The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×
Published Online:

Abstract

Objective: Cannabis use may produce neurophysiological disturbances similar to those observed in schizophrenia, particularly in relation to altered neural synchronization. Therefore, the current experiment examined the effect of cannabis use on EEG neural synchronization using the auditory steady-state evoked potential. Method: Auditory steady-state evoked potentials were assessed using varying rates of stimulation (auditory click-trains of 20, 30, 40 Hz) in current cannabis users (N=17) and drug-naive comparison subjects (N=16). EEG spectral power and signal-to-noise ratio at each stimulation frequency were compared between groups. Results: Cannabis users showed decreased EEG power and signal-to-noise ratio at the stimulation frequency of 20 Hz. In addition, current cannabis users demonstrated increased schizotypal personality characteristics as assessed with the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire, which positively correlated with total years of cannabis use. Finally, within the cannabis group, 20-Hz power values were negatively correlated with Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire scores. Conclusions: These data provide evidence for neural synchronization and early-stage sensory processing deficits in cannabis use. This finding, along with the observed increased rates of schizotypy in cannabis users, adds support for a cannabinoid link to schizophrenia spectrum disorders.

Marijuana, or Cannabis sativa, is one of the most commonly used illicit drugs, with over 12 million users in the U.S. alone (1) . The acute behavioral effects of cannabis include perceptual distortions, paranoia, altered time perception, and occasional hallucinations, along with concomitant disruptions in short-term memory, attention, concept formation, and motor coordination (24) . The similarities between these effects and many of the symptoms of schizophrenia have led several researchers to postulate that the cannabinoid system may play a key role in schizophrenia spectrum disorders (3 , 6ι) .

Andreasen (10) proposed that the symptom hetereogeneity observed in psychotic disorders (hallucinations, thought disorder, neurological soft signs, etc.) may be understood in terms of a general desynchronization of cortical circuitry. Thus, a possible neurophysiological link between schizophrenia symptoms and cannabis’ neurobehavioral effects may involve alterations in neural synchronization. Cellular studies and computational models have suggested that rhythmic synchronization across neuronal assembles in the beta (15–30 Hz) and gamma (30–80 Hz) range plays an important role in the integration and binding of perceptual features, associative learning processes, and conscious awareness (11 , 12) . In humans, neural synchronization can be assessed noninvasively by entrainment of the EEG to periodic sensory stimuli (e.g., auditory click trains at specific frequencies). Because the steady-state EEG waveform entrains to the frequency and phase of the presented stimulus, it serves as an indicator of the functional state of the neural circuits supporting synchronization (13 , 14) .

Direct evidence of altered neural synchronization using the evoked entrainment paradigm has been shown in schizophrenia patients. Patients demonstrate reductions in both spectral power and phase synchrony of steady-state visual and auditory evoked potentials. In the visual modality, this reduction is most pronounced in the alpha (6–13 Hz) (15 , 16) and beta-range frequencies (17–30 Hz) (17 , 18) . However, such dysregulation is more marked in the auditory modality, particularly at 40 Hz of stimulation (17 , 19β1) . In addition to disruptions in synchrony using evoked steady-state responses, several studies have demonstrated schizophrenia deficits in transient induced synchronization (gamma band) to gestalt images (22) and during auditory discrimination paradigms (23 , 24) . Thus, the ability of neural networks to support synchronous activity appear to be compromised in psychotic patients.

The neurochemical correlates of neural synchrony have been shown to be modulated by γ-aminobutyric acid (GABA)-ergic interneurons and metabotropic and ionotropic glutamate receptors (12) . Another putative mechanism may relate to the neuromodulatory actions of endocannabinoids. For example, it is now known that central cannabinoid (CB1) receptors are colocalized with GABA interneurons (25 , 26) . More directly, Hajos et al. (27) demonstrated that administration of the highly potent cannabinoid agonist CP55,940 markedly reduced the power of 40-Hz oscillations elicited in hippocampal slices by kainate. Thus, cannabinoid activity, either through endogenous or exogenous agonist administration, may exert neuromodulatory control of GABA-ergic and glutamatergic-dependent network oscillations. To date, no studies have sought to ascertain whether cannabis use produces alterations in neural synchronization in humans.

The purpose of the current study was to examine whether cannabis use produces neurophysiological disturbances in neural synchronization. Auditory steady-state EEG responses (20, 30, and 40 Hz) were examined in cannabis users versus drug-naive comparison subjects, and several questionnaires aiming to assess levels of schizotypy and perceptual distortion were administered. The frequencies of stimulation were chosen based on previous work that has examined beta- and gamma-range auditory stimulation in schizophrenia and schizotypy (20 , 21) .

