The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×
Letters to the EditorFull Access

Open Scientific Practices Are the Way Forward for Internet Gaming Disorder Research: Response to Yao et al.

To the Editor: We appreciate the comments by Yao and colleagues regarding our recent study on Internet gaming disorder, an important yet little-understood phenomenon. In these comments, the authors highlight three aspects of Internet gaming disorder research that we believe merit careful consideration as this area of research develops.

First, the letter highlighted that sampling strategies are important, and we agree. Current estimates of Internet gaming disorder prevalence vary widely as a function of the methodology employed and range from 0.2% of the general population (1) to nearly half of self-selecting gamers (2). Such tremendous variation can be addressed with approaches following best practices in health research (3), as was done in the present work. The issue of sampling methodology should be considered carefully because nonrepresentative samples likely yield biased estimates and lead readers to draw faulty conclusions about the effects of gaming. Survey design, in addition to sampling, is essential. For example, Yao et al. cite an estimate based on a study sample using a Likert-based assessment that deviates both from the proposed DSM-5 criteria and from alternative theoretically rigorous approaches (4). As a result, the 12-month period prevalence estimates range from a modest 1.2% (95% CI=1.0–1.4), in line with our findings, to an implausibly high 6.3% (95% CI=5.8–6.7). Such discrepancies within single studies underline the need to minimize researcher degrees of freedom by distinguishing between exploratory and confirmatory research reporting (5).

Second, as the authors of the letter suggest, there is a dearth of high-quality data investigating Internet gaming disorder in non-Western contexts, but we cannot at present conclude that Internet gaming disorder has any greater or lesser impact on the health or psychosocial functioning of individuals in Asian milieus. Although we acknowledge that there may indeed be unobserved cultural mediators, we believe such factors need to be investigated through a confirmatory lens (5) and without unduly stigmatizing specific cultures or recreational activities (6).

Finally, a strength of our work is the open-science approach we adopted. Because we shared our study materials on the Open Science Framework, there is no need to speculate about how low indicator incidence might have affected sensitivity analyses or how different thresholds would relate to the health outcomes we observed. Indeed, we invite others to draw their own conclusions from dives into the data. We strongly advise researchers investigating this phenomenon to follow our example and share their raw data and analysis code and register their hypotheses before they collect their data. This approach will produce robust empirical insights and improve our understanding of the links between Internet gaming disorder and a wider range of correlates, including personal and clinical factors.

From the Oxford Internet Institute and the Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford, United Kingdom; the School of Psychology, Cardiff University, Cardiff, Wales, United Kingdom; the School of Psychology and Clinical Language Sciences, University of Reading, Reading, Berkshire, United Kingdom; and Kochi University of Technology, Kochi, Japan.
Address correspondence to Dr. Przybylski ().

All authors contributed equally to this letter.

The authors’ disclosures accompany the original article.

References

1 Festl R, Scharkow M, Quandt T: Problematic computer game use among adolescents, younger and older adults. Addiction 2013; 108:592–599Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

2 Wan CS, Chiou WB: Psychological motives and online games addiction: a test of flow theory and humanistic needs theory for Taiwanese adolescents. Cyberpsychol Behav 2006; 9:317–324Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

3 Cranwell J, Opazo-Breton M, Britton J: Adult and adolescent exposure to tobacco and alcohol content in contemporary YouTube music videos in Great Britain: a population estimate. J Epidemiol Community Health 2016; 70:488–492Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

4 Griffiths MD, van Rooij AJ, Kardefelt-Winther D, et al.: Working towards an international consensus on criteria for assessing internet gaming disorder: a critical commentary on Petry et al. (2014). Addiction 2016; 111:167–175Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

5 Wagenmakers EJ, Wetzels R, Borsboom D, et al.: An agenda for purely confirmatory research. Perspect Psychol Sci 2012; 7:632–638Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

6 Aarseth E, Bean AM, Boonen H, et al.: Scholars’ open debate paper on the World Health Organization ICD-11 Gaming Disorder proposal. J Behav Addict (Epub ahead of print, Dec 30, 2016)Google Scholar