The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×
ArticleFull Access

Conversion From Mild Cognitive Impairment to Probable Alzheimer’s Disease Predicted by Brain Magnetic Resonance Spectroscopy

Abstract

OBJECTIVE: Mild cognitive impairment has been regarded as a pre-Alzheimer condition, but some patients do not develop dementia. Given the available therapies for Alzheimer’s disease, early diagnosis is of paramount importance. The authors’ objective was to determine whether findings from magnetic resonance spectroscopy (MRS) of the hippocampus and other cortical areas would predict conversion from amnestic mild cognitive impairment to probable Alzheimer’s disease. METHOD: A longitudinal inception cohort of 53 consecutive and incident subjects fulfilling the criteria of amnestic mild cognitive impairment was followed for a mean period of 3 years. At baseline, a neuropsychological examination (Mini-Mental State Examination, Blessed Dementia Rating Scale, Clinical Dementia Rating, verbal fluency test, and memory tests) and standard blood tests were performed, and three cortical areas were examined by proton MRS: left hippocampus, right parietal cortex, and left occipital cortex. The patients were evaluated periodically to detect conversion to probable Alzheimer’s disease. The statistical analysis of predictions was based on receiver operating characteristic curves. RESULTS: By the follow-up assessment that occurred on average after 3 years, 29 patients (55%) had developed probable Alzheimer’s disease. An occipital cortex N-acetylaspartate/creatine ratio ≤1.61 predicted dementia at 100% sensitivity and 75% specificity (area under the curve=0.91, 95% CI=0.80–0.97). The positive predictive value was 83%, and the negative predictive value was 100%, with an overall cross-validated classification accuracy of 88.7%. None of the values in the hippocampus and parietal cortex had significant predictive value. CONCLUSIONS: MRS of the brain performed on patients with mild cognitive impairment is a valuable tool in predicting conversion to probable Alzheimer’s disease. Occipital values were more reliable than hippocampal values in this prediction.

Mild cognitive impairment may be defined as a transitional state between normal aging and Alzheimer’s disease in which memory impairment is greater than expected for age, but general cognitive function and daily living activities are preserved (1). The rate of progression to dementia is considerable according to estimates from various longitudinal studies: 12% per year for 4 years (1), 40% over 2 years (2), 53% over 3 years (3), 34%–100% over 4–5 years (4, 5), and 100% over 9.5 years (6).

Currently, there has been open discussion about terminology and on whether mild cognitive impairment must be considered either a separate nosological entity at increased risk of dementia or a prodrome of Alzheimer’s disease (7). Whatever the option, it is of paramount importance to develop tools that enable us to predict conversion to dementia.

The availability of drug therapy for mild-to-moderate Alzheimer’s disease has led to an emphasis on the early detection and management of this disorder. As therapies with the potential to halt progression of Alzheimer’s disease are developed, identification of people with pre-Alzheimer conditions has become increasingly important to determine whether such individuals can benefit from interventional agents.

Previous reports have pointed to the major role of the hippocampal complex in memory (8, 9). Temporal atrophy and especially hippocampal atrophy have been detected in early Alzheimer’s disease (10, 11). Another report concluded that memory dysfunction was highly correlated with atrophy of the amygdala-hippocampal complex and of the temporal lobes (12). Magnetic resonance imaging-based hippocampal volume measurements in patients with mild cognitive impairment have demonstrated that anterior hippocampal formation is most affected in comparison with normal aging, and that the left parahippocampal gyrus shrinks disproportionately to the right (13). Longitudinal assessments of hippocampal atrophy rate support the hypothesis of a continuum from normal aging to mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease (14). Hippocampal atrophy has been regarded as a significant predictor of dementia in subjects with mild cognitive impairment (15).

Proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy (1H-MRS) studies have found decreased N-acetylaspartate and increased myo-inositol in the occipital, temporal, parietal, and frontal regions of patients with Alzheimer’s disease even at the early stages of Alzheimer’s disease (16). N-Acetylaspartate is one of the most abundant amino acids in the central nervous system, located predominantly in neurons, axons, and dendrites. Its function is not well known so far and remains speculative. It has been thought to play a role in lipid and protein synthesis, but it also may be a product of N-acetylaspartyl-glutamate degradation or an osmolyte. Because this metabolite is decreased in some neurological diseases such as degenerative dementia, it has been labeled as a surrogate marker of neuronal integrity (17). The sugar-alcohol compound myo-inositol may act as an osmoregulator, intracellular messenger, and detoxication agent; it is also regarded as marker of glial cells (18).