Method

Subjects

Current cannabis users (N=17) and healthy drug-naive comparison subjects (N=16) were assessed. The subjects were recruited from the local university community and paid for their participation in the study; written informed consent was obtained from each. Table 1 shows participant demographic, clinical, and cannabis use characteristics. There were no significant differences between the groups in age (F=1.71, df=1, 31, p=0.20) or years of education (F=1.19, df=1, 31, p=0.28).

The inclusion criteria were as follows:

1. For the cannabis group: current cannabis consumption (marijuana cigarettes smoked) at the rate of at least once per week (during the past month), and no other illicit substance use during the past 6 months.

2. For the drug-naive comparison subjects: no history of illicit substance use.

3. For all subjects: ages 18–35, completion of high school education, no history of DSM-IV axis I or II disorders other than cannabis dependence, no history of cardiovascular disease, hearing problems, neurological disease, learning disability, or head injury resulting in loss of consciousness.

In addition, subjects were excluded if they reported consumption of more than two alcoholic drinks per day (one per day for women). The cannabis group’s drug-use inclusion criteria (cannabis use at least once per week, 24-hour abstinence) were chosen to eliminate acute cannabis effects while retaining neurophysiological effects from residual cannabinoids. Previous work has shown that several areas of neuropsychological performance remain affected up to 7 days after cessation of cannabis use (possibly because of its long half-life and the fat solubility of tetrahydrocannabinol [THC]), and this heuristic was used in the current study (28 , 29) . Finally, it should be noted that although none of the cannabis-using subjects had used other substances during the past 6 months, several participants (10 of 17) reported polydrug use in the past (cocaine, amphetamines, LSD, and 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine [MDMA or Ecstasy]). Comparisons of the pure cannabis users and the past polydrug cannabis users on each of the measures described in the following paragraphs yielded no significant differences.

Clinical Interviews and Questionnaires

The following psychometric questionnaires and clinical interviews were administered to all subjects before EEG recording: the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I and II disorders (SCID I and SCID II), a substance use questionnaire, the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire (30) , the Perceptual Aberration Scale (31) , the Dissociative Experiences Scale (DES) (32) , and subscales of the WAIS-III (picture completion, digit symbol, similarities, and digit span).

Using the time-line follow-back procedure, measures of frequency, quantity, and density of cannabis consumption were determined, with the clinical interview for the past 6 months, then for 1 month before the test session. The subjects were instructed to consider each day of the week and indicate, for an average week, how much they consumed per drug-use occasion over the past 6 months and then for the month before testing. Recency or density of last use was assessed using the past-month section of the interview.

Stimuli and EEG Recording Procedure

Auditory stimuli consisted of click trains (square waves) presented at three different frequencies in each of three blocks (20, 30, and 40 Hz at 80 dB sound pressure level). Each block contained 100 trials of each frequency presented for 500 msec each (interstimulus interval of 1000 msec) and were presented through foam insert earphones (Etymotic Research, Elk Grove Village, Ill.). The order of the blocks was randomly assigned across subjects. The subjects were tested in a sound-attenuated room and asked to relax with eyes open and listen to the click trains while sitting in an upright chair with a headrest during the recording.

The EEG was recorded continuously (band pass 0.1–100 Hz; sampling rate 1000 Hz) from the scalp with a 12-channel electrode cap with a nose reference. Electrode impedances were maintained below 10 kΩ. The recorded EEG was segmented into epochs consisting of the 500 msec during stimulus presentation, with any epoch containing a voltage greater than 100 μV excluded. Ocular movement artifact correction was applied with the algorithm of Gratton et al. (33) , and averages were computed for each frequency block with commercially available software (Vision Analyzer, Brain Products GmbH, Gilching, Germany).

EEG Data Analysis

Spectral Power

A power spectrum was obtained from the averaged EEGs for each condition and subject using a Fast Fourier Transform (Vision Analyzer, Brain Products). The Fast Fourier Transform was performed on the time interval during stimulus presentation (500 msec). The average signal power from all subjects in each group for 12 channels (three frontal, three central, one parietal, two temporal, and three occipital regions: F7, F8, Fz, C3, C4, Cz, T4, T6, Pz, O1, O2, and Oz) at each frequency of stimulation was plotted on a scalp map using interpolated data to examine the scalp distribution of the responses. Confirming previous research on auditory steady-state evoked potentials (17 , 19β1) , the signals showed local maxima in frontal-central regions, and all statistical analyses were conducted on data from electrodes F7, F8, Fz, C3, C4, and Cz.

