceeding 100 μV) were rejected, and then vertical electro-oculogram and horizontal electro-oculogram data were used to correct EEGs for eye movements and blinks in a regression-based algorithm (33) . EEG epochs for these analyses spanned 500 msec, centered on speech onset.

EEG Analysis

Intertrial coherence analysis was implemented in EEGLAB (34) . Intertrial coherence provided frequency- and time-specific measures of cross-trial phase synchrony with respect to the button-press onset. Intertrial coherence values were calculated with a moving fast Fourier transform window, which was 64 msec wide. This window was applied 200 times to each epoch, producing as many intertrial coherence data points. Data were extracted in 1-msec increments. Although the 64-msec window gave us excellent temporal resolution, we could not unambiguously resolve frequencies below 15.625 Hz because one complete cycle of a 15.625 Hz signal lasts 64 msec. Because the forward model signal is likely to be brief, we focused our analysis on the data derived from the 64-msec window and sacrificed precision in the frequency domain. Thus, the first bin in our analysis was 15.625 Hz, with the next at 31.25 Hz. It is important to note that nearby unresolved frequencies can influence a fast Fournier transform measure. A Hanning window was applied to each 64-msec section of data to minimize such frequency leakage, but this is not a perfect remedy (35) . Thus, data extracted at 15.625 Hz includes contributions from adjacent frequencies.

Event-Related Potential Analysis

Before identification of N1, data were band-pass filtered between 2 and 8 Hz to optimize measurement of N1. N1 was identified as the most negative point between 50 and 175 msec after speech onset. The voltage at that point was measured relative to a prespeech baseline (–100 to 0 msec).

Statistical Analysis of EEG Data

Intertrial coherence time-frequency plots for comparison subjects ( Figure 1 ) showed increased phase synchrony between –150 and 0 msec before speech onset. To reduce the number of statistical tests, we focused on the slow EEG band at FCz. Data were subjected to a three-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) for group (comparison subjects versus patients), condition (talk versus listen), and time (six bins of 25 msec spanning 150 msec preceding speech onset). Interactions were parsed with follow-up ANOVAs (36) . Greenhouse-Geisser correction for nonsphericity was used, as appropriate. We used a p<0.05 level of significance.

Figure 1. Intertrial Coherence Plots for Healthy Comparison Subjects and Schizophrenia Patients for FCz During Talking and Listening Conditions a

a EEG frequency is indicated on the y axis and spans 0–60 Hz. Time is indicated on the x axis and spans –225 to 225 msec. Speech sound onset occurred at 0 msec. Intertrial coherence 150 msec before speech onset was greater for talking than listening in the 15.62 Hz band. This region is boxed for both talking and listening for both groups.

Statistical Analysis of Event-Related Potential Data

N1 peak amplitudes off midline were assessed in a four-way ANOVA for the between-subjects factor of group and the within-subjects factors of condition (talk versus listen), hemisphere (left versus right), laterality (far from midline, closer to midline, and closest to midline), and caudality (frontal, frontal-central, central, central-parietal, and parietal). Data from the following electrodes were used in this analysis: left frontal: F7, F5, F3; right frontal: F8, F6, F4; left frontal-central: FT7, FC5, FC3; right frontal-central: FT8, FC6, FC4; left-central: T3, C5, C3; right-central: T4, C6, C4; left central-parietal: TP7, CP5, CP3; right central-parietal: TP8, CP6, CP4; left-parietal: T5, P5, P3; right-parietal: T6, P6, and P4.

Correlations

Symptoms With EEG

To establish the specificity of the relationship between hallucinatory behavior (the BPRS) and prespeech asynchrony, hallucinatory behavior was entered into a multiple regression model with avolition/apathy (SANS) to predict prespeech intertrial coherence at FCz for each of the six 25-msec epochs. Bonferroni corrections for multiple comparisons require p<0.008 for testing six models in this family.

EEG and Event-Related Potentials

The intertrial coherence values before talking at FCz were correlated with the N1 amplitude suppression during talking at FCz (talk – listen). A larger positive value of the N1 talk – listen difference reflected more suppression. Not all subjects showed suppression during talking; indeed, many patients and some comparison subjects have negative values. Each of the six time bins was entered into one multiple regression analysis for comparison subjects and one for patients. Bonferroni corrections for multiple comparisons require p<0.025 for testing two models in this family.