Relative to healthy subjects, a reduced concentration of N-acetylaspartate has been found in the medial temporal lobe (21% difference) and the cortex of the parietal lobe (13% difference) of patients with Alzheimer’s disease as well as a smaller hippocampus (29% difference) without significant differences between both sides (19). Poor performance on recognition memory tests was correlated with low gray matter N-acetyl compounds and increased choline concentrations in Alzheimer’s disease patients (20). Significantly lower N-acetylaspartate/creatine ratios were found in both hippocampi of 34 Alzheimer’s disease patients in comparison with 22 healthy subjects (21), but in another study comparing Alzheimer’s disease patients and healthy subjects the differences were only significant for the left hippocampus (22). However, to our knowledge there have been no longitudinal studies that used 1H-MRS in patients with mild cognitive impairment as a predictor of conversion to Alzheimer’s disease.

On the basis of these reports, we hypothesized that hippocampal alterations in patients with mild cognitive impairment detected with 1H-MRS would predict conversion to Alzheimer’s disease when the patients are followed longitudinally. It is also likely that these alterations may appear early in the occipital and parietal cortices.

Method

The patients we see in our outpatient clinics are usually referred from the community by family physicians. Given the high proportion of elderly people in our area, mild cognitive impairment and dementia of Alzheimer type are relatively frequent diagnoses in our outpatient clinics.

Before the beginning of the study we asked the general practitioners to refer to us all elderly patients with recent memory complaints. From this group we recruited a cohort of 59 consecutive incident patients who fulfilled the criteria of amnestic mild cognitive impairment proposed by Petersen et al. (1): memory complaints, normal activities of daily living, normal general cognitive function, and abnormal memory for age but no dementia. We also gave special importance to the fact that the memory inefficiencies of the patient had to be corroborated by an informant (wife/husband or relative).

In the clinical history we asked for family history of dementia, vascular risk factors, depression, and other diseases. The final diagnosis was made by taking into account as much information as possible from all sources (general interview, neuropsychological examination, informant report). We only considered patients with a disease duration of up to 12 months as being incident cases of mild cognitive impairment.

Neuropsychological examination at baseline included the Spanish version of the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) (which has a maximum score of 35 points and a cutoff point of 23 for elderly subjects [23]), Blessed Dementia Rating Scale, and category fluency test (Set Test). Memory was explored with subscales of the 144 Battery of Signoret (Spanish validated version) (24):

1.

Immediate verbal recall of a maximum of 12 words with three trials, as well as delayed recall of those words. The maximum possible score is 12 points for immediate recall and 12 for delayed recall.

2.

Immediate visual recall: the patient has to reproduce a complex geometric figure after 1 minute of observation. Delayed visual recall was also evaluated. The maximum possible score is 24 points for each.

3.

Logical memory: immediate and delayed recall of a brief story. Maximum score=24 points for each.

4.

Digit span (number of digits forward).

Memory functions were also assessed in a random comparison group consisting of 20 healthy elderly subjects recruited from the same population so as to demonstrate baseline differences between patients with mild cognitive impairment and subjects without mild cognitive impairment and to establish the cutoff point. We defined abnormal memory as a total memory score <60, which represented a difference greater than one standard deviation from the mean of healthy subjects.

The patients were also evaluated with the Clinical Dementia Rating Scale and the Global Deterioration Scale for staging purposes. Depression was initially assessed by means of the Geriatric Depression Scale (25). Laboratory tests included standard blood tests; measurement of sedimentation rate, cell count, and levels of vitamin B12, folic acid, and TSH; a proteinogram; and serologic tests for syphilis. The final diagnosis of amnestic mild cognitive impairment was made when the patients fulfilled the criteria of Petersen et al. (1) and an informant’s report corroborated the memory complaints. We interpreted with caution some low scores yielded in neuropsychological examination, since some patients had a low level of education. Scores higher than 60 on the memory scale did not exclude the diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment if the clinical history and caregiver report were clearly compatible with memory loss.