Signal-to-noise ratio

The power of the averaged EEG response at the frequency of stimulation (averaged trial power) was used as the signal value and compared to mean single trial power. Single trial power was obtained by calculating the mean power at the stimulus frequency across all trials. Noise was then computed as the difference between the mean of the single trial power for the frequency of stimulation minus the signal (averaged trial power).

Noise=single trial power – averaged trial power

Signal-to-noise ratio=averaged trial power/(single trial power – averaged trial power)

This formula takes the power derived from averaged trial data as the signal, and its difference from power calculated from single trials as noise. This measure of the signal-to-noise ratio has been shown to be affected in schizophrenia for event-related potentials to auditory stimuli during a tone discrimination task (34 , 35) .

Statistical Analysis

Power and signal-to-noise ratio at the frequencies of stimulation were measured for each condition in which the entrainment power was largest (frontal-central electrodes). The log 10 -transformed power and signal-to-noise ratio values were used for each statistical analysis. A repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) was used to evaluate the between-subject factor of group (two) and the within-subject factor of electrode (six) on the power and signal-to-noise ratios at each stimulation frequency. A one-way ANOVA was used to compare scores on the various questionnaires between groups. In order to examine possible relationships between variables, Pearson correlation coefficients were used. A criterion of p<0.05 was used throughout to determine statistical significance, and all tests were two-tailed.

Results

An ANOVA revealed that cannabis users had higher scores on the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire (F=21.8, df=1, 31, p<0.0001) and the Perceptual Aberration Scale (F=8.9, df=1, 31, p<0.005) ( Figure 1 ). No differences were found for DES or WAIS-III scores. The lack of differences between the groups on WAIS scores indicates that any alterations in neural synchrony are not associated with generalized cognitive or sensory deficits.

Figure 1. Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire and Perceptual Aberration Scale Scores for Healthy Comparison Subjects and Cannabis Users a

a Cannabis users demonstrated significantly higher scores.

In the auditory entrainment protocol, subjects in both groups entrained to all three stimulus frequencies, which can be seen in the graphs of spectral power taken from electrode Fz ( Figure 2 ). The 20-Hz stimulation also elicited a strong 40-Hz harmonic response, which can be seen in the upper panel of Figure 2 . A repeated-measures ANOVA examining the effect of group on 20 Hz power across all frontal and central electrodes revealed a main effect of group, indicating that cannabis-using subjects demonstrated significantly less entrainment power during 20-Hz stimulation (F=5.23, df=1, 31, p<0.03). A main effect of group was also observed in the concomitant 40-Hz harmonic response (F=4.55, 1, 31, p<0.05). A significant group-by-electrode interaction was also observed in the 20-Hz condition (F=3.58, df=5, 27, p<0.02). Individual AVOVAs on power at each electrode revealed that although the cannabis-using subjects had decreased 20 Hz power across all frontal-central electrodes, the group differences were significant at C4 (F=5.3, df=1, 31, p<0.03), F7 (F=4.7, df=1, 31, p<0.04), and Fz (F=4.1, df=1, 31, p<0.05). Both groups exhibited the largest 20-Hz response at Fz, and the decreased entrainment in the cannabis-using group in relation to comparison subjects at Fz can be seen in the upper panel of Figure 2 . The ANOVAs revealed no group differences in the 30- or 40-Hz stimulation conditions.

Figure 2. Mean Power Spectra From Electrode Fz and Topographic Maps From EEGs Recorded to Periodic Auditory Click-Trains a

a The 20-Hz and 40-Hz harmonic responses were significantly lower in the cannabis group.

For signal-to-noise ratios, a significant main effect of group was observed in the 20-Hz condition, indicating that subjects with cannabis had decreased signal-to-noise ratios at 20 Hz (F=5.86, df=1, 31, p<0.03). A significant group-by-electrode interaction was also observed for signal-to-noise ratio at 20 Hz (F=3.1, df=5, 27, p<0.03). Individual ANOVAs indicated that cannabis-using subjects had decreased signal-to-noise ratios across all frontal-central electrodes, with significant group differences at C4 (F=7.20, 1, 31, p<0.01), and F7 (F=6.90, df=1, 31, p<0.01) (Fz and C3 were also lower but were below the level of significance). However, no differences were observed between the groups in noise power for 20 Hz (F=0.47, df=6, 26, p=0.82). This pattern indicates that the group differences in spectral power at 20-Hz stimulation were not due to greater system noise but may be the result of lower signal strength. No differences in the signal-to-noise ratios were found for 30 or 40 Hz of stimulation or in the 40-Hz harmonic during the 20-Hz condition.