Results

EEG

As can be seen in Figure 1 , there was greater intertrial coherence preceding talking than listening (talk/listen: F=31.55, df=1, 47, p<0.0001) and a talk/listen-by-group interaction (F=6.91, df=1, 47 p<0.02). Although the talk-listen effect was stronger in comparison subjects (F=29.26, df=1, 24, p<0.0001), it was also significant in patients F=5.43, df=1, 23, p<0.03). There was also a time-by-group interaction (F=2.53, df=5, 235, p<0.05), but the effect of time was not significant in either comparison subjects (p=0.28) or patients (p=0.28). Inspection of data in Figure 1 suggested that much earlier in the prespeech epoch the group difference was reduced or reversed. We specifically addressed whether patients had greater synchrony between –225 and –200 msec preceding vocalization, and, indeed, we found no talk/listen-by-group interaction (p=0.31), although there was greater synchrony before talking than listening (F=11.91, df=1, 47, p<0.001).

EEG and Hallucinations

When both hallucination severity and avolition/apathy were entered into a multiple regression analysis to control for the effects of one on the other, only hallucination severity was related to synchrony. A scatterplot of the inverse relationship between prespeech intertrial coherence and hallucination severity (residualized on avolition/apathy) can be seen in Figure 2 . The scalp topography map shows that the strongest correlations between hallucinations and prespeech synchrony are at frontal-central sites.

Figure 2. Scatterplot Showing Relationship Between Prespeech Intertrial Coherence at FCz Occurring Between –125 and –100 msec and Hallucinatory Behavior a

a Standardized residuals are for hallucinatory behavior after removal of shared variance with avolition and apathy. On the right is a scalp topography map of the correlation coefficients between hallucination severity (standardized residuals) and prespeech intertrial coherence from –125 to –100 msec. Darker colors indicate stronger negative correlations, with the strongest correlations at frontal-central midline sites.

Event-Related Potentials

N1 amplitude to speech sound onset was affected by a talk/listen-by-group interaction (F=4.4, df=1, 47, p<0.05). In comparison subjects, N1 amplitude during talking (–1.62 μV 1.26) was suppressed compared to listening (–2.04 μV 0.85). In patients, N1 amplitude during talking (–2.05 μV 1.1) was not suppressed compared to listening (–1.81 μV 0.88). Although we predicted that this effect would be larger over the left than the right hemisphere (27) , neither the talk/listen-by-hemisphere effect (F=0.063, df=1, 47, p=0.82) nor the talk/listen-by-group-by-hemisphere interaction (F=0.663, df=1, 47, p=0.42) was significant.

EEG/Event-Related Potential Correlations

We suggested that intertrial coherence preceding speech might be a reflection of the efference copy signal sent from speech production areas to the auditory cortex, where the resulting corollary discharge prepares auditory processing areas for the imminent arrival of the speech sound about to be produced. If so, efference copy/corollary discharge strength preceding speech should be related to the degree of suppression of auditory cortical responsiveness to the speech sound. To assess that association, we correlated N1 suppression at FCz with prespeech intertrial coherence at FCz. After we controlled for the contribution of intertrial coherence from the other time bins, intertrial coherence between –100 and –75 msec before speech onset was strongly related to N1 suppression ( Figure 3 ) in comparison subjects but not patients ( Table 2 ).

Figure 3. Event-Related Potentials From FCz, Locked to Speech Sound Onset, During Talking and Listening for Comparison Subjects and Schizophrenia Patients a

a Amplitude is on the y axis, and time is on the x axis. Negativity is plotted down. The N1 component is noted with arrows. N1 suppression during talking was estimated as the difference between N1 to speech onset during talking and listening (N1[talking] – N1[listening]), such that positive values reflect more suppression. Scatterplots for healthy comparison subjects and schizophrenia patients show relationships between N1 suppression at FCz and intertrial coherence at FCz, measured –100 to –75 msec before speaking, after removal of shared variance from the other time epochs.