Upon completion of the clinical history and neuropsychological examination, the patients with mild cognitive impairment underwent 1H-MRS of the brain in three different areas (Figure 1): left hippocampus, right parietal cortex, and left occipital cortex. Every spectroscopy was performed on a 1.5-T clinical scanner (Signa Horizon, GE Medical Systems, Milwaukee). Sagittal T1-weighted topogram and T2-weighted axial localizing series were used to locate volumes (8 cm3) in cortical areas. Single-voxel 1H-MRS was performed by means of an echo time of 30 msec and a repetition time of 2500 msec with spin-echo technique that uses selective excitation with gradient spoiling for water suppression. The mode of spectral acquisition was Probe-p (PRESS technique).

Data were transferred to a Sun Microsystems workstation and analyzed in an automated manner. Free induction decays were zero filled, exponentially filtered (line broadening=1 Hz), and Fourier transformed. Each spectrum was automatically fitted to four peaks corresponding to levels of N-acetylaspartate (2.02 ppm), total creatine (3.03 ppm), choline-containing compounds (3.23 ppm), and myo-inositol (3.56 ppm). We also calculated the ratios of the peak amplitude of the metabolites relative to creatine and the N-acetylaspartate/myo-inositol ratio in each of the three areas of exploration.

For reproducibility purposes we repeated the exploration after 2 months in 10 patients with the same localizing protocol.

Every patient was followed and reevaluated every 6 months—or earlier if required by caregivers or family physicians—for a mean period of 3 years (range=2.5–3.5 years). Patients with depressive symptoms were adequately treated in the psychiatry unit and reevaluated after 2 months so as to clarify that those patients really met the criteria for mild cognitive impairment. The main end point to be considered was the development of dementia. Neurological evaluations were blinded to the 1H-MRS findings until the end of the follow-up.

Criteria for probable Alzheimer’s disease per the National Institute of Neurological and Communicative Disorders and Stroke and the Alzheimer’s Disease and Related Disorders Association (NINCDS-ADRDA) (26) were applied to the patients who became demented.

The demographic, clinical, and radiological data of patients whose baseline mild cognitive impairment did or did not convert to probable Alzheimer’s disease were analyzed separately and compared statistically. Quantitative variables were compared with t tests for independent samples. The predictive value of the different measurements obtained in baseline 1H-MRS scans was analyzed by means of receiver operating characteristic curves. For this purpose we used the Med-Calc software, which selects the best cutoff for prediction taking into account sensitivity, specificity, likelihood ratio, and predictive values. Complementary statistical analysis was based on a cross-validation technique with the option discriminant analysis/classify/leave-one-out technique of the SPSS, version 10 (SPSS, Inc., Chicago).

Informed consent was obtained from patients and relatives.

Results

At the beginning of the study we had to exclude four patients who did not tolerate the practice of MRI. Therefore, 55 patients were included in the final follow-up assessments. The average age was 72.7 years (SD=5.3, range=61–84). There were 34 women and 21 men. The mean follow-up length was 3 years (range=2.5–3.5), and the clinical assessments were done every 6 months. Each patient was examined six times on average (SD=1).

The baseline clinical variables are summarized in Table 1, including the memory performance of a normative group of 20 healthy subjects (11 women and nine men, mean age=71.4 years). We found significant between-group differences in scores on most memory subscales and total memory score according to two-tailed t tests. All patients with mild cognitive impairment scored 0.5 on the Clinical Dementia Rating and 3 on the Global Deterioration Scale. Six patients scored below 23 on the Spanish version of the MMSE, but this was due to low educational level and not to mental deterioration.

In Table 2 we present the mean metabolite values and ratios from the first and second scans of 10 patients and found no significant changes. In Table 3 are reported the baseline mean values of the different metabolite ratios found in the three areas of exploration. In Figure 2, scatterplots representing the N-acetylaspartate/creatine ratios for the three areas explored are shown.