Correlational analyses revealed that within the cannabis group, a significant negative association was observed between 20 Hz power at Fz and Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire scores (r=–0.55, p<0.02). Thus, individuals within the cannabis-using group with the highest schizotypy demonstrated the lowest power at 20 Hz ( Figure 3 ). This correlation was not apparent for the other frequencies of stimulation or within the comparison group. Finally, within the cannabis group, a significant positive correlation was observed between total years of cannabis use and Schizotypal Personality Disorder scores (r=0.49, p<0.05) ( Figure 4 ).

Figure 3. Negative Correlation Between 20 Hz Log 10 Power and Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire Scores Within a Cannabis-Using Group
Figure 4. Positive Correlation Between Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire Scores and Total Years of Cannabis Use

Discussion

The current study demonstrated that cannabis users exhibited altered neural synchronization as shown by reduced 20 Hz EEG power and signal-to-noise ratio to auditory stimulation. No differences were found between the groups in noise power, indicating that the decreased 20 Hz power may be due to decreased signal strength of oscillating circuits and not the increased noise stemming from neural background activity. Increased perceptual aberration and schizotypy scores were also observed, and the increased schizotypy was positively correlated with total years of cannabis use. In addition, within the cannabis group, schizotypy scores were negatively correlated with 20 Hz power, indicating that cannabis-using individuals scoring higher in schizotypy demonstrated larger deficits in neural synchronization at 20 Hz.

The observed association between cannabis use and schizotypy is consistent with previous research demonstrating increased schizotypy in otherwise healthy cannabis users (3 , 36γ9) . Although it is difficult to determine the direction of causality of this association without longitudinal studies, previous work has shown that past cannabis users who were drug-free for at least 6 months showed no differences in schizotypy in relation to the comparison subjects (3) . Furthermore, in the recent study by D’Souza et al. (9) , it has been shown that direct intravenous cannabinoid administration (Δ-9-THC) in healthy subjects can induce acute perceptual aberrations, memory or attentional impairments, and psychotomimetic effects that resemble the symptoms of schizophrenia. Thus, it appears that cannabis use mimics many of the behavioral symptoms observed in schizophrenia and may constitute an independent risk factor for the development of psychosis in vulnerable individuals (4046) . Indeed, it has recently been shown that individuals with a predisposition toward psychosis who later consume cannabis are at a greater risk for psychotic symptoms (47) , suggesting a possible gene-by-environment interaction. Toward this end, Caspi et al. (48) showed that a functional polymorphism in the catechol- O -methyltransferase (COMT) gene, which is involved in synaptic dopamine metabolism, interacts with adolescent cannabis use to predict adult psychosis. Given that there is tight coupling of the endocannabinoid and dopamine systems (49) , it appears possible that genetic anomalies leading to altered dopamine activity may interact with early cannabis exposure to produce overt psychosis.

The present study is the first to our knowledge to demonstrate decreased auditory steady-state responses in cannabis users. Decreases in this response using similar EEG paradigms have been shown in patients with schizophrenia (17 , 20 , 21) . However, the reductions in EEG power in schizophrenia primarily occur in the gamma-range (~40 Hz). It is unclear why the observed reduction in power was confined to the lower frequency of 20 Hz, although it should be noted that the 40-Hz harmonic of 20 Hz was also significantly lower in cannabis users. One possibility is that cannabis consumption modulates beta-range neural synchronization. Computational models suggest that gamma rhythms may represent synchronous activity in local neural circuits, whereas beta rhythms represent synchronization over larger distances in the brain (50) . Such a role of beta activity has been shown in human EEGs during multimodal object processing in which increased coherence at 12–20 Hz between parietal and temporal electrodes was observed during stimulus processing (51) . Thus, there may be dissociation between the types of synchronization abnormalities in cannabis use versus full-blown schizophrenia. Although it is purely speculative, it is possible that this differing pattern is due to the fact that the psychotomimetic effects of cannabinoids are subthreshold and may initially affect only beta synchrony. As several studies have also demonstrated that gamma and beta rhythms can occur simultaneously (52 , 53) , it is clear that the relationship between gamma and beta synchronization is complex, and future work is needed to clarify the differing patterns of abnormal steady-state responses in cannabis-using and schizophrenia populations.