Sound Quality

The intensity of sounds did not differ between groups (p=0.88), but the patients (290 137 msec) tended to produce sounds of longer duration than the comparison subjects (226 74 msec; F=4.18, df=1, 48, p<0.05). In the comparison subjects, prespeech synchrony presaged sound quality: those who generated more prespeech synchrony between –100 and –75 msec (r=0.41, p=0.04) and between –75 and –50 msec (r=0.43, p=0.03) spoke sounds of longer duration later, and those who generated more prespeech synchrony between –50 and –25 msec spoke louder later. These relationships were not significant in the patients. However, in the patients, synchrony and sound quality were related in the moment that sounds were uttered; sounds of longer duration were associated with more neural synchrony between 0 and 25 msec (r=0.43, p<0.03), and more intense sounds were associated with more neural synchrony between 100 and 125 msec (r=0.43, p<0.04).

Discussion

With time-frequency decomposition of EEG recorded during a simple vocalization task, we examined phase synchrony of neural oscillations preceding speech onset. We found that both healthy comparison subjects and patients with schizophrenia showed an increase in phase synchrony during the 150 msec preceding an utterance in the lower frequency range studied. This is consistent with increased neural synchronization preceding whisking in the 7–10 Hz range (10) and the 30–35 Hz range (11) in rats. Like Hamada et al. (11) , we suggest that this premovement burst of synchronous neural activity is a reflection of the forward model preparing the CNS for the sensory consequences of its own actions. Consistent with prespeech synchrony reflecting the forward model communication from speech production to reception areas, we found evidence in comparison subjects of a relationship between prespeech synchrony and N1 suppression during talking compared to listening.

Consistent with the theory that the forward model mechanism is dysfunctional in schizophrenia (e.g., 21 , 23) , this prespeech signal was smaller in patients and was not associated with suppression of cortical responsiveness to speech sounds. Nevertheless, there was still evidence of a corollary discharge signal in patients because the prespeech intertrial coherence was larger during talking than listening and the amount of prespeech synchrony in patients was inversely correlated with auditory hallucination severity. That is, in addition to dampening irrelevant sensations resulting from our own actions, the forward model may distinguish between internally and externally generated percepts. This would apply to memories and inner experiences because thoughts have been described as our most complex motor act (20) . Specifically, if an efference copy of a thought or inner experience does not produce a corollary discharge of its expected sensory consequences, internal experiences may be experienced as external.

These data are similar to those recorded during self-paced button pressing in some of these patients (unpublished report by Ford et al.). In that study, we found that a lack of prepress neural synchrony over the contralateral sensory motor cortex in the patients was related to the motor symptoms of avolition and apathy and not to auditory hallucination severity. In the current study, when both hallucination severity and avolition/apathy were entered into a multiple regression analysis to control for the effects of one on the other, hallucination severity only was related to prespeech synchrony.

We are becoming increasingly aware that locally specialized functions in the brain must be coordinated with each other, and coordination failures may be responsible for a wide range of problems in schizophrenia, from psychotic experiences to cognitive dysfunction. The asynchrony seen in patients preceding self-initiated movements, such as talking and pressing a button, suggests that patients with schizophrenia are out of synch at a most basic level in tasks that should be relatively unaffected by differences in intellectual ability and motivation and in tasks that are not speaking-bound (unpublished report by Ford et al.). This suggests that these symptoms could result from a deficient message being sent to the sensory cortex that the action is “self-generated” and that it results in modality-specific symptoms.

EEG is an excellent way to study temporal synchrony, and new time-frequency analyses show promise as brain-imaging tools. Furthermore, neural oscillations are readily translated to more basic neural mechanisms studied in laboratory animals and in vitro. Although these data support the notion that the forward model system can be assessed with these tools and that this system is dysfunctional in patients, especially those who hallucinate, there are many questions still needing to be addressed: whether the deficit seen in patients is due to a faulty corollary discharge to the auditory cortex or if faulty processing of the information in the auditory cortex can be addressed in fMRI studies with this paradigm. Whether neural synchrony is abnormal in patients during inner speech can be addressed in cued-inner-speech studies such as we have done before (37) . Whether the interplay of γ-aminobutyric acid (GABA) and glutamate affects the frequency (38) or the phase (39) of neural oscillations of premovement activity can be addressed with drug challenge studies in human and nonhuman primates. Relationships between neural synchrony and spoken sound quality should be addressed in a series of parametric manipulations.