After 1 year, one patient was lost, and another nondemented patient had died from cardioembolic stroke. The remaining 53 patients completed the follow-up period. Of these 53 patients with mild cognitive impairment at baseline, 13 (25%) converted to a diagnosis of probable Alzheimer’s disease per NINCDS-ADRDA criteria. At this stage we found that an occipital cortex N-acetylaspartate/creatine ratio equal to or lower than 1.61 predicted conversion to dementia at 100% sensitivity and 46% specificity; the area under the curve was 0.69 (95% CI=0.55–0.80). In the hippocampus, a myo-inositol/creatine ratio higher than 1.04 predicted conversion at 66.7% sensitivity and 72% specificity, with an area under the curve of 0.66 (95% CI=0.51–0.79). None of the remaining variables predicted conversion to dementia with statistical significance as the value 0.5 was included in the confidence intervals.

After a mean follow-up period of 3 years, 29 (55%) out of 53 patients converted to a probable Alzheimer’s disease diagnosis, and 24 remained with memory loss but did not exhibit dementia. We did not encounter any other type of dementia (Lewy body disease, frontotemporal dementia, or vascular dementia). After this period two patients died after developing dementia. None of the patients reverted to normality from mild cognitive impairment or reverted to mild cognitive impairment from dementia. Treatment for depression in 15 patients barely improved memory performance.

Only the occipital cortex N-acetylaspartate/creatine ratio at baseline predicted conversion to dementia with confident values (Table 3 and Table 4). The mean ratio for demented patients was 1.46 (SD=0.11) and for patients free of dementia it was 1.63 (SD=0.08), a significant difference. Figure 3 shows spectrum examples with the metabolite peaks for a nonconverter and converter, respectively. N-Acetylaspartate was significantly decreased in relation to creatine in converters. The mean occipital cortex myo-inositol/creatine ratio was 0.69 for nonconverters and 0.72 for converters, which was not significant (p=0.50). In the receiver operating characteristic curve analysis, an occipital cortex N-acetylaspartate/creatine ratio equal to or lower than 1.61 predicted conversion at 100% sensitivity and 75% (95% CI=53%–90%) specificity. The area under the receiver operating characteristic curve (Figure 4) was 0.91 (95% CI=0.80–0.97), with a positive predictive value of 83% and a negative predictive value of 100%. The cross-validation analysis yielded a classification accuracy of 88.7% (canonical correlation=0.8, Wilks’s lambda=0.37, χ2=49.1, p=0.0001). The addition of other variables such as the MMSE score or other metabolite ratios did not improve the model of predictions in the discriminant analysis.

If the cutoff point were ≤1.46 for the N-acetylaspartate/creatine ratio, then specificity would be 95% (95% CI=78%–99%), but sensitivity would decrease to 55% with a positive predictive value of 94% and a negative predictive value of 64%.

The remaining occipital cortex ratios and ratios obtained from the hippocampal and parietal regions did not predict conversion significantly (Table 3 and Table 4). A choline/creatine ratio equal to or lower than 0.63 in the right parietal cortex disclosed a 42.9% sensitivity and a 90.2% specificity with a positive predictive value of 85.7%. The absolute metabolite concentrations were not of value for predicting purposes. All predictions were based on baseline 1H-MRS scans.

We also observed that none of the following baseline variables predicted conversion to dementia with confidence: MMSE score, memory performance, verbal fluency, age, or family history of dementia. After 1 year, a score equal to or lower than 26 on the MMSE (Spanish version with a maximum of 35 points) predicted conversion at 61.5% sensitivity and 83.3% specificity, with an area under the curve of 0.71 (95% CI=0.55–0.84). At the 3-year follow-up assessment, the values of prediction for a cutoff point of 29 were as follows: 76.9% sensitivity, 68.2% specificity, and an area under the curve of 0.76 (95% CI=0.60–0.88). The values were below 70% for verbal fluency scores and memory performance scores and, therefore, far from being clinically useful.

Discussion

Memory impairment is a frequent complaint in the elderly, but hitherto we did not have any parameter objective enough to predict conversion to Alzheimer’s disease. Numerous studies have focused on this issue; some of them have used neuropsychological tests as predictors, others have used neuroimaging techniques. Although most studies are interesting, the matter has not been completely resolved because of either insufficient sensitivity or specificity of the tools used.