Concerning the mechanism of cannabis’ effect on neural synchronization, several possibilities exist. The first relates to cerebellar modulation of neural synchrony. The cerebellum is involved in a multitude of nonmotor functions, such as cognition (executive function and attention) and neural timing (54 , 55) . Recently, it has been shown that periodic stimulation in the gamma range causes an increase in cerebellar activity in both auditory and visual steady-state paradigms, as assessed with positron emission tomography (56 , 57) . Because several studies have shown that cannabinoid-dependent timing defects are related to cerebellar activity, cannabis use may cause disruptions in neural synchrony through alterations in neural events in the cerebellum (58 , 59) . These data, along with the fact that cerebellar neurons are replete with GABA-ergic and cannabinoid neurons (60 , 61) , both of which are implicated in neural synchronization (12 , 27) , provide indirect support that cannabis consumption may modulate synchrony with the cerebellum.

Another possible mechanism of cannabis’ effect on neural synchronization relates to the possible role of endogenous cannabinoids in GABA-ergic network oscillations. In the hippocampus, CB1 receptors are located primarily on cholecystokinin-containing GABA-ergic interneurons (2527) . Activation of these presynaptic CB1 receptors reduces GABA release by interneurons (25 , 62) , which, in turn, could disrupt the synchronization of pyramidal cell activity. The reduced power of network oscillations by cannabinoid administration demonstrated by Hajos et al. (27) provides initial support for this idea. Such cannabis-modulated desynchronization could thereby interfere with associative functions, disrupt normal gating mechanisms, and eventually lead to psychotic or altered perceptual experiences.

Several issues related to the conclusions of the present study need to be addressed. Levels of cannabis use were determined through self-reports. Hence, inaccuracies in estimated drug use patterns, frequency, and time of last use are possible. However, previous work has shown that self-reports of cannabis use tend to be fairly reliable (63 , 64) . Additionally, because subjects were tested after 24 hour of abstinence, it is unclear whether the observed effects were due to cannabis withdrawal (65) or residual cannabinoids (28) . Finally, several of the cannabis subjects reported a past history of use of other illicit substances, which could potentially confound the current results. Each of these concerns could be negated by future studies using both urinary or plasma levels of THC metabolites and assessment of EEGs during direct cannabinoid administration.

A second issue relates to the known attentional deficits observed in cannabis use (3) . It is possible that the decreased power observed at 20 Hz is mediated by cannabis-induced attentional dysregulation. However, if attentional effects were responsible for the decreased synchronization, then similar patterns would be expected for the other frequencies of stimulation (30 and 40 Hz). In addition, unlike visual steady-state paradigms, there are inconsistent data regarding whether power in the auditory modality is affected by selective attention (66 , 67) . Future studies seeking to concomitantly assess attention and neural synchrony in relation to cannabinoids could address these issues.

Finally, assessments of induced gamma or beta synchronization during object recognition, motion perception, or discrimination tasks are necessary because these paradigms have higher ecological validity and probe processes of perceptual integration that are known to be aberrant in cannabis use. Such studies would serve to elucidate the neurophysiological effects of cannabis on the brain, along with further delineating the relationship between the cannabinoid system and processes known to be affected in schizophrenia.

Received Nov. 30, 2004; revision received March 31, 2005; accepted April 26, 2005. From the Department of Psychology, Indiana University. Address correspondence and reprint requests to Dr. Skosnik, Indiana University, Department of Psychology, 1101 East 10th St., Bloomington, IN 47405; [email protected] (e-mail).The authors report no competing interests.Supported in part by an NIMH grant (1RO1-MH62150-01) and an Indiana University Postdoctoral Clinical Science Fellowship (NIMH T32).The authors thank Jen Vohs, Peter Finn, Bill Hetrick, Deepak C. D’Souza, J. Michael Walker, and Michael Paulin for their help.

References

1. Office of Applied Studies: Results From the 2001 National Household Survey on Drug Abuse: Volume II, Technical Appendices and Selected Data Tables. NHSDA Series H-18. US Department of Health and Human Services Publication number (SMA) 02–3759. Rockville, Md, SAMHSA, 2002Google Scholar

2. Abood ME, Martin BR: Neurobiology of marijuana use. Trends in Pharmcol Sci 1992; 13:202–207Google Scholar

3. Skosnik PD, Spatz-Glenn L, Park S: Cannabis use is associated with schizotypy and attentional disinhibition. Schizophr Res 2001; 48:83–92Google Scholar

4. Iversen L: Cannabis and the brain. Brain 2003; 126 (part 6):1252–1270Google Scholar

5. Emrich HM, Leweke FM, Schneider U: Towards a cannabinoid hypothesis of schizophrenia: cognitive impairments due to dysregulation of the endogenous cannabinoid system. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 1997; 56:803–807Google Scholar

6. Leweke FM, Giuffrida A, Wurster U, Emrich HM, Piomelli D: Elevated endogenous cannabinoids in schizophrenia. Neuroreport 1999; 10:1665–1669Google Scholar