Received May 21, 2006; revision received Aug. 2, 2006; accepted Aug. 31, 2006. From the Psychiatry Service, Veterans Affairs Connecticut Healthcare System; the Department of Psychiatry, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, Conn.; the Psychology Service, Veterans Affairs Palo Alto Healthcare System, Palo Alto, Calif.; and the Department of Psychiatry, Stanford University School of Medicine, Stanford, Calif. Address correspondence and reprint requests to Dr. Ford, Department of Psychiatry, Yale University School of Medicine, 950 Campbell Ave., VA Connecticut Healthcare System, 116A, West Haven, CT 06516; [email protected] (e-mail).

Supported by grants from the VA Schizophrenia Biological Research Center, NIMH (MH-58262 and MH-067967), the National Alliance for Research in Schizophrenia and Affective Disorders, and the VA.

All authors report no competing interests.

References

1. Singer W: Neuronal synchrony: a versatile code for the definition of relations? Neuron 1999; 24:49–65Google Scholar

2. Phillips WA, Silverstein SM: Convergence of biological and psychological perspectives on cognitive coordination in schizophrenia. Behav Brain Sci 2003; 26:65–82Google Scholar

3. Helmholtz H: Treatise on physiological optics, in Optical Society of America, vol III. Edited by Southall JPC. Rochester, NY, 1925, pp 44–51Google Scholar

4. Von Holst E, Mittelstaedt H: Das reafferenzprinzip. Naturwissenschaften 1950; 37:464–476Google Scholar

5. Sperry RW: Neural basis of the spontaneous optokinetic response produced by visual inversion. J Comp Physiol Psychol 1950; 43:482–489Google Scholar

6. Muller-Preuss P, Ploog D: Inhibition of auditory cortical neurons during phonation. Brain Res 1981; 215:61–76Google Scholar

7. Eliades SJ, Wang X: Sensory-motor interaction in the primate auditory cortex during self-initiated vocalizations. J Neurophysiol 2003; 89:2194–2207Google Scholar

8. Heinks-Maldonado TH, Mathalon DH, Gray M, Ford JM: Fine-tuning of auditory cortex during speech production. Psychophysiology 2005; 42:180–190Google Scholar

9. Bastiaansen MCM, Hagoort P: Oscillatory brain dynamics during language comprehension, in Event-Related Dynamics of Brain Oscillations: Progress in Brain Research Series, vol. 259. Edited by Klimesch W, Neuper C. Amsterdam, Elsevier, 2006, pp 179–199Google Scholar

10. Nicolelis MA, Baccala LA, Lin RC, Chapin JK: Sensorimotor encoding by synchronous neural ensemble activity at multiple levels of the somatosensory system. Science 1995; 268:1353–1358Google Scholar

11. Hamada Y, Miyashita E, Tanaka H: Gamma-band oscillations in the “barrel cortex” precede rat’s exploratory whisking. Neuroscience 1999; 88:667–671Google Scholar

12. Friston KJ, Frith CD: Schizophrenia: a disconnection syndrome? Clin Neurosci 1995; 3:89–97Google Scholar

13. Kwon JS, O’Donnell BF, Wallenstein GV, Greene RW, Hirayasu Y, Nestor PG, Hasselmo ME, Potts GF, Shenton ME, McCarley RW: Gamma frequency-range abnormalities to auditory stimulation in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1999; 56:1001–1005Google Scholar

14. Spencer KM, Nestor PG, Niznikiewicz MA, Salisbury DF, Shenton ME, McCarley RW: Abnormal neural synchrony in schizophrenia. J Neurosci 2003; 23:7407–7411Google Scholar

15. Buzsaki G: Large-scale recording of neuronal ensembles. Nat Neurosci 2004; 7:446–451Google Scholar

16. Delorme A, Makeig S: EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J Neurosci Methods 2004; 134:9–21Google Scholar

17. Tallon-Baudry C, Bertrand O, Delpuech C, Pernier J: Stimulus specificity of phase-locked and non-phase-locked 40: hz visual responses in human. J Neurosci 1996; 16:4240–4249Google Scholar