A cohort of 195 patients with questionable dementia underwent comprehensive neuropsychological examination to predict conversion to dementia, but these predictions achieved at most 64% sensitivity and 76% specificity for verbal recognition and verbal fluency (27). There have been some reports suggesting that hippocampal volume changes in patients with mild cognitive impairment may help predict conversion to Alzheimer’s disease (15, 28, 29). However, the subject of which region of the brain, entorhinal cortex or hippocampus, demonstrates the earliest abnormalities in early Alzheimer’s disease has not yet been elucidated because of the large variability of imaging measures, the intersubject variability of brain anatomy, and the imaging artifacts in the medial temporal region (30). In addition, some degree of hippocampal atrophy may be seen in up to one-third of cognitively normal old people (31). It is clear that we need more powerful objective parameters, such as magnetic resonance spectroscopy, on which predictions can rely.

Our results are encouraging, but it is surprising to us that hippocampal values did not predict conversion with confidence. It is not consistent with the early involvement of this area in Alzheimer’s disease, but there may be some reasons to explain this finding. First, the partial volume effect in the hippocampus and parietal lobe resulting from the presence of CSF in some patients may affect the absolute value of metabolites but exerts little influence on metabolite ratios, since the CSF virtually does not contain the metabolites being assessed (32). Given the large size of the voxel, it is probable that extrahippocampal tissues had been included in the analysis, which may decrease the sensitivity of the technique in this area. Another possible explanation is that converters and nonconverters have equal or minimal neuropathological alterations in the hippocampus. A pathoanatomical study that included 13 nondemented cases, four cases with preclinical Alzheimer’s disease, eight cases with very mild symptomatic Alzheimer’s disease, and four cases with severe Alzheimer’s disease found a significant decrease in neuron number in the entorhinal cortex and hippocampus of patients with very mild Alzheimer’s disease but not in healthy and preclinical Alzheimer’s disease cases (33). The authors concluded that Alzheimer’s disease carries clinical deficits only when it produces significant neuronal loss and recognized that most subjects ascribed to the group of very mild Alzheimer’s disease cases were rated as probable Alzheimer’s disease according to Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease criteria. Theoretically, the lack of significant decrease in neuron number could explain the lack of predictive power of 1H-MRS hippocampal values in our study. Likewise, in a report of 15 patients with mild cognitive impairment who underwent neuropathological examination, all had more neuritic plaques in the hippocampus than did normal subjects, but the plaque density was less than one-third of that found in temporal and insular cortices (34).

On the one hand the useful values found in the occipital cortex look inconsistent at first glance with the hypothesis that the occipital cortex is only affected in the advanced stages of Alzheimer’s disease (35). In addition, a cross-sectional study that included 21 patients with mild cognitive impairment, 21 patients with Alzheimer’s disease, and 63 elderly comparison subjects showed similar N-acetylaspartate/creatine ratios in the occipital lobe (1.78, 1.8, and 1.82, respectively) (36). However, the authors stated that 14 out of the 21 Alzheimer’s disease patients were being treated with donepezil, which may elevate the N-acetylaspartate/creatine ratios.

On the other hand, there have been three reports (3739) showing low N-acetylaspartate/creatine ratios in the midline occipital cortex of Alzheimer’s disease patients (1.14, 1.16, and 1.1, respectively). Furthermore, in one of these studies it was concluded that the occipital cortex myo-inositol/N-acetylaspartate ratio had the best discriminant power because it distinguished Alzheimer’s disease from normality at 83% sensitivity and 98% specificity. The authors argued that this cortical area is rich in gray matter and, therefore, ideal for analysis in Alzheimer’s disease (38).

Our results are also consistent with the delay of the P2 component of visual evoked potential in Alzheimer’s disease patients but not in comparison subjects or patients with other dementing processes (40), which suggests cholinergic defect in visual association areas. The normal latencies of early visual evoked potential components are compatible with spared primary visual cortex. It was corroborated in a histological study showing low neurofibrillary tangles in primary projection area 17 but increased 20-fold in adjacent association areas 18 and 20, although no differences were seen with regard to number of neuritic plaques (41).

The exploration of other cortical areas such as the cingulate gyrus also could be of aid in improving predictions. In a cross-sectional study that included 24 patients with mild cognitive impairment and 22 Alzheimer’s disease patients, 1H-MRS of the posterior cingulate gyrus discriminated Alzheimer’s disease from mild cognitive impairment at 80% specificity and 67% sensitivity by determining the N-acetylaspartate/creatine ratio (42).