7. Pryor SR: Is platelet release of 2-arachidonoylglycerol a mediator of cognitive deficits? an endocannabinoid theory of schizophrenia and arousal. Med Hypotheses 2000; 55:494–501Google Scholar

8. Skosnik PD, Yao JK: From membrane phospholipid defects to altered neurotransmission: is arachidonic acid a nexus in the pathophysiology of schizophrenia? Prostaglandins Leukot Essent Fatty Acids 2003; 69:367–384Google Scholar

9. D’Souza DC, Perry E, MacDougall L, Ammerman Y, Cooper T, Wu YT, Braley G, Gueorguieva R, Krystal JH: The psychotomimetic effects of intravenous delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol in healthy individuals: implications for psychosis. Neuropsychopharmacology 2004; 29:1558–1572Google Scholar

10. Andreasen NC: A unitary model of schizophrenia: Bleuler’s “fragmented phrene” as schizencephaly. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1999; 56:781–787Google Scholar

11. Singer W: Neuronal synchrony: a versatile code for the definition of relations? Neuron 1999; 24:49–65, 111–125Google Scholar

12. Whittington MA, Faulkner HJ, Doheny HC, Traub RD: Neuronal fast oscillations as a target site for psychoactive drugs. Pharmacol Ther 2000; 86:171–190Google Scholar

13. Reagan D: Human brain electrophysiology: evoked potentials and evoked magnetic fields in science and medicine. New York, Elsevier, 1989Google Scholar

14. O’Donnell BF: Visual Evoked Potentials: Encyclopedia of Cognitive Science-Psychology, Vol 4. London, Macmillan, 2003, pp 516–520Google Scholar

15. Jin Y, Sandman CA, Wu JC, Bernat J, Potkin SG: Topographic analysis of EEG photic driving in normal and schizophrenic subjects. Clin Electroencephalogr 1995; 26:102–107Google Scholar

16. Clementz BA, Keil A, Kissler J: Aberrant brain dynamics in schizophrenia: delayed buildup and prolonged decay of the visual steady-state response. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 2004; 18:121–129Google Scholar

17. O’Donnell BF, Wilt MA, Brenner C, Busey TA, Kwon JS: EEG synchronization deficits in schizophrenia spectrum disorders, in Recent Advances in Human Brain Mapping, International Congress Series. Edited by K Hirata, Y Koga, K Nagata, K Yamazaki. New York, Elsevier, 2002, pp 697–703Google Scholar

18. Krishnan GP, Vohs JL, Hetrick WP, Carroll CA, Shekhar CA, Bockbrader MA, O’Donnell: Steady state visual evoked potential abnormalities in schizophrenia. Clin Neurophysiol 2005; 116:614–624Google Scholar

19. Pantev C, Makeig S, Hoke M, Galambos R, Hampson S, Gallen C: Human auditory-evoked gamma-band magnetic fields. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 1991; 88:8996–9000Google Scholar

20. Kwon JS, O’Donnell BF, Wallenstein GV, Greene RW, Hirayasu Y, Nestor PG, Hasselmo ME, Potts GF, Shenton ME, McCarley RW: Gamma frequency-range abnormalities to auditory stimulation in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1999; 56:1001–1105Google Scholar

21. Brenner CA, Sporns O, Lysaker PH, O’Donnell BF: EEG synchronization to modulated auditory tones in schizophrenia, schizoaffective disorder, and schizotypal personality disorder. Am J Psychiatry 2003; 160:2238–2240Google Scholar

22. Spencer KM, Nestor PG, Niznikiewicz MA, Salisbury DF, Shenton ME, McCarley RW: Abnormal neural synchrony in schizophrenia. J Neurosci 2003; 23:7407–7411Google Scholar

23. Lee KH, Williams LM, Haig A, Goldberg E, Gordon E: An integration of 40 Hz gamma and phasic arousal: novelty and routinization processing in schizophrenia. Clin Neurophysiol 2001; 112:1499–1507Google Scholar

24. Gallinat J, Winterer G, Herrmann CS, Senkowski D: Reduced oscillatory gamma-band responses in unmedicated schizophrenic patients indicate impaired frontal network processing. Clin Neurophysiol 2004; 115:1863–1874Google Scholar

25. Katona I, Sperlagh B, Sik A, Kafalvi A, Vizi ES, Mackie K, Freund TF: Presynaptically located CB1 cannabinoid receptors regulate GABA release from axon terminals of specific hippocampal interneurons. J Neurosci 1999; 19:4544–4558Google Scholar

26. Tsou K, Mackie K, Sanudo-Pena MC, Walker JM: Cannabinoid CB1 receptors are localized primarily on cholecystokinin-containing GABA-ergic interneurons in the rat hippocampal formation. Neuroscience 1999; 93:969–975Google Scholar

27. Hajos N, Katona I, Naiem SS, MacKie K, Ledent C, Mody I, Freund TF: Cannabinoids inhibit hippocampal GABA-ergic transmission and network oscillations. Eur J Neurosci 2000; 12:3239–3249Google Scholar

28. Pope HG Jr, Gruber AJ, Yurgelun-Todd D: Residual neuropsychologic effects of cannabis. Curr Psychiatry Rep 2001; 3:507–512Google Scholar

29. Pope HG Jr, Gruber AJ, Hudson JI, Huestis MA, Yurgelun-Todd D: Neuropsychological performance in long-term cannabis users. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2001; 58:909–915Google Scholar

30. Raine A: The SPQ: a scale for the assessment of schizotypal personality based on DSM-III-R criteria. Schizophr Bull 1991; 17:554–564Google Scholar

31. Chapman LJ, Chapman JP, Raulin ML: Body-image aberration in schizophrenia. J Abnorm Psychol 1978; 87:399–407Google Scholar

32. Putnam FW, Carlson EB, Ross CA, Anderson G, Clark P, Torem M, Bowman ES, Coons P, Chu JA, Dill DL, Loewenstein RJ, Braun BG: Patterns of dissociation in clinical and nonclinical samples. J Nerv Ment Dis 1996; 184:673–679Google Scholar

33. Gratton G, Coles MG, Donchin E: A new method for off-line removal of ocular artifact. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1983; 55:468–484Google Scholar

34. Winterer G, Ziller M, Dorn H, Frick K, Mulert C, Wuebben Y, Herrmann WM, Coppola R: Schizophrenia: reduced signal-to-noise ratio and impaired phase-locking during information processing. Clin Neurophysiol 2000; 111:837–849Google Scholar

35. Winterer G, Coppola R, Goldberg TE, Egan MF, Jones DW, Sanchez CE, Weinberger DR: Prefrontal broadband noise, working memory, and genetic risk for schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 2004; 161:490–500Google Scholar

36. Williams JH, Wellman NA, Rawlins JN: Cannabis use correlates with schizotypy in healthy people. Addiction 1996; 91:869–877Google Scholar

37. Mass R, Bardong C, Kindl K, Dahme B: Relationship between cannabis use, schizotypal traits, and cognitive function in healthy subjects. Psychopathology 2001; 34:209–214Google Scholar

38. Nunn JA, Rizza F, Peters ER: The incidence of schizotypy among cannabis and alcohol users. J Nerv Ment Dis 2001; 189:741–748Google Scholar

39. Dumas P, Saoud M, Bouafia S, Gutknecht C, Ecochard R, Dalery J, Rochet T, d’Amato T: Cannabis use correlates with schizotypal personality traits in healthy students. Psychiatry Res 2002; 109:27–35Google Scholar

40. Andreasson S, Allebeck P, Engstrom A, Rydberg U: Cannabis and schizophrenia: a longitudinal study of Swedish conscripts. Lancet 1987; 2:1483–1486Google Scholar

41. Hambrecht M, Hafner H: Cannabis, vulnerability, and the onset of schizophrenia: an epidemiological perspective. Aust N Z J Psychiatry 2000; 34:468–475Google Scholar

42. Arseneault L, Cannon M, Poulton R, Murray R, Caspi A, Moffitt TE: Cannabis use in adolescence and risk for adult psychosis: longitudinal prospective study. BMJ 2002; 325:1212–1223Google Scholar

43. Patton GC, Coffey C, Carlin JB, Degenhardt L, Lynskey M, Hall W: Cannabis use and mental health in young people: cohort study. BMJ 2002; 325:1183–1184Google Scholar

44. Zammit S, Allebeck P, Andreasson S, Lundberg I, Lewis G: Self reported cannabis use as a risk factor for schizophrenia in Swedish conscripts of 1969: historical cohort study. BMJ 2002; 325:1199–1204Google Scholar

45. Fergusson DM, Horwood LJ, Swain-Campbell NR: Cannabis dependence and psychotic symptoms in young people. Psychol Med 2003; 33:15–21Google Scholar

46. Verdoux H, Gindre C, Sorbara F, Tournier M, Swendsen JD: Effects of cannabis and psychosis vulnerability in daily life: an experience sampling test study. Psychol Med 2003; 33:23–32Google Scholar

47. Henquet C, Krabbendam L, Spauwen J, Kaplan C, Lieb R, Wittchen HU, van Os J: Prospective cohort study of cannabis use, predisposition for psychosis, and psychotic symptoms in young people. BMJ 2005; 330:11Google Scholar