18. Ford JM, Mathalon DH, Whitfield S, Faustman WO, Roth WT: Reduced communication between frontal and temporal lobes during talking in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 2002; 21:485–492Google Scholar

19. Bland BH, Oddie SD: Theta band oscillation and synchrony in the hippocampal formation and associated structures: the case for its role in sensorimotor integration. Behav Brain Res 2001; 127:119–136Google Scholar

20. Jackson JH: Selected Writings of John Hughlings Jackson. New York, Basic Books, 1958Google Scholar

21. Feinberg I: Efference copy and corollary discharge: implications for thinking and its disorders. Schizophr Bull 1978; 4:636–640Google Scholar

22. Lindner A, Thier P, Kircher TT, Haarmeier T, Leube DT: Disorders of agency in schizophrenia correlate with an inability to compensate for the sensory consequences of actions. Curr Biol 2005; 15:1119–1124Google Scholar

23. Frith CD, Blakemore S, Wolpert DM: Explaining the symptoms of schizophrenia: abnormalities in the awareness of action. Brain Res Rev 2000; 31:357–363Google Scholar

24. Turken AU, Vuilleumier P, Mathalon DH, Swick D, Ford JM: Are impairments of action monitoring and executive control dissociable dysfunctions in patients with schizophrenia? Am J Psychiatry 2003; 160:1881–1883Google Scholar

25. Shergill SS, Samson G, Bays PM, Frith CD, Wolpert DM: Evidence for sensory prediction deficits in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 2005; 162:2384–2386Google Scholar

26. Ford JM, Mathalon DH, Heinks T, Kalba S, Roth WT: Neurophysiological evidence of corollary discharge dysfunction in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 2001; 158:2069–2071Google Scholar

27. Heinks-Maldonado TH, Mathalon DH, Houde JF, Gray E, Ford JM: Imprecise corollary discharge in schizophrenia is related to auditory hallucinations. Arch Gen Psychiatry (in press)Google Scholar

28. American Psychiatric Association: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th ed. (DSM-IV). Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Association, 1994Google Scholar

29. First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JBW: Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders. New York, New York State Psychiatric Institute, Biometrics Research Department, 1995Google Scholar

30. Andreasen NC: Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS). Iowa City, University of Iowa, 1983Google Scholar

31. Andreasen NC: Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms (SAPS). Iowa City, University of Iowa, 1984Google Scholar

32. Faustman WO, Overall JE: The Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale, in The Use of Psychological Testing for Treatment, Planning and Outcome Assessment, 2nd ed. Edited by Maruish M. Hillsdale, NJ, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1999, pp 791–830Google Scholar

33. Gratton G, Coles MGH, Donchin E: A new method for off-line removal of ocular artifact. Electroencephal Clin Neurophysiol 1983; 55:468–484Google Scholar

34. Makeig S: Auditory event-related dynamics of the EEG spectrum and effects of exposure to tones. Electroencephal Clin Neurophysiol 1993; 86:283–293Google Scholar

35. Lyons R: Understanding Digital Signal Processing. Englewood Cliffs, NJ, Prentice Hall, 2004Google Scholar

36. Keppel G: Design and Analysis: A Researcher’s Handbook. Engelwood Cliffs, NJ, Prentice Hall, 1991Google Scholar

37. Ford JM, Mathalon DH, Kalba S, Whitfield S, Faustman WO, Roth WT: Cortical responsiveness during inner speech in schizophrenia: an event-related brain potential study. Am J Psychiatry 2001; 158:1914–1916Google Scholar

38. Hughes SW, Lorincz M, Cope DW, Blethyn KL, Kekesi KA, Parri HR, Juhasz G, Crunelli V: Synchronized oscillations at alpha and theta frequencies in the lateral geniculate nucleus. Neuron 2004; 42:253–268Google Scholar

39. Blatow M, Rozov A, Katona I, Hormuzdi SG, Meyer AH, Whittington MA, Caputi A, Monyer H: A novel network of multipolar bursting interneurons generates theta frequency oscillations in neocortex. Neuron 2003; 38:805–817Google Scholar