In conclusion, 1H-MRS of the occipital cortex may be a valuable tool in predicting conversion from mild cognitive impairment to probable Alzheimer’s disease and has proven superior to other predictors including neuropsychological performance. Given the correlational and descriptive nature of the present investigation, our findings should be replicated in a larger investigation. If our results are confirmed, this technique could be used for screening or prognostic purposes in patients with mild cognitive impairment. In addition, it could be helpful to determine which patients would benefit from early therapy for Alzheimer’s disease.

TABLE 1
TABLE 2
TABLE 3
TABLE 4

Presented at the 128th meeting of the American Neurological Association, San Francisco, Oct. 19–22, 2003. Received Jan. 13, 2004; revisions received March 16 and May 25, 2004; accepted June 10, 2004. From the Neurology Unit, Hospital de Alcañiz, Spain; the Magnetic Resonance Unit, Clínica Quirón de Zaragoza, Spain; and the Neurology Unit, Hospital de Teruel, Spain. Address correspondence and reprint requests to Dr. Modrego, Avda Juan Carlos I, 21, 50009 Zaragoza, Spain; (e-mail). This work was conducted with the financial support of Laboratorios Esteve, Spain. The design, writing, and the decision of publication are only of the authors, who have no conflicts of interest to declare.

Figure 1.

Figure 1. Regions Explored by Proton Magnetic Resonance Spectroscopy in Subjects With Mild Cognitive Impairmenta

aMemory complaints, normal activities of daily living, normal general cognitive function, and abnormal memory for age but no dementia, with memory inefficiencies corroborated by an informant.

Figure 2.

Figure 2. Baseline N-Acetylaspartate/Creatine Ratios in Hippocampal, Parietal, and Occipital Regions in Subjects With Mild Cognitive Impairmenta That Over Time Did or Did Not Convert to Probable Alzheimer’s Disease

aMemory complaints, normal activities of daily living, normal general cognitive function, and abnormal memory for age but no dementia, with memory inefficiencies corroborated by an informant.

Figure 3.

Figure 3. Occipital Cortex Spectruma

aIn the patient who did not go on to develop probable Alzheimer’s disease, the peak amplitude is strikingly higher for N-acetylaspartate than for creatine. For the patient who over time did develop probable Alzheimer’s disease, there is a decrease of N-acetylaspartate peak amplitude in relation to creatine.

Figure 4.

Figure 4. Baseline Occipital Cortex N-Acetylaspartate/Creatine Ratio as a Predictor of Conversion From Mild Cognitive Impairment to Probable Alzheimer’s Disease

References

1. Petersen RC, Smith GE, Waring SC, Ivnik RJ, Tangalos EG, Kokmen E: Mild cognitive impairment: clinical characterization and outcome. Arch Neurol 1999; 56:303–308; correction: 56:760Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

2. Johnson KA, Jones K, Holman BL, Becker JA, Spiers PA, Satlin A, Albert MS: Preclinical prediction of Alzheimer’s disease using SPECT. Neurology 1998; 50:1563–1571Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

3. McKelvey R, Bergman H, Stern J, Rush C, Zahirney G, Chertkow H: Lack of prognostic significance of SPECT abnormalities in non-demented elderly subjects with memory loss. Can J Neurol Sci 1999; 26:23–28MedlineGoogle Scholar

4. Bennett DA, Wilson RS, Schneider JA, Evans DA, Beckett LA, Aggarwal NT, Barnes LL, Fox JH, Bach J: Natural history of mild cognitive impairment in older persons. Neurology 2002; 59:198–205Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

5. Krasuski JS, Alexander GE, Horwitz B, Daly EM, Murphy DG, Rapoport SI, Schapiro MB: Volumes of medial temporal lobe structures in patients with Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment (and in healthy controls). Biol Psychiatry 1998; 43:60–68Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

6. Morris JC, Storandt M, Miller JP, McKeel DW, Price JL, Rubin EH, Berg L: Mild cognitive impairment represents early-stage Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol 2001; 58:397–405MedlineGoogle Scholar

7. Ritchie K, Touchon J: Mild cognitive impairment: conceptual basis and current nosological status. Lancet 2000; 355:225–228Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