48. Caspi A, Moffitt TE, Cannon M, McClay J, Murray R, Harrington H, Taylor A, Arseneault L, Williams B, Braithwaite A, Poulton R, Craig IW: Moderation of the effect of adolescent-onset cannabis use on adult psychosis by a functional polymorphism in the catechol-O-methyltransferase gene: longitudinal evidence of a gene X environment interaction. Biol Psychiatry 2005; 57:1117–1127Google Scholar

49. Rodriguez De Fonseca F, Gorriti MA, Bilbao A, Escuredo L, Garcia-Segura LM, Piomelli D, Navarro M: Role of the endogenous cannabinoid system as a modulator of dopamine transmission: implications for Parkinson’s disease and schizophrenia. Neurotox Res 2001; 3:23–35Google Scholar

50. Kopell N, Ermentrout GB, Whittington MA, Traub RD: Gamma rhythms and beta rhythms have different synchronization properties. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2000; 97:1867–1872Google Scholar

51. von Stein A, Rappelsberger P, Sarnthein J, Petsche H: Synchronization between temporal and parietal cortex during multimodal object processing in man. Cereb Cortex 1999; 9:137–150Google Scholar

52. Bressler SL, Coppola R, Nakamura R: Episodic multiregional cortical coherence at multiple frequencies during visual task performance. Nature 1993; 366:153–156Google Scholar

53. Pantev C: Evoked and induced gamma-band activity of the human cortex. Brain Topogr 1995; 7:321–330Google Scholar

54. Rapoport M, van Reekum R, Mayberg H: The role of the cerebellum in cognition and behavior: a selective review. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 2000; 12:193–198Google Scholar

55. Gottwald B, Wilde B, Mihajlovic Z, Mehdorn HM: Evidence for distinct cognitive deficits after focal cerebellar lesions. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2004; 75:1524–1531Google Scholar

56. Pastor MA, Artieda J, Arbizu J, Marti-Climent JM, Penuelas I, Masdeu JC: Activation of human cerebral and cerebellar cortex by auditory stimulation at 40 Hz. J Neurosci 2002; 22:10501–10506Google Scholar

57. Pastor MA, Artieda J, Arbizu J, Valencia M, Masdeu JC: Human cerebral activation during steady-state visual-evoked responses. J Neurosci 2003; 23:11621–11627Google Scholar

58. O’Leary DS, Block RI, Turner BM, Koeppel J, Magnotta VA, Ponto LB, Watkins GL, Hichwa RD, Andreasen NC: Marijuana alters the human cerebellar clock. Neuroreport 2003; 14:1145–1151Google Scholar

59. Mathew RJ, Wilson WH, Turkington TG, Hawk TC, Coleman RE, DeGrado TR, Provenzale J: Time course of tetrahydrocannabinol-induced changes in regional cerebral blood flow measured with positron emission tomography. Psychiatry Res 2002; 116:173–185Google Scholar

60. Herkenham M, Lynn AB, Little MD, Johnson MR, Melvin LS, de Costa BR, Rice KC: Cannabinoid receptor localization in brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 1990; 87:1932–1936Google Scholar

61. Carlson BX, Elster L, Schousboe A: Pharmacological and functional implications of developmentally-regulated changes in GABA(A) receptor subunit expression in the cerebellum. Eur J Pharmacol 1998; 352:1–14Google Scholar

62. Sullivan JM: Mechanisms of cannabinoid-receptor-mediated inhibition of synaptic transmission in cultured hippocampal pyramidal neurons. J Neurophysiol 1999; 82:1286–1294Google Scholar

63. Brown J, Kranzler HR, Del Boca FK: Self-reports by alcohol and drug abuse inpatients: factors affecting reliability and validity. Br J Addict 1992; 87:1013–1024Google Scholar

64. Harrison ER, Haaga J, Richards T: Self-reported drug use data: what do they reveal? Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse 1993; 19:423–441Google Scholar

65. Budney AJ, Hughes JR, Moore BA, Vandrey R: Review of the validity and significance of cannabis withdrawal syndrome. Am J Psychiatry 2004; 161:1967–1977Google Scholar

66. Linden RD, Picton TW, Hamel G, Campbell KB: Human auditory steady-state evoked potentials during selective attention. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1987; 66:145–159Google Scholar

67. Muller MM, Picton TW, Valdes-Sosa P, Riera J, Teder-Salejarvi WA, Hillyard SA: Effects of spatial selective attention on the steady-state visual evoked potential in the 20–28 Hz range. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 1998; 6:249–261Google Scholar