8. Fazio F, Perani D, Gilardi MC, Colombo F, Cappa SF, Vallar G, Bettinardi V, Paulesu E, Alberoni M, Bressi S, Franceschi M, Lenzi GL: Metabolic impairment in human amnesia: a PET study of memory networks. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 1992; 12:353–358Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

9. Golomb J, de León MJ, Kluger A, George AE, Tarshish C, Ferris SH: Hippocampal atrophy in normal aging. an association with recent memory impairment. Arch Neurol 1993; 50:967–973Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

10. Convit A, de León MJ, Golomb J, George AE, Tarshish CY, Bobinski M, Tsui W, De Santi S, Wegiel J, Wisniewski H: Hippocampal atrophy in early Alzheimer’s disease: anatomic specificity and validation. Psychiatr Q 1993; 64:371–387Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

11. Erkinjuntti T, Lee DH, Gao F, Steenhuis R, Eliasziw M, Fry R, Merskey H, Hachinski VC: Temporal lobe atrophy on magnetic resonance imaging in the diagnosis of early Alzheimer’s disease. Arch Neurol 1993; 50:305–310Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

12. Heun R, Mazanek M, Atzor KR, Tintera J, Gawehn J, Burkart M, Gänsicke M, Falkai P, Stoeter P: Amygdala-hippocampal atrophy and memory performance in dementia of Alzheimer type. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 1997; 8:329–336Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

13. Petersen RC, Jack CR Jr, Xu YC, Waring SC, O’Brien PC, Smith GE, Ivnik RJ, Tangalos EG, Boeve BF, Kokmen E: Memory and MRI-based hippocampal volumes in aging and AD. Neurology 2000; 54:581–587Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

14. Jack CR Jr, Petersen RC, Xu YC, O’Brien PC, Smith GE, Ivnik RJ, Boeve BF, Tangalos EG, Kokmen E: Rates of hippocampal atrophy correlate with change in clinical status in aging and AD. Neurology 2000; 55:484–489Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

15. de León MJ, Golomb J, George AE, Convit A, Tarshish CY, McRae T, De Santi S, Smith G, Ferris SH, Noz M, Rusinek H: The radiologic prediction of Alzheimer disease: the atrophic hippocampal formation. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol 1993; 14:897–906MedlineGoogle Scholar

16. Valenzuela MJ, Sachdev P: Magnetic resonance spectroscopy in AD. Neurology 2001; 56:592–598Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

17. Barker PB: N-Acetyl aspartate—a neuronal marker? (editorial). Ann Neurol 2001; 49:423–424Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

18. Ross BD: Biochemical considerations in 1H spectroscopy: glutamate and glutamine; myo-inositol and related metabolites. NMR Biomed 1991; 4:59–63Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

19. Schuff N, Capizzano AA, Du AT, Amend DL, O’Neill J, Norman D, Kramer J, Jagust W, Miller B, Wolkowitz OM, Yaffe K, Weiner MW: Selective reduction of N-acetylaspartate in medial temporal and parietal lobes in AD. Neurology 2002; 58:928–935Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

20. Pfefferbaum A, Adalsteinsson E, Spielman D, Sullivan EV, Lim KO: In vivo brain concentrations of N-acetyl compounds, creatine, and choline in Alzheimer disease. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1999; 56:185–192Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

21. Block W, Jessen F, Traber F, Flacke S, Manka C, Lamerichs R, Keller E, Heun R, Schild H: Regional N-acetylaspartate reduction in the hippocampus detected with fast proton magnetic resonance spectroscopic imaging in patients with Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol 2002; 59:828–834Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

22. Dixon RM, Bradley KM, Budge MM, Styles P, Smith AD: Longitudinal quantitative proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy of the hippocampus in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain 2002; 125:2332–2341Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

23. Lobo A, Ezquerra J, Gómez Burgada F, Sala JM, Seva Díaz A: El Mini-Exxamen Cognoscitivo: un test sencillo y práctico para detectar alteraciones intelectuales en pacientes médicos. Actas Luso Esp Neurol Psiquiatr Cienc Afines 1979; 7:189–202MedlineGoogle Scholar

24. González Torres M: Batería 144 para el estudio de las funciones mnésicas y de aprendizaje, in La exploración neuropsicológica. Edited by Peña Casanova J. Barcelona, Spain, MCR, 1988Google Scholar

25. Spreen O, Strauss E: Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS), in A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests, 2nd ed. Edited by Spreen O, Strauss E. New York, Oxford University Press, 1998, pp 612–616Google Scholar

26. McKhann G, Drachman D, Folstein M, Katzman R, Price D, Stadlan EM: Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: report of the NINCDS-ADRDA Work Group under the auspices of the Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer’s Disease. Neurology 1984; 34:939–944Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

27. Tian J, Bucks RS, Haworth J, Wilcock G: Neuropsychological prediction of conversion to dementia from questionable dementia: statistically significant but not yet clinically useful. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2003; 74:433–438Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

28. Du AT, Schuff N, Amend D, Laakso MP, Hsu YY, Jagust WJ, Yaffe K, Kramer JH, Reed B, Norman D, Chui HC, Weiner MW: Magnetic resonance imaging of the entorhinal cortex and hippocampus in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2001; 71:441–447Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

29. Jack CR Jr, Petersen RC, Xu YC, O’Brien PC, Smith GE, Ivnik RJ, Boeve BF, Waring SC, Tangalos EG, Kokmen E: Prediction of AD with MRI-based hippocampal volume in mild cognitive impairment. Neurology 1999; 52:1397–1403Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

30. Mega MS: The entorhinal cortex in Alzheimer’s disease (editorial). J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2001; 71:431–432Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

31. Golomb J, de León MJ, Kluger A, George AE, Tarshish C, Ferris SH: Hippocampal atrophy in normal aging: an association with recent memory impairment. Arch Neurol 1993; 50:967–973Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

32. Blüml S: Data acquisition in the research mode, in Clinical MR Spectroscopy: Europrobe Training Course. Edited by Ross BD. Pasadena, Calif, Huntington Medical Research Institutes, 1996, pp 60–84 Google Scholar

33. Price JL, Ko AI, Wade MJ, Tsou SK, McKeel DW, Morris JC: Neuron number in the entorhinal cortex and CA1 in preclinical Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol 2001; 58:1395–1402Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

34. Price JL, Morris JC: Tangles and plaques in nondemented aging and “preclinical” Alzheimer’s disease. Ann Neurol 1999; 45:358–368Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

35. Braak H, Braak E: Neuropathological stageing of Alzheimer-related changes. Acta Neuropathol (Berl) 1991; 82:239–259Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

36. Kantarci K, Jack CR Jr, Xu YC, Campeau NG, O’Brien PC, Smith GE, Ivnik RJ, Boeve BF, Kokmen E, Tangalos EG, Petersen RC: Regional metabolic patterns in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease: a 1H MRS study. Neurology 2000; 55:210–217Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

37. Miller BL, Moats RA, Shonk T, Ernst T, Woolley S, Ross BD: Alzheimer disease: depiction of increased cerebral myo-inositol with proton MR spectroscopy. Radiology 1993; 187:433–437Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

38. Shonk TK, Moats RA, Gifford P, Michaelis T, Mandigo JC, Izumi J, Ross BD: Probable Alzheimer disease: diagnosis with proton MR spectroscopy. Radiology 1995; 195:65–72Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

39. Rai GS, McConnell JR, Waldman A, Grant D, Chaudry M: Brain proton spectroscopy in dementia: an aid to clinical diagnosis (letter). Lancet 1999; 353:1063–1064Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

40. Coburn KL, Ashford JW, Moreno MA: Visual evoked potentials in dementia: selective delay of flash P2 in probable Alzheimer’s disease. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 1991; 3:431–435Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

41. Lewis DA, Campbell MJ, Terry RD, Morrison JH: Laminar and regional distributions of neurofibrillary tangles and neuritic plaques in Alzheimer’s disease: a quantitative study of visual and auditory cortices. J Neurosci 1987; 7:1799–1808Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

42. Kantarci K, Xu YC, Shiung MM, O’Brien PC, Cha RH, Smith GE, Ivnik RJ, Boeve BF, Edland SD, Kokmen E, Tangalos EG, Petersen RC, Jack CR Jr: Comparative diagnostic utility of different MR modalities in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2002; 14:198–207Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar